<<

The Journal of Neuroscience, October 1995, 15(10): 68466855

Impaired Conditioning Following Unilateral Temporal Lobectomy in Humans

Kevin S. LaBar,’ Joseph E. LeDoux,’ Dennis D. Spencer,2 and Elizabeth A. Phelps3 Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York 10003, *Section of Neurosurgery, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven. Connecticut 06510, and 3Depattment of , Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut 06520

Classical was used in the present study structuresin emotionallearning and in humans,as mea- as a model for investigating emotional and mem- sured by classicalfear conditioning. ory in human subjects with lesions to the medial temporal Fear conditioning is a generic term usedto describe an aver- lobe. Animal studies have revealed a critical role for medial sive classicalconditioning procedure in which a neutral condi- temporal lobe structures, particularly the , in sim- tioned (CS), such as a tone, is presentedin ple and complex associative emotional responding. Wheth- with a noxious unconditionedstimulus (US), such as an electric er these structures perform similar functions in humans is shock. The US elicits an unconditionedresponse (UR), and over unknown. On both simple and conditional discrimination several trials of CS-US pairings, the CS itself comesto elicit a tasks, unilateral temporal lobectomy subjects showed im- conditioned fear response(CR; Estes and Skinner, 1941; Mc- paired conditioned response acquisition relative to control Allister and McAllister, 1971). Normal human subjectsexhibit subjects. This impairment could not be accounted for by reliable autonomic and behavioral URs to unconditionedstimuli deficits in nonassociative sensory or autonomic perfor- (shocks and loud noises), as well as CRs to conditioned fear mance factors, or by differences in declarative memory for stimuli (e.g., Ghman, 1979; Hodes et al., 1985; Grillon et al., the experimental parameters. These results show that me- 1991). dial temporal lobe structures in humans, as in other mam- Fear conditioning in human clinical populations,however, has mals, are important components in an emotional memory not been well-characterized. Existing reports have primarily in- network. volved anxious or schizophrenicpatients, where the criteria for [Key words: amygdala, , discrimi- study inclusion were based on psychiatric evaluation (e.g., nation learning, emotion, fear, skin conductance response] Howe, 1958; Ax et al., 1970; Gorham et al., 1978; Pitman and Orr, 1986; Ashcroft et al., 1991). Since information regarding the underlying brain etiology was not available in the patient The neural substrateof emotional learning and memory in ani- samples,these studieshave not been informative in advancing malshas been extensively studiedusing classicalfear condition- knowledge of the neural structuresinvolved in conditioned fear. ing paradigmsas a model approach (for reviews, see Kapp et Fear conditioning was once attempted in H.M., a well-studied al., 1990; Davis, 1992; Fanselow, 1994; LeDoux, 1995). The amnesic subject with extensive bilateral medial temporal lobe emerging view from this researchsuggests that the amygdala is damage, but H.M. did not show skin conductance responses an essentialcomponent in a distributed neural network through (SCRs) to electric shock, so evaluation of conditioned respond- which emotional reactions are coupled to novel stimuli. Al- ing in H.M. was suspended(Milner et al., 1968). Human patients though the role of other temporal lobe structures is less clear, with restricted bilateral damageto the amygdala are extremely recent lesion studiesindicate that the hippocampusis not essen- rare (Tranel and Hyman, 1990). Given these factors, brain-be- tial for simpleconditioned fear associationsbut is important for havior relations in fear conditioning phenomenain humanshave more complex aspectsof fear conditioning, such as contextual been difficult to assess. conditioning (Seldenet al., 1991; Kim and Fanselow, 1992; Phil- In the present study, we examined fear conditioning in human lips and LeDoux, 1992, 1994).This model of emotionalmemory subjectswith unilateral anteromedial temporal lobe resectionto formation, however, has not been applied to investigate analo- control medically intractable epilepsy. These patients have sus- gous brain mechanismsin humans,where little is known about tained damage to the amygdala and associated the neural basisof emotional information processing.The goal with complex partial seizures of medial temporal lobe origin, of the presentstudy is to assessthe role of medial temporal lobe but they do not show the severedeclarative memory impairment typical of amnesicswith bilateral hippocampallesions (Novelly Received Dec. 19, 1994: revised June 14, 1995; accepted June 20, 1995. et al., 1984). Becausethe etiology of this population sampleis This work was supported by PHS Grant MH10537 to K.S.L., PHS Grant well-defined, brain-behavior relations can be assessedmore MH38774 to J.E.L., and McDonnell-Pew Grant 90-34 to E.A.P. We thank Dr. Kimberlee Sass for providing neuropsychological results and Ohad Ziv for his carefully than in previous studies.Both simple and conditional assistance in data collection. discrimination tasks were conducted to examine the role of the Correspondence should be addressed to Kevin S. LaBar, Center for Neural medial temporal lobe in simple and complex aspectsof associa- Science, New York University, 6 Washington Place, Room 809, New York, NY 10003. tive emotional responding. A finding of impaired performance Copyright 0 1995 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/95/156846-10$05.00/O in this group would demonstratethat the relationship between The Journal of Neuroscience, October 1995, 15(10) 6847

a elly et al., 1984), and most patients are gradually withdrawn from an- ticonvulsant medication in accordance with postoperative neurological assessment. Twenty-two patients participated in the simple discrimination task (11 left hemisphere, 11 right hemisphere; 9 male, 13 female), and 23 pa- tients participated in the conditional discrimination task (12 left hemi- sphere, 11 right hemisphere; 8 male, 15 female). Most subjects partic- ipated in both experiments in a counterbalanced presentation order sep- arated by a 1 week interval. All but four subjects were taking anticon- vulsant medication at the time of testing. Neuropsychological testing results, including the Wechsler Adult In- telligence Scale-Revised (WARS-R; Wechsler, 1981) and the Russell ad- aptation of the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS; Russell, 1975) are presented in Table 1 along with data on other clinical variables. Neu- ropsychological data was available for a subset of patients tested both 6 months preoperatively and 12 months postoperatively. Consistent with previous studies (e.g., Novelly et al., 1984), patients with left (speech- dominant) medial temporal lobe epilepsy showed more impaired verbal memory in comparison to patients with right (nondominant) medial temporal lobe epilepsy on WMS subtest scores [preoperative: WMS delayed verbal t(17) = -2.80, p = 0.012; WMS immediate verbal t(17) = -2.45, p = 0.026; postoperative: WMS delayed verbal t(12) = -2.82, p = 0.016; WMS immediate verbal t(13) = -2.42, p = 0.031]. Right- and left-medial temporal lobe epilepsy patients were not signif- icantly different on any of the WAIS-R IQ scores or other subject char- acteristics. The control group consisted of nonepileptic adult subjects matched for age and gender without a history of epilepsy or other neurological impairment. Twenty-two subjects participated in the simple discrimi- nation task (10 male, 12 female), and 23 subjects participated in the conditional discrimination task (10 male, 13 female). As in the temporal lobectomy group, most of the control subjects participated in both tasks in a counterbalanced presentation order separated by a 1 week interval. Control subjects were recruited through local newspaper advertisements. An additional epileptic control group (N = 3; 1 male, 2 female) was run on the simple discrimination task to address issues of the specificity of the results. These patients have a similar clinical profile as the tem- poral lobectomy group, including a history of epileptic seizure activity uncontrolled by anticonvulsant medication followed by surgical treat- ment. However, in these patients, the focus of the seizure activity was not localized to the medial temporal lobe: 2 of the subjects received a partial left occipital lobectomy, while the remaining subject received section of the anterior 2/3 of the corpus callosum. Data from this control group will be presented separately due to the small sample size. This group was not run on the conditional discrimination task, which in- cluded a visual component, as the occipital lobectomy subjects in this Figure 1. Representative postoperative Tl-weighted MRI scan dem- group had mild visual deficits in the contralateral visual field. onstrating the extent of the surgical procedure. a, Coronal section at the All subjects provided informed consent and were paid for their par- level of the amygdala. b, Parasagittal section. ticipation. All subjects showed normal hearing thresholds based on an audiometric screening (Beltone Instruments). None of the subjects run on the conditional discrimination task had color vision deficits. The medial temporal lobe structures and learned emotional re- protocol received Human Subjects Committee approval at New York sponses, which has been established in animal models of fear University and Yale University. conditioning, extends to the human brain. Materials Materials and Methods Subjects Auditory stimuli were delivered by Coulbourn Instruments tone and Twenty-six patients with medically refractory complex partial seizures white noise generators (models S8 I-06 and S8 l-02, Lehigh Valley, PA) of medial temporal lobe origin were studied 2-6 years following uni- and were presented binaurally to the subjects through Koss headphones lateral anteromedial en bloc temporal lobe resection. The surgical pro- (model TD/60). A Realistic sound level meter was used to maintain cedure involves an approximate 3.5 cm resection of the anterior middle sound levels to within ?2 dB. Visual stimuli for the conditional dis- and inferior temporal gyri allowing access to the temporal horn. This crimination task were presented centrally on an IBM-compatible VGA is followed by dissection of the occipito-temporal fasciculus and sub- monitor, which was adjusted to full brightness and contrast under am- sequent removal of 70-80% of the amygdala and all of the hippocam- bient room luminance. pus, parahippocampus, and projection fibers to their posterior extent at SCRs were recorded bilaterally from Thought Technology skin con- the atrium of the lateral ventricle (Spencer et al., 1984; Spencer and ductance units (Lafayette Instruments, Lafayette, IN), consisting of Ag- Spencer, 1985). All patients received postoperative anatomical MRI AgCl electrodes attached by velcro straps to the middle phalanges of scans confirming the extent of the surgical procedure. A representative the subjects’ third and fourth digits. Lafayette Instruments electrode gel scan in coronal and parasagittal sections is provided in Figure 1. This was used as an electrolyte. The incoming analog signal was amplified procedure is highly successful in controlling seizure activity in these and digitized by a LabMaster A/D converter controlled by ASYST soft- patients, with most patients experiencing few, if any, seizures postop- ware on an IBM AT computer. Skin conductance was sampled at 100 eratively. These patients typically show some selective cognitive im- Hz throughout a trial and was stored for off-line amplitude analysis. provement following surgery due to reductions in seizure activity (Nov- The minimum SCR resolution was 0.0144 pSiemen (p,S). 6848 LaBar et al. l Fear Conditioning and the Temporal Lobe

Table 1. Demographic and neuropsychological profile of patient population

Seizure WAIS-R WMS Education onset (6 mo. (12 mo. (6 mo. (12 mo. Group Age Gender (yrs) (age) p-v) post-op) pre-op) post-op) LTL 36 2 10 7M, 6F 13 + 2 9 + 10 94 + 10F 99 2 14F 11 f 1IP 11 + 21P (N = 13) 96 2 16P 102 + 13P 8t3DP 8+3DP 94 ? 16V 104 + 14v 15 + 6IV* 14 + 51v* 9? 5DV* 7 f- 6DV* RTL 38 + 9 3M, 10F 14 + 2 7+7 97 + 10F 107 t 12F 10 2 41P 12 + 31P (N = 13) 98 * 11P 110 + 11P 7 5 4DP 10 + 5DP 96 + 13V 104 + 14v 21 + 5IV* 20 2 5 Iv* 16 + 7DV* 16 ? 6DV* WAS-R: E full scale; P, performance scale; V, verbal scale. WMS: IF’, immediate pictoral; DP, delayed pictoral; IV, immediate verbal; DV, delayed verbal. All values represent means (CSD) unless otherwise indicated. *Statistically significant [left temporal lobectomy (LTL) vs right temporal lobectomy (RTL), p < 0.05].

Task instructions In the acquisition phase, subjects received eight CS+ trials (tone pre- ceded by a colored light and terminated with US onset) and eight CS- Before being attached to the skin conductance units, subjects washed trials (tone preceded by the color-opponent light without ). their hands and were given a description of the recording instruments. During , eight CS- trials and eight CS+ trials, now unrein- Subjects were instructed that their SCRs were being recorded in re- forced, were given. A 30 + 6 set intertrial interval was used throughout sponse to tones heard through the headphones. They were asked to the session. Subjects were again asked to verbally report their knowl- attend to the sounds (and lights in the conditional discrimination task) edge of the experimental parameters at the end of each phase. and to try to detect a pattern to the stimuli, which the experimenter would ask them to report periodically throughout the session. Subjects were asked to keep their hands still during the 25 min session. A 1 min Response measurement period of relaxation was given prior to the start of the experiment. SCRs to the conditioned stimuli were used as the dependent measure of conditioning. Any SCR deflection with a peak latency less than 0.5 Experimental paradigm set or greater than 5 set was rejected as artifact. Both first interval and Simple discrimination. Two tones served as CSs in this task (950 Hz second interval SCRs were recorded, as defined by the largest SCR and 3000 Hz; 70 dB; 5 msec rise/fall time; 8 set duration). The US occurring within 0.54 set or 4-S set after tone onset, respectively. was a white noise burst (100 dB; 10 Hz to 20 kHz; 4 cycles/set; 1 set Consistent with previous studies (Ohman et al., 1976; Hugdahl, 1978; duration). The conditioning paradigm consisted of three phases: habit- Lovibond, 1992; Fredrikson et al., 1993), the first interval response was uation, acquisition, and extinction. One of the two CSs was paired with more reliable and was therefore used as the primary measure of con- the US in the acquisition phase (CS+ trial), while the other CS was ditioning. SCRs to the US were scored as a measure of unconditioned not (CS- trial). Choice of auditory stimulus for CS+/CS- was coun- responding in the subjects. All SCRs were square-root transformed to terbalanced across subjects. Trial type (CS+/CS-) was presented to attain statistical normality (Levey, 1980). subjects in a pseudorandom order, subject to the limitation that no more During SCR amplitude measurement, left- and right-hand responses than two of the same trial type be presented successively. Two trial type for each subject were scored independently, and the scorer was blind orderings were composed and were counterbalanced across subjects. to the trial type (CS+/CS-). For the purpose of analysis, trials were In the phase, subjects received four trials each of the CS- averaged into two-trial blocks separately for each trial type. A differ- and CS+ alone. In the acquisition phase, subjects received eight trials ence score was obtained as a measure of differential conditioning by of the CS- presented alone, and eight trials of the CS+ terminated subtracting CS - block responses from CS + block responses (Ax et al., with US onset (nonoverlapping). In extinction, eight trials each of the 1970). According to this measure, difference scores greater than 0 re- CS- and CS+ alone were presented. The intertrial interval used flect greater relative conditioning on CS+ trials; difference scores equal throughout the experiment was 30 t- 6 sec. to 0 reflect no differential conditioning as a function of trial type; and At the end of each experimental condition, subjects were asked to difference scores less than 0 reflect greater relative conditioning on describe what they heard during the preceding phase. If subjects did not CS- trials. An rx level of 0.05 was used in all analyses. verbally report the correct stimulus parameters, their knowledge was probed by a series of questions (e.g., How many sounds did you hear? Results Did you notice any pattern to the stimuli? Did the noise come after one of the tones you heard?). This verbal assessment provided a measure Four general observations were noted acrossboth simple and of the subjects’ declarative knowledge of the experimental parameters conditional discrimination tasks: (1) no significant hand differ- throughout the session. ences (left hand vs right hand) were found for any of the ex- Conditional discrimination. The CS used in this task was a 2000 Hz tone (70 dB; 5 msec rise/fall time; 8 set duration). All other aspects of perimental groups in any experimental phase, so SCRs were auditory stimulus presentation, including US characteristics, were the averaged acrossboth handsin all analyses;(2) no differences same as in the simple discrimination task. Visual stimuli consisted of emergedbetween subjectswho performed the simple discrimi- I-set duration red and green blocks subtending 2.15” of visual angle. nation task first and those who performed the conditional dis- This task also incorporated three conditioning phases: habituation, ac- quisition, and extinction. In the acquisition phase, if the CS was pre- crimination task first, so subject data was collapsedacross task ceded by a colored light (e.g., green), the CS was reinforced immedi- presentationorder; (3) although the small samplesize precluded ately by the US (CS+ trial). If the CS was preceded by the color- direct statistical comparison,there were no apparent differences opponent light (e.g., red), the trial was unreinforced (CS- trial). A 2 in respondingbetween temporal lobectomy patients free of an- set interval separated the onset of the visual cue and onset of the CS. ticonvulsant medication(N = 4) and medicatedpatients, so data Color choice for CS+/CS- trials was counterbalanced across subjects. As in the simple discrimination task, two pseudorandom trial schedules from all temporal lobectomy subjectswas used; (4) finally, no were constructed and were counterbalanced across subjects. significant differences in differential respondingwere found be- During habituation, subjects received four trials of the tone CS alone. tween the left and right temporal lobectomy groups in any ex- The Journal of Neuroscience, October 1995, 75(10) 6649

lized the same set of trial blocks. The ANOVA revealed a sig- nificant main effect of group, F( 1,42) = 17.13, p < 0.001, and a significant group X block interaction, F(4,168) = 3.66, p = 0.007. Follow-up ANOVAs on the group interaction with a sig- nificance level adjusted by the number of follow-up tests (Ta- bachnick and Fidell, 1983) showed a significant block effect only in the control data, F(4,84) = 3.58, p = 0.010, which, in the trend analysisusing orthogonal polynomial decomposition, emerged as a significant quadratic trend, F(1,21) = 8.65, p = 0.008. These data suggestthat control subjectsresponded more to CS+ trials during CR acquisition, with a peak on the last ac- quisition trial and subsequentdecline in discrimination perfor- ---+-- Temporal Lobectomy manceon early extinction blocks. Although it appearsfrom Fig- -0.3 u I I I I 1 I ure 2 that temporal lobectomy subjectsresponded more to CS- Hl H2 Al A2 A3 A4 El E2 E3 E4 trials in the acquisition phase, in the trend analysis using or- HABITUATION ACQUISITION EXTINCTION thogonal polynomial decomposition,this trend was not statisti- cally significant. These results provide evidence for significant Figure 2. Differential conditioning during simple discrimination. Tri- als were averaged into two-trial blocks, and CS- blocks were subtract- differencesin group performanceduring discriminative learning, ed from CS+ blocks to yield a mean SCR difference score (?SEM) with control subjects demonstratingnormal differential condi- for each subject. A difference score of 0 indicates no differential con- tioning ability and temporal lobectomy subjects showing im- ditioning. Temporal lobectomy subjects demonstrated impaired differ- paired CR acquisition. ential conditioning in acquisition relative to nonepileptic control sub- jects. To further explore theseCR acquisition effects, separatetwo- way mixed ANOVAs (group X block) were computed for CS+ trials and CS- trials separately. SCR data averaged by experi- perimental phase, so data from thesesubjects were combinedto mental group as a function of trial type is given in Figure 3. The form one temporal lobectomy group. ANOVA computed on CS+ trials revealed a main effect of group, F(1,42) = 5.37, p = 0.025, indicating significantly higher Simple discrimination SCRs elicited by the CS+ in the control group. A marginally CR acquisition. Figure 2 presentsthe differential conditioning significant group X block interaction was also found, F(4,168) data for simplediscrimination expressedas a function of exper- = 2.10, p = 0.083, showing a trend of responsehabituation in imental group. To investigate CR acquisition, a two-way mixed the control group late in acquisition. Both groups did show an ANOVA was performed on this data using experimental group enhancedresponse on the first block of acquisition (block Al in (temporal lobectomy vs nonepileptic control) as the between- Fig. 3a). However, this trend was not statistically significant- subjectsvariable and experimental block as the within-subjects it was also apparent on the CS- trials (Fig. 3b) and probably variable. The only trial blocks included in the analysis were reflects a nonassociativearousal effect due to the introduction blocks 2-4 of acquisition and blocks l-2 of extinction, as these of the noxious US at the beginning of this phaseof the experi- were the blocks where discriminative learning was hypothesized ment. to be observed. All subsequentanalyses of CR acquisition uti- A two-way mixed ANOVA (group X block) computed on

a. b. 0.5 0.5

0.4 0.,-T T

- 0.3 Y L 8 0.2

0.1 1 - Control --a--- TemporalLobectomy 0. ‘I 0’ , I I I I I I I Hl HL Al A2 A3 A4 El E2 E3 E4 Hl HZ Al A2 A3 A4 El E2 E3 E4 ACQUISITION HABlTUATTON EXTlNCl-ION HABITUATION ACQUISITION EXTINCI’ION Figure 3. Simple discrimination by trial type. SCRs were averaged into two-trial blocks by experimental group. a, Mean SCRs (+SEM) elicited on CS + trials. b, Mean SCRs (+SEM) elicited on CS - trials. Temporal lobectomy subjects showed deficits on CS + trials (a) relative to nonepileptic controls, while no significant differences emerged on CS- trials (b). 6850 LaBar et al. - Fear Conditioning and the Temporal Lobe

Table 2. CS+/CS- Difference scores for the last acquisition block expressed in raw units (microsiemens) and in standard deviation of habituation units (SD,,,)

Proportionof Proportionof Raw score subjectswith subjectswith Group (mean5 SD) scores> 2 SD,,, scores< -2 SD,,, Temporallobectomy -0.11 Z 0.05 o/22 (0%) 2122(9%) Nonepilepticcontrols 0.21 2 0.04 1l/22 (50%) 0122(0%) Epilepticcontrols 0.15 Z 0.10 2/3 (66%) 013 (0%)

CS- trials yielded no significant main effects, although a mar- Differential respondingduring extinction was analyzed by a ginally significant group X block interaction was found, two-way mixed ANOVA (group X block) performed on the last F(4,168) = 2.40, p = 0.052. This interaction suggestsa mar- two extinction trial blocks. No significant main effects or inter- ginal trend for temporal lobectomy subjectsto increaserespond- action terms emergedfrom this analysis,suggesting equivalent ing to the CS- while controls decreaseresponding to the CS- performance in the experimental groups during extinction (see at the end of acquisition. Analysis of the discrimination index Fig. 2). In addition, a two-way (group X block) ANOVA per- discussedabove (Figure 2), however, revealed that the trend in formed on the UR data yielded no significant group differences temporal lobectomy subjectswas not statistically reliable, and in levels of unconditionedresponding (see Fig. 4). However, the Table 2 (see“Individual Variability” below) showsthat this pat- UR analysis did reveal a significant block effect, F(3,108) = tern is carried primarily by only two subjects.These data show 19.03, p < 0.0001, showing a trend for URs to decreaseover that the deficit in differential conditioning during acquisition in time acrossgroups, and a significant group X block interaction, the temporal lobectomy group (Fig. 2) is mainly attributable to F(3,108) = 4.83, p = 0.003. Follow-up dependentt tests indi- relatively low responseson CS+ trials, with no significant group cated that the group interaction was due to marginally significant differencesevident on CS- trials. differences betweenthe groupson block 3. These data show that Habituation, extinction, and UR analysis. Differential re- SCRsduring habituation and extinction were at comparablelev- spondingduring habituation was analyzed by a two-way mixed els in the experimental groups, and that the responsedeficit seen ANOVA with experimentalgroup as the between-subjectsfactor in the temporal lobectomy group during acquisition cannot be and trial block as the within-subjects factor. Only a marginally attributed to a reduction in US sensitivity in thesesubjects. significant group effect, F(1,42) = 3.19, p = 0.081, and a mar- Role of awareness.All but four temporal lobectomy subjects ginally significant block effect, F( 1,42) = 3.77, p = 0.059, were were able to successfullyreport the correct experimental param- found. Theseresults signify a trend for temporal lobectomy sub- eters at the end of each experimental phase. Because so few jects to respondmore to the CS- on the first habituation block subjectswere unaware of the CS-US relationshipsin this task, and for control subjectsto respond more to the CS+ on the a direct statistical comparisonof conditioning in aware and un- secondhabituation block (seeFig. 2). The group X block inter- aware subjectswas not possible.However, observation of the action, however, was not significant. Despite some marginal data from these patients revealed no qualitative differences in trends, there is no significant statistical evidence for response comparison to the results obtained from aware patients. This differences in the experimental groups during the habituation verbal assessmentprovides evidence that declarative knowledge phase. of the rules operating during the conditioning procedure was sparedin most of the temporal lobectomy subjectsstudied. The 1.5 impairmentsseen during CR acquisition in the temporal lobec- tomy group therefore cannot be explained by impaired declara- tive memory for the reinforcement contingencies. Spec$city of the results. An epileptic control group (N = 3) was also run on the simplediscrimination task to addressissues 1 regarding the specificity of the findings. These patients received 5 surgical treatment of medically refractory seizure activity, but 4 the epileptic focus was not localized to the temporal lobe (see if2 Materials and Methods). Consequently, medial temporal lobe 0.5 structuresin thesesubjects are intact. The differential condition- ing data from this group is given in Figure 5, along with the data from the other subjects(taken from Fig. 2) collapsedacross --O- Control trials for comparison purposes.The data show that, as in the --+-. TemporalLobectomy nonepileptic control group, the epileptic controls demonstrated 0 1 2 3 4 simple discrimination ability, with relatively higher SCRs elic- ited on CS+ trials during acquisition. Formal statisticswere not BLOCK computed becauseof the small sample size. Although prelimi- Figure 4. Mean unconditionedresponse performance (?SEM) during nary, the results from the epileptic control group suggestthat simplediscrimination. SCRs to the US were averagedinto two-trial the impairment seenin the temporal lobectomy subjectsis not blocksby experimentalgroup. Temporallobectomy and nonepileptic controlsubjects showed comparable levels of unconditionedresponding due to history of anticonvulsant medication,epilepsy in general, on this task. or other factors related to brain surgery. This finding supports The Journal of Neuroscience, October 1995, 15(10) 6851

--,.a,.. Epileptic Control -0.2- -a- Con501 -c+ Control --+-. Temporal Lobectomy --+ -. Temporal Lobectomy -0.3 - 1 I I -0.2- , HABITUATION ACQUISITION EXTINCI-ION Hl Al A2 A3 A4 El E2 E3 E4

Figure 5. Comparison of differential conditioning during simple dis- HABlTUATION ACQUISITION EXTINCTION crimination with epileptic controls. Data from the temporal lobectomy and nonepileptic control groups are collapsed across trials from Figure Figure 6. Differential conditioning during conditional discrimination. 2 for comparison with data from epileptic controls with lesions to other Trials were averaged into two- 1 blocks, and CS- blocks were sub- brain areas (see Materials and Methods). CS- blocks were subtracted tracted from CS + blocks to yield a mean SCR difference score (? SEM) from CS+ blocks to yield a mean SCR difference score (+SEM) for for each subject. Habituation data is extrapolated to 0, as there was no each subject. A difference score of 0 indicates no differential condi- distinction between CS+ and CS- trials during this phase. A difference tioning. Epileptic control subjects showed intact differential condition- score of 0 indicates no differential conditioning. Temporal lobectomy ing, suggesting that the impairment in the temporal lobectomy group subjects showed impaired differential conditioning in acquisition rela- may be specific to this epileptic population. tive to nonepileptic control subjects.

learning impairment in the temporal lobectomy group relative to the interpretation that damage specific to medial temporal lobe nonepileptic controls. structures is related to the deficits observed in this group. Be- To assess the relative contributions of CS+ and CS- trials to cause of mild visual impairment in some of the epileptic control this response deficit, two-way mixed group X block ANOVAs subjects (see Materials and Methods), this group was not run on were performed on CS+ and CS- trial data separately (see Fig. the conditional discrimination task, which contained a visual 7). The ANOVA computed on CS+ trials revealed a significant component. main effect of group, F( 1,44) = 17.73, p < 0.001, with higher Individual variability. The differential conditioning data from CRs elicited in the control group. There was also a significant the last acquisition block is given in Table 2 to provide a mea- block effect, F(4,176) = 4.46, p = 0.002, showing response sure of individual variability within each group. For each group, habituation in both groups across acquisition trials. Although baseline mean and SDS were derived from habituation trials, and both groups did show an enhanced response to the CS+ on the the raw CS+/CS- difference scores from the last block of ac- first acquisition block (see block Al in Fig. 7), as discussed in quisition were converted into standard deviation units of habit- the simple discrimination task (see “CR Acquisition”), this re- uation (SD,,,). We used a 95% confidence interval criterion to sponse may be artificially elevated by nonassociative factors due describe the proportion of subjects in each group who showed to the introduction of the noxious US. This effect would greater relative conditioning on CS+ trials in this acquisition mask the appearance of a more gradual learning curve in con- block (>2 SD,,,), and the proportion of subjects in each group trols (Fig. 6) and should not be interpreted as evidence of learn- who showed greater relative conditioning on CS- trials in this ing in the temporal lobectomy subjects-it is the only trial block acquisition block (<2 SD,,,). These data show that, according in which an enhanced response occurs, is only an average of to this criterion, none of the temporal lobectomy subjects two trials, and is not a statistically significant increase in this showed greater relative conditioning on CS + trials in this block, group. and only two temporal lobectomy subjects showed greater rel- A group X block ANOVA on CS - trials yielded a significant ative conditioning on CS- trials in this block. In addition, the main effect of block, F(4,176) = 3.28, p = 0.013, indicating data show that there was some intersubject variability in control response habituation in both groups late in acquisition. Although subjects in terms of conditionability, a finding which is consis- there is a trend for group differences in acquisition (see Fig. 7), tent with other conditioning studies (e.g., Woodruff-Pak and this trend was only marginally significant, F(1,44) = 3.37, p = Thompson, 1988). 0.073. In addition, no significant group X block interaction was evident. Thus, comparable response patterns between the exper- Conditional discrimination imental groups were seen during acquisition on CS- trials. The CR acquisition. The differential conditioning data for the con- impairment in differential conditioning for the temporal lobec- ditional discrimination task is given in Figure 6. A two-way tomy subjects (see Fig. 6) can therefore be primarily attributed mixed ANOVA (group X block) was computed to examine CR to low CRs during CS+ acquisition. These results parallel the acquisition in the experimental groups. As in the simple discrim- findings obtained in the simple discrimination task. ination task, trial blocks 24 of acquisition and blocks l-2 of Habituation, extinction, and UR analysis. An independent t extinction were included in this and all subsequent CR acqui- test computed on responses elicited by the CS during habituation sition analysis. The ANOVA yielded a significant main effect revealed no significant effects, indicating equivalent response of group, F(1,44) = 7.73, p = 0.008, indicating discriminant patterns in the experimental groups during the habituation phase 6852 taEar et al. - fear Conditioning and the Temporal Lobe

a. b.

0.4 1

ii:: lz$j+# ‘. .’ *. .*y’* *’ ..‘. ‘. *...‘-- 0.1 - 1 : *.“l is--...1 ‘.T .* e Control k 1 -e-- TemporalLobectomy 0’ , , I o- ‘ Hl Al A2 A3 A4 El E2 E3 E4 HI Al A2 A3 A4 El E2 E3 E4

HABlTUATlON ACQUISITION EXl-lNCllON HABlTUATION ACQUISII’ION EXTINCllON Figure 7. Conditional discrimination by trial type. SCRs were averaged into two-trial blocks by experimental group. a, Mean SCRs (5SEM) elicited on CS+ trials. b, Mean SCRs (?SEM) elicited on CS- trials. Habituation data is averaged across all trials, as there was no distinction between CS+ and CS- trials during this phase. Temporal lobectomy subjects showed deficits on CS+ trials (a) relative to nonepileptic controls, while no significant differences emerged on CS- trials (b).

(see Fig. 7). A group X block ANOVA computed on differential procedurewas sparedin most of the temporal lobectomy patients extinction performance utilizing the last two extinction blocks studied. This finding again reflects a dissociationbetween intact also yielded no significant main effects or interactions (seeFig. declarative memory for the experimental parametersand im- 6). Finally, an analysisof unconditionedresponding showed no paired conditioned responding in these subjects, even in this significant group differences in SCRs to the US (see Fig. 8), more complex conditioning paradigm. althougha significant block effect was found, F(3,117) = 37.05, p < 0.0001, showing an overall decline in UR amplitudes in Discussion both groupsover time. Habituation, extinction, and UR perfor- In the present set of experiments, fear conditioning was inves- mance was therefore comparablein the experimental groups, as tigated as a model for studying the neural basis of emotional previously seenin the simplediscrimination task. learning and memory in humans.Results from the simple and Role of awareness.With the exception of two temporal lo- conditional discrimination procedures demonstrated impaired bectomy patients and one control subject, all subjectscorrectly conditioning in patients with unilateral anteromedial temporal reported the CS-US relationshipsin this paradigm at the end of lobe resection to control medically intractable epilepsy.The im- each experimentalphase. As before, this verbal assessmentpro- pairment was found to be primarily attributable to deficits in vides evidence that declarative knowledge of the conditioning respondingon paired CS-US acquisition trials. Since uncondi- tioned responsesand responding during habituation, CS- ac- 1: quisition, and extinction appearedto be preserved, the discrim- inative learning impairment could not be accountedfor by def- icits in autonomic performance factors. The individual subject analysis, which accounted for group performance levels, also revealed an acquisition impairment in the temporal lobectomy I subjectsirrespective of baselineSCRs in habituation. Moreover, the impairment was not attributable to nonassociative sensory lfj factors (as measuredby audiometric screening),or to differences in declarative memory for the experimental parameters.These i 0.5 findings suggestthat the role of medial temporal lobe structures governing simple and complex conditioned fear associations,as postulatedin animal models of fear conditioning, extends to the + Control human brain. --em--Temnoral Lohectomv 0 It is important to emphasizethat the impairment reportedhere 1 2 3 4 is related to conditioned fear acquisition, that is, the ability of a neutral stimulus to come to elicit an autonomicresponse through BLOCK repeated pairings with an aversive unconditioned stimulus. In Figure 8. Mean unconditioned response performance (tSEM) during the present study, subjects did not necessarily report feeling conditional discrimination. SCRs to the US were averaged into two-trial blocks by experimental group. Temporal lobectomy and nonepileptic “afraid” during the task, but they did report that the noxious control subjects showed comparable levels of unconditioned responding US was annoying or unpleasant-this anticipation of an aversive on this task. event, signaledby the CS and measuredphysiologically, is de- The Journal of Neuroscience, October 1995, 15(10) 6863 fined as conditioned fear. It is also important to note that the learning ability on this more complex procedure. This parallel patients did not show a complete lack of conditioned responding, result may be related to similar contributions of the hippocampus but rather, their SCRs on CS-US acquisition trials were signifi- on more complex versions of both fear conditioning and eyelid cantly attenuated in comparison to controls. In humans, SCRs conditioning tasks (e.g., Rickert et al., 1978; Berger and Orr, have been shown to provide a reliable index of conditioning 1986; Solomon et al., 1986; Kaye and Pearce, 1987). However, (e.g., Fredrikson et al., 1993) and fear conditioning paradigms the response pattern underlying the conditioning impairment was have been proposed as a model for studying human affective different in the two experiments: while Daum and colleagues disorders (Ghman, 1979; Charney et al., 1993). Although auto- reported a deficit due to excess responding on CS- trials, the nomic nervous system activity was used as the dependent mea- current study found a deficit due to inadequate responding on sure of conditioning in the present study, we believe that other CS+ trials. High responses on unreinforced trials is a charac- measures of conditioned fear acquisition (e.g., fear-potentiated teristic of hippocampal dysfunction in animal models of discrim- startle) would also be impaired in these patients. This hypothe- inative somatomotor learning (Gray and McNaughton, 1983; sis, however, remains to be tested. In contrast, the patients’ un- Ross et al., 1984), while insufficient CR development is a char- conditioned response to an eliciting stimulus (a loud white noise) acteristic of amygdala damage in animal models of fear condi- appeared to be intact, and we have not examined other aspects tioning (e.g., LeDoux, 1995). This contrasting pattern of results of fear-related behavior in these patients, such as recognition of suggests the possibility that different medial temporal lobe struc- facial expressions of fear (Adolphs et al., 1994). tures may have been critical for the emergent response patterns We did not observe any hemispheric differences during con- across the human fear and eyelid conditioning studies. ditioning on either the simple or conditional discrimination Because unilateral temporal lobectomy patients have resection tasks. Left and right temporal lobectomy patients responded sim- of most structures in the medial temporal lobe area, including ilarly, and no SCR asymmetries in the left versus right hands the amygdala, hippocampus, and surrounding cortices, direct as- were seen in these subjects or in controls. Studies in human sessment of the relative contributions of these structures to task subjects which have been specifically designed to assess hemi- performance was not possible. Based on animal studies, it is spheric asymmetries have found some hemispheric differences likely that the amygdala damage in these patients was primarily in electrodermal orienting and conditioning, although other re- responsible for the learning impairment for the following rea- sults have been inconclusive (for review, see Hugdahl, 1984, sons: (1) the patients exhibited similar response patterns across 1995). Asymmetric differences may have been masked in our both simple and complex tasks, suggesting a common neuro- tasks by bilateral stimulus presentation and the response measure biological substrate; and (2) animal studies have shown that se- chosen (SCR). However, it is possible that hemispheric special- lective amygdala lesions disrupt both simple and complex as- ization in conditioning is mediated by other brain regions, or pects of fear conditioning (for review, see Kapp et al., 1990; may be more apparent in behavioral and neuroimaging studies Davis, 1992; LeDoux, 1995), while selective hippocampal le- in normal subjects. sions impair higher-order but not simple CS-US associations Daum et al. (1992) reported preserved simple discrimination (e.g., Rickert et al., 1978; Kaye and Pearce, 1987; Phillips and ability on an eyelid conditioning task in unilateral temporal lo- LeDoux, 1992). One issue raised in extrapolating from the ani- bectomy patients similar to those studied in the present set of mal literature to the present report is that bilateral lesions are experiments. Their finding directly contrasts the outcome of the typically made in animal models of fear conditioning. The sub- simple discrimination experiment reported here. This dissocia- jects used in the present study had predominantly unilateral dam- tion is most likely due to differences in the integrity of the neural age, although it is possible that dysfunction in some structures circuitry essential for mediating these conditioning paradigms. contralateral to the site of the epileptic focus may have contrib- In animal models of eyelid conditioning, learning ability is crit- uted to the response impairment (see also Daum et al., 1991). ically dependent on cerebellar and brainstem structures (for re- Given the present results in humans, we recently examined view, see Lavond et al., 1993) while in animal models of fear the effects of unilateral amygdala lesions in rats, using a simple conditioning, the amygdala is crucial for performance in this delay paradigm with a loud white noise as the US (LaBar and paradigm (for review, see LeDoux, 1995). The temporal lobec- LeDoux, 1995). Unilateral lesions attenuated but did not elimi- tomy subjects tested in the human studies have sustained lesions nate conditioned fear responses, a finding consistent with the to the amygdala, with cerebellar and brain stem circuitry left results reported here. Bilateral lesions, however, completely relatively intact (with the possible exception of some cerebellar eliminated conditioned responding. We expect that bilateral le- deterioration due to long-term Dilantin therapy; see Woodruff- sions of the amygdala would have a similar effect in humans. Pak, 1993). Thus, a dissociation between impaired fear condi- Although human patients with selective bilateral amygdala le- tioning and intact eyelid conditioning on simple learning para- sions are extremely rare (Tranel and Hyman, 1990), future re- digms in this subject population is consistent with the known search investigating human subjects with more restricted damage role of these structures from animal studies. However, alternative and functional imaging studies in normal adults will be useful explanations related to qualitative task differences between the to further characterize the relative contributions of medial tem- human. conditioning studies cannot be ruled out at the present poral lobe structures to performance on conditioning tasks. time. These include procedural differences (experimental param- In both the fear conditioning studies reported here and in eye- eters, such as reinforcement schedules, CS-US intervals, etc.), lid conditioning tasks (Weiskrantz and Warrington, 1979; Daum differences in US modality (white noise vs airpuff), and differ- et al., 1989; Daum et al., 1991, 1992; Woodruff-Pak, 1993), ences in response measurement (SCR vs eyeblink; nonadaptive declarative knowledge of the conditioning parameters was dis- vs adaptive responses). sociated from (or unrelated to) learning performance, even in Furthermore, Daum et al. (1991) also investigated conditional more complex paradigms. Awareness of stimulus relationships discrimination in temporal lobectomy subjects, and, consistent has generally been found to enhance conditionability in normal with the main result from our study, they reported deficits in human subjects (for discussion, see Grant, 1973; Furedy et al., 6854 LaBar et al. * Fear Conditioning and the Temporal Lobe

1982; Frcka et al., 1983; Dawson and Schell, 1987). Current Internal consistency and temporal stability of classically conditioned theories propose that relatively simple conditioning procedures, skin conductance responses. Biol Psycho1 35: 153-163. Frcka G, Beyts J, Levey AB, Martin I (1983) The role of awareness such as a simple delay paradigm, are governed by nondeclarative in human conditioning. Pavlov J Biol Sci 18:69-76. learning processes, while more complex conditioning procedu- Furedy JJ, Arabian JM, Thiels E, George L (1982) Direct and contin- res, such as a conditional discrimination paradigm, are regulated uous measurement of relational learning in human Pavlovian condi- by declarative learning processes (Squire and Cohen, 1984; tioning. Pavlov J Biol Sci 17:69-79. Squire, 1987). The main conclusion to be drawn from condi- Gorham IC, Novelly RA, Ax A, Frohman CE (1978) Classically con- ditioned autonomic discrimination and tryptophan uptake in chronic tioning studies in brain-damaged populations conducted to date schizophrenia. Psychophysiology 15:158-164. is that intact declarative memory may not be sufficient for suc- Grant DA (1973) Cognitive factors in eyelid conditioning. Psycho- cessful performance, even in more complex conditioning para- physiology 10:75-8 1. digms. In order for conditioning to occur, conditioned stimuli Gray JA, McNaughton N (1983) Comparison between the behavioural representations must enter a functional neural network which effects of septal and hippocampal lesions: a review. Neurosci Bio- behav Rev 8: 119-188. mediates the CS-US association and directs an efferent signal to Grillon C, Ameli R, Woods SR, Merikangas K, Davis M (1991) Fear- other brain regions controlling the conditioned response. potentiated startle in humans: effects of anticipatory anxiety on the To conclude, we have demonstrated impaired response acqui- acoustic startle . Psychophysiology 28588-595. sition during simple and complex fear conditioning tasks follow- Hodes RL, Cook EW, Lang PJ (1985) Individual differences in auto- ing unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. As a model for nomic response: conditioned association or conditioned fear? Psy- chophysiology 22:545-560. how fear and other aversive emotional events are learned and Howe ES (1958) GSR conditioning in anxiety states, normals, and remembered, fear conditioning paradigms have been instrumen- chronic functional psychotic subjects. J Abnorm Sot Psycho1 56: 183- tal to uncovering the neural mechanisms of emotional processing 189. in animals. In humans, however, the brain structures involved in Hugdahl K (1978) Electrodermal conditioning to potentially phobic emotional information processing are still poorly understood, al- stimuli: effects of instructional extinction. Behav Res Ther 16:315- 321. though recently there has been an interest in applying animal Hugdahl K (1984) Hemispheric asymmetry and bilateral electrodermal models of emotional memory to human populations (Charney et recordings: a review of the evidence. Psychophysiology 21:371-393. al., 1993; Adolphs et al., 1994; Cahill et al., 1994). These results Hugdahl K (1995) Classical conditioning and implicit learning: the extend what is currently known about the neural substrate of right hemisphere hypothesis. In: Brain asymmetry (Davidson RJ, emotional learning and memory from animal models, suggesting Hugdahl K, eds), pp 235-268. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kapp BS, Wilson A, Pascoe JE Supple W, Whalen PJ (1990) A neu- that medial temporal lobe structures are integral components of roanatomical systems analysis of bradycardia in the rabbit. In: Neu- an emotional memory network in the human brain. recomputation and learning: foundations of adaptive networks (Ga- briel M, Moore J, eds), pp 53-90. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. References Kaye H, Pearce JM (1987) Hippocampal lesions attenuate latent inhi- Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR (1994) Impaired rec- bition of a CS and of a neutral stimulus. Psychobiology 15:293-299. ognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage Kim JJ, Fanselow MS (1992) Modality-specific retrograde of to the human amygdala. Nature 372:669-672. fear. Science 256:675-677. Ashcroft K, Guimaraes FS, Wang M, Deakin JFW (1991) Evaluation LaBar KS, LeDoux JE (1995) Unilateral amygdala lesions attenuate of a psychophysiological model of classical fear conditioning in anx- fear conditioning in rats. Sot Neurosci Abstr, in press. ious patients. Psychopharmacology 104:215-219. Lavond DG, Kim JJ, Thompson RF (1993) Mammalian brain sub- Ax AE Banford JL, Beckett PGS, Fretz NE Gottlieb JS (1970) Auto- strates of aversive classical conditioning. Annu Rev Psycho1 44:317- nomic conditioning in chronic schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psycho1 76: 342. 140-154. LeDoux JE (1995) Emotion: clues from the brain. Annu Rev Psycho1 Berger Tw, Orr WB (1983) Hippocampectomy selectively disrupts dis- 46:209-235. crimination reversal conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane Levey AB (1980) Measurement units in psychophysiology. In: Tech- response. Behav Brain Res 8:49-68. niques in psychophysiology (Martin I, Venables PH, eds), pp 597- Cahill L, Prins B, Weber M, McGaugh JL (1994) B-Adrenergic acti- 628. New York: Wiley. vation and memory for emotional events. Nature 371:702-704. Lovibond PF (1992) Tonic and phasic electrodermal measures of hu- Charney DS, Deutch AY, Krystal JH, Southwick SM, Davis M (1993) man aversive conditioning with long duration stimuli. Psychophysi- Psychobiologic mechanisms of posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch ology 29:621-632. Gen Psychiatry 50:294-305. McAllister WR, McAllister DE (1971) Behavioral measurement of Daum I, Channon S, Canavan AGM (1989) Classical conditioning in conditioned fear. In: Aversive conditioning and learning (Brush FR, patients with severe memory problems. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychi- ed). DD 105-179. New York: Academic. atry 52:47-5 1. , IA Daum I, Channon S, Polkey CE, Gray JA (1991) Classical conditioning Milner B, Corkin S, Teuber H-L (1968) Further analysis of the hip- after temporal lobe lesions in man: impairment in conditional dis- pocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow up study of H. M. Neu- crimination. Behav Neurosci 107:748-756. ropsychologia 6:215-234. Daum I, Channon S, Gray JA (1992) Classical conditioning after tem- Novelly RA, Augustine EA, Mattson RH, Glaser GH, Williamson PD, poral lobe lesions in man: sparing of simple discrimination and ex- Spencer DD, Spencer SS (1984) Selective and tinction. Behav Brain Res 52:159-165. impairment in temporal lobectomy for epilepsy. Ann Neurol 15:64- Davis M (1992) The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear. In: The 67. amygdala: neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental Ghman A (1979) Fear-relevance, autonomic conditioning, and : dysfunction (Aggleton JP, ed), pp 255-305. New York: Wiley-Liss. a laboratory model. In: Trends in behavior therapy (Sj&den Pb, Bates Dawson ME, Schell AM (1987) Human autonomic and skeletal clas- S, Dockens WS. eds). DD 107-133. New York: Academic. sical conditioning: the role of conscious cognitive factors. In: Cog- Ghman A, Fredrikson M:Hugdahl K, Rimmo PA (1976) The premise nitive processes and Pavlovian conditioning in humans (Davey G, of equipotentiality in human classical conditioning: conditioned elec- ed), pp 83-l 14. Chichester: Wiley. trodermal responses to potentially phobic stimuli. J Exp Psycho1 Gen Estes WK, Skinner BF (1941) Some quantitative properties of anxiety. 105:313-337. J Abnorm Sot Psycho1 52: 143-150. Phillips RG, LeDoux JE (1992) Differential contribution of the amyg- Fanselow MS (1994) Neural organization of the defensive behavior dala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Be- system responsible for fear. Psychonomic Bull Rev 1:429-438. hav Neurosci 106:274-285. Fredrikson M, Annas E Georgiades A, Hursti T, Tersman Z (1993) Phillips RG, LeDoux JE (1994) Lesions of the dorsal hippocampal The Journal of Neuroscience, October 1995, 75(10) 6655

formation interfere with background but not foreground contextual Spencer DD, Spencer SS, Mattson RH, Williamson PD, Novelly RA fear conditioning. Learning Memory 1:3444. (1984) Access to the posterior medial temporal lobe structures in the Pitman RK, Orr SP (1986) Test of the conditioning model of neurosis: surgical treatment of temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurosurgery 15:667- differential aversive conditioning of angry and neutral facial expres- 671. sions in anxiety disorder patients. J Abnorm Psycho1 95:208-213. Squire LR (1987) Mechanisms of memory. Science 232:1612-1619. Rickert EJ, Bennett TL, Lane PL, French J (1978) Hippocampectomy Tabachnick BG, Fidel1 LS (1988) Using multivariate statistics. New and the attenuation of blocking. Behav Biol 22:147-160. York: Harper and Row. Ross RT, Orr WB, Holland PC, Berger TW (1984) Hippocampectomy Squire LR, Cohen NJ (1984) Human memory and amnesia. In: Neu- disrupts acquisition and retention of learned conditioned responding. robiology of learning and memory (Lynch G, McGaugh JL, Wein- Behav Neurosci 98:21 l-225. berger NM, eds), pp 3-64. New York: Guilford. Russell EW (1975) A multiple scoring method for the assessment of TraneiD, Hyman BT (1990) Neuropsychological correlates of bilateral amvedala damage. Arch Neurol 47:349-355. complex memory function. J Consult Clin Psycho1 43:800-809. Wech& D (1981) WAIS-R manual: Wechsler adult intelligence Selden NRW, Everitt BJ, Jarrard LE, Robbins TW (1991) Comple- scale-revised. San Antonio: Williams and Wilkins. mentary roles of the amygdala and hippocampus in aversive condi- Weiskrantz L, Warrington EK (1979) Conditioning in amnesic patients. tioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience 42:335-350. Neuropsychologia 17: 187-194. Solomon PR, Vander Schaaf E, Thompson RF, Weisz DJ (1986) Hip- Woodruff-Pak DS (1993) Eveblink classical conditioning in H. M.: pocampus and trace conditioning of the rabbit’s classically condi- delay and trace paradigms.-Behav Neurosci 107:91 l-925. tioned nictitating membrane response. Behav Neurosci 100:729-744. Woodruff-Pak DS. Thomoson RF (1988) Classical conditioninp. of the Spencer DD, Spencer SS (1985) Surgery for epilepsy. Neurol Clin eyeblink response in the delay paradigm in adults aged 18-81 years. 3:313-330. Psycho1 Aging 3:219-229.