Book Reference

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Book Reference Book The social psychology of minority influence MUGNY, Gabriel, PÉREZ, Juan Antonio Reference MUGNY, Gabriel, PÉREZ, Juan Antonio. The social psychology of minority influence. Cambridge : Cambridge University Press, 1991 Available at: http://archive-ouverte.unige.ch/unige:4021 Disclaimer: layout of this document may differ from the published version. 1 / 1 Mugny, G., & Pérez, J. A. (1991). The social psychology of minority influence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, Paris: L.E.P.S. Translated from French by Vivian Waltz Lamongie TABLE OF CONTENTS 1. EXPLAINING CHANGE 1.1 The illusion of immobility and the reality of change 1.2 Majority effects, minority effects 1.3 Searching for the social impact of minorities 1.3.1 First-generation studies: the social context of innovation 1.3.2 Second-generation studies: influence as social identification 1.3.3 Third-generation studies: some gaps in discrimination 1.3.4 Fourth-generation studies: towards inter- relating discrimination and validation 2. MINORITY INFLUENCE AND SOCIAL COMPARISON 2.1 The intergroup nature of innovation processes 2.2 Categorization, identity, and categorical differentiation 2.3 Minority influence and social categorization 2.3.1 Ingroup favoritism and public influence 2.3.2 Salience of the intergroup context 2.3.3 The feeling of shared identity 2.4 Minority influence and evaluation contexts 2.4.1 Original minorities and deviant minorities 2.4.2 When the moderates are more extreme and the extremists more moderate 2.4.3 Influence and ethnocentrism 2.5 Minority influence and identification conflicts 2.5.1 Between favoritism and discrimination: the ingroup paradox 2.5.2 The dissimilation effect 2.5.3 Identification and regulation of membership 2.6 Identification as a model of minority influence 3. THE LIMITS OF DISCRIMINATION 3.1 Behavioral rhetoric and categorization 3.2 Categorization and degree of freedom 3.2.1 Influence as a function of conflict intensity 3.2.2 Negotiation style and the feeling of shared identity 3.2.3 Ideological negotiation and categorization 3.2.4 Ideological discourse and psychosocial regulations 3.3 The indirect effects of categorization 3.3.1 The private influence of outgroups 3.3.2 Outgroup minorities and delayed influence 3.3.3 Beyond social costs: the alternative 3.3.4 Conversion: from the interpersonal to the intergroup 3.4 Conclusions 4. VALIDATION AND MINORITY INFLUENCE 4.1 Learning by imitation or social constructivism? 4.1.1 Selective focusing on the source or on the object 4.1.2 Indirect influence and social constructivism 4.1.3 Minorities and creativity 4.2 The model of validation 4.3 Psychosocial construction of the minority 4.3.1 The ambivalence of minority representation 4.3.2 From a stereotype to a dual representation 4.3.3 Consistency 4.3.4 Validation implies focusing on the source 4.3.5 From intergroup conflict to intragroup change 4.3.6 Homologies and their negation 4.3.7 Conversion: not "in spite of" but "thanks to" categorization 4.4 Validation and recognition of an organizing principle 4.4.1 Organizing principle and perceptual context 4.4.2 Inferential cognitive activity and validation 4.4.3 Consensus about the specificity of a minority norm 4.4.4 Experimental induction of the organizing principle 4.4.5 To validate is not to approve 4.5 Conclusions 5. COMPARISON AND VALIDATION IN MINORITY INFLUENCE 5.1 Explaining the possible 5.1.1 Resistance and/or change 5.1.2 Comparison and validation: their theoretical link 5.2 The paradox of identification 5.2.1 Comparison versus validation 5.2.2 The dual dynamics of social cost 5.3 Beyond comparison, validation 5.3.1 The cognitive way to conversion 5.3.2 Dimensionality of judgments and validation 5.3.3 Salience of the intergroup context and validation 5.3.4 Interdependent or independent judgments, and dissociation 5.3.5 Comparison, validation, and ideological distance 5.3.6 Conflict and dissociation: from discrimination to conversion 5.3.7 Independence, dissociation, and organizing principle 5.4 Conclusions 6. RESISTANCE AND CHANGE 6.1 Psychologization as a form of resistance 6.1.1 Psychologization: the logic of indissociation 6.1.2 Resistance and dimensionality of representation 6.2 Beyond resistance: change 6.2.1 Minorities and the spirit of the times: what causality? 6.2.2 The paradoxical effects of resistance to change 6.2.3 Beyond denial, truth 6.2.4 Changing the norms: a privilege of minorities 6.3 Conclusion 7. REFERENCES not available 8. APPENDICES not available TABLES and REFERENCES: not available Acknowledgements Like most scientific works, this book is not the sole product of the one, or in this case, two authors to whom it is credited. It was made possible through several exchange networks all contributing to its creation. The first is the social psychology research team of the University of Geneva, and in particular, Willem Doise, who introduced us to the study of intergroup relations and with whom we developed our constructivist approach to social psychology; Stamos Papastamou, collaborator in our first book on the issue, The Power of Minorities; and Clause Kaiser and Patricia Roux, with whom so many of our studies were conducted. A second network, this time international and connected to the first, was instrumental in enabling the experiments reported in this book. Serge Moscovici played a central role here, providing initial inspiration and constant suggestions. These networks were supported from outside the University of Geneva, where we found the means to develop our research and confront our ideas with others, particularly through two conferences on minority influence, one held in Barcelona in 1980, the other in Geneva in 1985, with the assistance of the Laboratoire Europ‚en de Psychologie Sociale and the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology. Our experimental research, so fundamental to this approach, was greatly facilitated by several research grants from the Fonds National suisse de la Recherche Scientifique. Gabriel Mugny is a professor of social psychology at the School of Psychology and Education Sciences of the University of Geneva. Juan Antonio Pérez is a professor of social psychology at the School of Psychology of the University of Valencia. Chapter 1 Explaining Change 1.1 The Illusion of immobility and the reality of change Influence phenomena refer to the processes through which individuals and groups form, maintain, spread, and modify their thought and action modes during direct or symbolic social interaction. The question of how to approach these processes is a fundamental one, not only for researchers in the fields of psychology, sociology, anthropology, or like ours, social psychology, but also for groups and societies themselves. These processes form the very basis of how society functions and evolves, for as Touraine (1973, 10) writes, "Human society is the only natural system known to have the ability to form and transform its functioning on the basis of its investments and of the image it has of its own ability to act upon itself" (our translation). Two conflicting tendencies exist in this respect, both from a scientific point of view and the broader social perspective. One approach has focused on the reproduction and maintenance of social relations, the status quo. This perspective is a "conservative" one from the sociopolitical standpoint and a "functionalist" one from the scientific standpoint. Moscovici (1976) was one of the first to uncover the implicit postulates wherein social systems are conceived as optimal, functional, even ideal, or nearly so. The advocates of this approach essentially look at the adaptation processes through which social systems become long lasting and protect themselves against change, regarded as a social evil. What is implicit here is the idea that there is only one correct, ahistorical, and somehow predetermined view of the world and its values. In short, the functionalist view is based on the illusion that norms and ideas are immobile, using Balandier's (1985) term. Whenever the value a norm takes on is defined in this manner, i.e. in absolute rather than relative terms, the issue becomes searching for the conditions and mechanisms, the (nearly "natural") laws through which individuals and groups internalize or adapt to that norm, or in the worst of cases, deviate from it. For the most part these studies have dealt with the mechanisms of conformity and obediance, in short, of social control (the adaptation pole) and self-control (the internalization pole). These mechanisms are the constituents of majorities and dominant positions, which are both the cause and the effect of social influence, considered to be responsible for maintaining uniformity and consensus by reducing interindividual differences. Others, including ourselves, are fascinated instead by the question of social change and innovation. Ours is an "interactionist" perspective from the scientific standpoint, and a "progressive" perspective from the social standpoint. In the social psychology of influence in particular, researchers working in this perspective in the past twenty or so years have concentrated their efforts on compensating for the emphasis that has been and still is being placed on the dynamics of social control. Given that norms are considered here to be relative, i.e. to be the outcome of compromise or submission, emphasis is placed on the mechanisms underlying these transformations, in this case, the processes of innovation and the spread of innovation, viewed as fundamental to social and historical evolution. At the root of such change, we find individuals and groups that are not so inclined to conform to the status quo or to abide by "universal norms": they constitute the minorities. From both the scientific and social points of view, then, fundamental tensions exist, as demonstrated in a recent book on perspectives in the study of minority influence (cf.
Recommended publications
  • A Recipe for Social Change: Indirect Minority Influence and Cognitive Rebalancing
    1 A Recipe for Social Change: Indirect Minority Influence and Cognitive Rebalancing Jiin Jung1 and Aaron Bramson2 1 Department of Psychology, Claremont Graduate University 2 Laboratory for Symbolic Cognitive Development, Riken Brain Science Institute Abstract. The present paper proposes an agent-based model of indirect minority influence to examine a theoretical assumption that indirect minority influence leads to social change as a function of cognitive rebalancing. An attitude updating algorithm was constructed with minimal assumptions based on social psychological theories on indirect minority influence processes. Simulation results reveal that in the face of direct majority influence, indirect minority influence in combination with cognitive rebalancing is a recipe for social change. We discuss the findings in terms of the social influence literature and suggest some promising avenues for modeling extensions for theory building in minority influence and social change. Keywords: indirect minority influence, cognitive rebalancing, social change, agent-based modeling 1 Introduction Minority influence on social change has been a major topic of social psychology over the past half century (Prislin & Crano, 2012). Early researchers viewed minority dissents as frequent catalysts of social change (Moscovici, 1976),and typically assumed that if minority dissents successfully influence the majority’s attitude in favor of a minority position, this can potentially change the societal norm. Social change refers to the process whereby a society adopts a new belief, attitude, or idea which eventually becomes accepted as a norm. Although by definition social change is a societal-level phenomenon, social psychologists often reduce it to an individual level and then measure individuals’ attitude change, or to a small interactive group level and measure the small groups’ decision in favor of an initial minority position as a proxy of social change (Levine & Tindale, 2015).
    [Show full text]
  • Group Size and Conformity Rod Bond
    Group Size and Conformity Rod Bond To cite this version: Rod Bond. Group Size and Conformity. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, SAGE Publica- tions, 2005, 8 (4), pp.331-354. 10.1177/1368430205056464. hal-00571611 HAL Id: hal-00571611 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00571611 Submitted on 1 Mar 2011 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. 01 Bond 056464 (bc-s) 30/9/05 1:50 pm Page 331 Group Processes & G Intergroup Relations P 2005 Vol 8(4) 331–354 I R Group Size and Conformity Rod Bond University of Sussex This paper reviews theory and research on the relationship between group size and conformity and presents a meta-analysis of 125 Asch-type conformity studies. It questions the assumption of a single function made in formal models of social influence and proposes instead that the function will vary depending on which social influence process predominates. It is argued that normative influence is likely to be stronger when participants make public responses and are face-to-face with the majority, whereas informational influence is likely to be stronger when participants make private responses and communicate with the majority indirectly.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Terrorism As a Tactic of Minority Influence Arie W. Kruglanski University of Maryland Paper Presented at F. Buttera and J. Le
    1 Terrorism as a Tactic of Minority Influence Arie W. Kruglanski University of Maryland Paper presented at F. Buttera and J. Levine (Chairs). Active Minorities: Hoping and Coping. April, 2003. Grenoble, France. 2 Since the late 1970s the topic of minority influence has been an important research issue for social psychologists. Introduced by Serge Moscovici’s seminal papers, minority influence research was itself an example of minority influence in that it innovated and deviated from the tendency to view social influence predominantly from the majority’s perspective. However, as Moscovici aptly pointed out, majority influence serves to preserve existing knowledge whereas the formation of new knowledge, germinating as it typically does in the mind of a single individual, or forged in a small group of persons presupposes the influence of a minority on a dominant majority. The typical metaphor for much of minority influence research was nonviolent influence conducted by the minority members through socially sanctioned means, such as debates, publications, appearances in the media, lawful protests and licensed public demonstrations conducted according to rules. And the prototypical cases of minority influence phenomena were innovations in science and technology, minority-prompted change in political attitudes, shifts in the world of fashion, etc. But in the several last decades a very different type of influence tactic has captivated the world’s attention and mobilized the world’s resources, going by the name of “terrorism” and considered by many the scourge of our times. Though a small groups of social scientists (primarily political scientists, sociologists, and psychiatrists) have been studying terrorism since the early 1970s, only the events of 9/11 catapulted the topic to the very top of everyone’s research agenda.
    [Show full text]
  • Building an Ethical Small Group (Chapter 9 of Meeting the Ethical Challenges of Leadership) Craig E
    Digital Commons @ George Fox University Faculty Publications - School of Business School of Business 2018 Building an Ethical Small Group (Chapter 9 of Meeting the Ethical Challenges of Leadership) Craig E. Johnson George Fox University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.georgefox.edu/gfsb Part of the Business Commons, and the Ethics and Political Philosophy Commons Recommended Citation Johnson, Craig E., "Building an Ethical Small Group (Chapter 9 of Meeting the Ethical Challenges of Leadership)" (2018). Faculty Publications - School of Business. 94. https://digitalcommons.georgefox.edu/gfsb/94 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the School of Business at Digital Commons @ George Fox University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications - School of Business by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ George Fox University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Building a11 Ethical Small Group Cooperation is the thorough conviction that nobody can get there unless everybody gets there. -AUTHOR VIRGINIA BURDEN TOWER Never underestimate a minority. -BRITISH PRIME MINISTER WINSTON CHURCHILL WHAT'S AHEAD This chapter examines ethical leadership in the small-group context. To help create groups that brighten rather than darken the lives of participants, leaders must foster individual ethical accountability among group members, ensure ethical group interac­ tion, avoid moral pitfalls, and establish ethical relationships with
    [Show full text]
  • 11 Social Influence
    9781405124003_4_011.qxd 10/31/07 3:09 PM Page 216 Social Influence 11 Miles Hewstone and Robin Martin KEY CONCEPTS autokinetic effect compliance consistency conversion deindividuation door-in-the-face technique evaluation apprehension foot-in-the-door technique group polarization groupthink informational influence lowballing technique majority influence (conformity) minority influence (innovation) norms normative influence obedience to authority referent informational influence self-categorization theory social comparison social facilitation social influence whistleblowing 9781405124003_4_011.qxd 10/31/07 3:09 PM Page 217 CHAPTER OUTLINE This chapter considers two main types of social influence, both of which can be understood in terms of fundamental motives. First, we discuss ‘incidental’ social influence, where people are influenced by the presence or implied presence of others, although there has been no explicit attempt to influence them. We consider the impact of the mere presence of other people on task performance, and the impact of social norms. In the second part of the chapter, we ask why people succumb to social influence, highlighting types of social influence and motives underlying influence on the part of the target of influence. In the third part of the chapter, we turn to ‘deliberate’ social influence. We introduce theory and research on compliance, the influence of numerical majorities and minorities, group decision-making and obedience. Throughout we will see that social influence is an ambivalent concept. On the one hand, it is the glue of society: it makes things work, and society would be utterly chaotic without it. But on the other hand it can be a dark force, underlying some of the most extreme, even immoral, forms of human social behaviour.
    [Show full text]
  • Minority Influence in Opinion Spreading
    Proceedings of the 2015 Winter Simulation Conference L. Yilmaz, W. K. V. Chan, I. Moon, T. M. K. Roeder, C. Macal, and M. D. Rossetti, eds. MINORITY INFLUENCE IN OPINION SPREADING Ugo Merlone Davide Radi Department of Psychology Department of Management University of Turin Universita` Politecnica delle Marche Via Verdi 10 P.zza Roma 22 Torino, TO 10124, ITALY Ancona, AN 60121, ITALY Angelo Romano Department of Psychology Unversity of Turin Via Verdi 10 Torino, TO 10124, ITALY ABSTRACT Social influence has been an object of interest of social psychology for a long time. More recently, sociophysics and Galam’s model provide an explanation of rumors spreading in a population explaining some interesting social phenomena as diffusion of false information. Although Galam’s model and its recent formalizations are suitable to describe some social behavior, they take into account populations with homogeneous agents. Some recent contributions consider agents who do not change opinion and in some cases are able to persuade the others. Starting from social psychology studies about the role of specific seat occupation we provide a heterogeneous model in which those holding minority opinions can strategically choose specific social gatherings to exert their influence. We simulate the opinion dynamics comparing situations in which there is the minority to others with homogeneous agents. Our results show how the opinion dynamics is affected by the presence of such a minority. 1 INTRODUCTION Social psychology has widely investigated the role of social context in shaping individuals’ opinions, attitude and decision-making (Brown 1988, Wood, Lundgren, Ouellette, Busceme, and Blackstone 1994). Social influence, a research area in social psychology, (Brown 1988), shows how consensus arises among individuals, explains how minority positions may succeed or fail in their diffusion, and sheds light on the relation between cognitive processes underlying majority and minority influence (Moscovici and Personnaz 1980).
    [Show full text]
  • Uva-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)
    UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Minority influence in organizations: its origins and implications for learning and group performance de Dreu, C.K.W.; Beersma, B. Publication date 2001 Published in Group consensus and minority influence: implications for innovation Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): de Dreu, C. K. W., & Beersma, B. (2001). Minority influence in organizations: its origins and implications for learning and group performance. In C. K. W. de Dreu, & N. K. de Vries (Eds.), Group consensus and minority influence: implications for innovation (pp. 258-283). Blackwell. General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) Download date:25 Sep 2021 ~~ - .--,.,;.;;.~~..;,;;;~;.~~~~~i;';;';~:;;.~-,.,-:';';;;:':",~_~~";:;;~';::;;';"~;''''''';''--;,-.,.·.:--",.~-:.·-; :~"-~. .:;:::~~_~~~~-~--::;:~-~ ~-. :::..:.-..:.=-~~: ~--~.-,,,"~-,,-.- •. "...... ,.,.~,-~ Minority Influence in Organizations 259 with the majority POlllt of view (Cialdini & Trost, 1998).
    [Show full text]
  • Minority Influence Theory
    UC Berkeley Working Paper Series Title Minority Influence Theory Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1pz676t7 Author Nemeth, Charlan Jeanne Publication Date 2010-05-08 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California To appear in :Van Lange, P., Kruglanski, A. and Higgins, T. (eds) Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology . New York: Sage 2009, in press Minority Influence Theory Charlan Jeanne Nemeth 1,2 University of California, Berkeley Abstract The study of minority influence began as a reaction to the portrayal of influence as the province of status and numbers and from a realization that minorities need not just be passive recipients of influence but can actively persuade. From these beginnings, a considerable body of research, including ours, has investigated how minority views prevail. In the decades that followed, we concentrated, not so much on persuasion or attitude change but, rather, on the value of minority views for the stimulation of divergent thinking. Dissent, as has been repeatedly documented, “opens” the mind. People search for information, consider more options and, on balance, make better decisions and are more creative. Dissenters, rather than rogues or obstacles, provide value: They liberate people to say what they believe and they stimulate divergent and creative thought even when they are wrong . The implications for group decision making, whether in juries or companies, have been considerable and there is increasing interest in research and in practice for the value of authentic dissent in teams and in creating “cultures” of innovation. Overview: My life long interest has been the study of influence --and in particular, influence by those who hold minority opinions.
    [Show full text]
  • How Do Social Dominance and Minority Influence Affect The
    Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and private study only. The thesis may not be reproduced elsewhere without the permission of the Author. i How do Social Dominance and Minority Influence affect the Collaboration of Refugee Services? A thesis presented in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Psychology at Massey University, Albany, New Zealand Sarah Helene Hahn 2015 ii Abstract Social Dominance Theory has problematic implications for humanitarian work: It suggests that stakeholders of the humanitarian sector collectively maintain the social hierarchies that disadvantage the very minorities that they are supposed to empower. Minority Influence Theory, on the other hand, suggests that social innovation in the humanitarian sector can emerge from the bottom-up, thus against the grain of social hierarchies. This thesis explores for the refugee service sector of Auckland, New Zealand, (a) if former refugees are indeed marginalised within the inter-organisational context that is supposed to empower them, (b) if this has detrimental effects on the sector’s performance, and (c) if fostering minority influence might alleviate such effects. The first research question was approached through a stakeholder analysis, which revealed that the social hierarchies within the refugee service sector indeed mirror the marginalisation of former refugees in general New Zealand society. Then, stories of positive and negative incidents of collaboration in the sector were analysed through the lenses of Social Dominance Theory and Minority Influence Theory. A thematic analysis of negative incidents of collaboration gathered accounts of discrimination through disregard, as well as legitimising myths.
    [Show full text]
  • MICROFILMED 1969 INFORMATION to USERS the Most Advanced Technology Has Been Used to Photo­ Graph and Reproduce This Manuscript from the Microfilm Master
    UMI MICROFILMED 1969 INFORMATION TO USERS The most advanced technology has been used to photo­ graph and reproduce this manuscript from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are re­ produced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. These are also available as one exposure on a standard 35mm slide or as a 17" x 23" black and white photographic print for an additional charge. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. University Microfilms International A Belt & Howell Information Com pany 300 North Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA 313/761-4700 800/521-0600 Order Number 8013701 Minority stress, coping and moderating effects of social support Sridhar, Sandhya, Ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • Persistence of Jewish-Muslim Reconciliatory Activism in the Face of Threats and “Terrorism” (Real and Perceived) from All Si
    Persistence of Jewish-Muslim Reconciliatory Activism in the Face of Threats and “Terrorism” (Real and Perceived) From All Sides Micah B.D.C. Naziri ORCID Scholar ID# 0000-0001-9035-1178 A Dissertation Submitted to the PhD in Leadership and Change Program of Antioch University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy November 2019 This dissertation has been approved in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of PhD in Leadership and Change, Graduate School of Leadership and Change, Antioch University. Dissertation Committee • Philomena Essed, PhD, Committee Chair • Jon Wergin, PhD, Committee Member • Anne de Jong, PhD, Committee Member Copyright 2019 Micah B.D.C. Naziri All rights reserved Acknowledgements I would like to express my deep gratitude to my dissertation committee Professor Philomena Essed, Professor Jon Wergin, and Professor Anne de Jong, for their patient guidance, enthusiastic encouragement, and useful critiques of this research work. I would also like to thank Dr. Ashley Lackovich-van Gorp for her advice and assistance as a mentor in the pilot study that initially inspired this dissertation, and as a guide whenever I felt lost. I would like to further thank Professor Laurien Alexandre for inspiring so many aspects of the Hashlamah Project academically, and for helping realign the focus of this dissertation after serious threats to this work emerged. I additionally extend my thanks to Dr. Norman Dale, who has tirelessly and skillfully helped edit this work. My grateful thanks are also extended to my family for dealing with years of study, travel, writing, frustration, and more that all went into this work, and to unnamed friends and martial arts students who have assisted in travel when it was otherwise impossible.
    [Show full text]
  • Normative Social Influence Is Underdetected
    1 Normative Social Influence is Underdetected Jessica M. Nolan University of Arkansas P. Wesley Schultz California State University, San Marcos Robert B. Cialdini Arizona State University Noah J. Goldstein University of Chicago Vladas Griskevicius University of Minnesota The present research investigated the persuasive impact and detectability of normative social influence. The first study surveyed 810 Californians about energy conservation and found that descriptive normative beliefs were more predictive of behavior than were other relevant beliefs, even though respondents rated such norms as least important in their conservation decisions. Study 2, a field experiment, showed that normative social influence produced the greatest change in behavior com- pared to information highlighting other reasons to conserve, even though respondents rated the normative social influence produced the greatest change in behavior compared to information highlighting other reasons to conserve, even though respondents rated the normative information as least motivating. Results show that normative messages can be a powerful lever of persuasion but that their influence is underdetected. Keywords: social norms; social influence; pro-environmental behavior; social inference Authors’ Note: Funding for this study was provided by a grant from the Hewlett Foundation (2001- 7396) and by National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowships provided to the fourth and fifth authors. Our appreciation goes to Kimberly Brown, Allen Risley, and Lori Large from the Social and Behavioral Research Institute and Azar Khazian for their help on Study 1. We also want to acknowledge the work of our field research team on Study 2: Veronica Briseno, Dulcinea Contreras, Matt Dorlaque, Reginald Hartfield, Edgar Medina, Laura Murphy, Leezel Nazareno, Rene Quiroz, Ronald Tilos, Monica Tinajero, and Christina Wade.
    [Show full text]