David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel BarlowHB (1982). David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel. Their contribution towards understanding the primary visual cortex. Trends in Neuroscience, 5, 145-152 TINS - May 1982 145 tical neurons, but had reasoned that light flooding the whole visual field, which made the whole field look bright, was the commonsense stimulus to use, to explore cortical neurons. Even at the retina and lat- eral geniculate nucleus (LGN) a uniform field is not nearly as effective as a small David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel point of light because of lateral inhibition, and Hubel and Wiesel therefore explored Their contributions towards understanding the field with small spots in order to map the the primary visual cortex individual receptive fields of their cortical neurons. Examples of these maps are given in Fig. 2. Unlike the receptive fields at H. B. Barlow lower levels in the visual pathway, those of the cortical neurons were found to be elon- gated, and in consequence the patterned stimulus they responded to best was an Last year's Nobel Prize winners in Physiology and Medicine, David Hubel and oriented slit, bar or edge. Hubel himself Torsten Wiesel (see Fig. 1), published a paper 20 years ago that was a landmark in says that they were aided in making this cortical neurophysiology is. And then a year later, in 196314.~.~, they published their important discovery by the accidental first work on the development of the visual pathway in kittens, and this too opened a observation that a fine dark line caused by a completely new field for physiological study. Even by that daze many people felt their crack on one of their slides gave rise to a work was of suffu:ient merit for the prize, and it has been followed by 15 years of much larger response than the black spot outstandingly successful collaboration producing a flow of important new results year that was intended as the stimulus. afier year. By now the award must be considered, not only one of the most Pattern selectivity in single cells was richly-deserved, but also one of the hardest-earned. already known, and the behavioural import- ance of 'fly-detectors' in the frog's retina' Their work on visual cortex started when development, we shall gain a fair idea of was at that time being emphasized by David Hubel joined Torsten Wiesel in what they have achieved. Lettvin and his colleagues at MIT'zr. But Steve Kuffler's laboratory at Johns Hopkins selectivity for orientation was new, and so in 1958. Hubel, working at the Walter Reed Pattern ~dleettvtty of cortical neurons was the fact that all recordable neurons in Research Institute, had developed a method In 1962, many people were not at all the cortex were pattern selective. The of recording from single neurons in the cor- convinced that it would be possible to make implications were revolutionary; instead of tex, whereas Wiesel had been continuing any functional sense of the activity of a thinking of the primary visual cortex (area Steve Kuffler's own work on the retina. single nerve cell in a sensory pathway, let 17) as a structure with myriads of cells each They published their fu-st results on visual alone in the cerebral cortex which contains taking part in forming every visual image. cortex in 195912. soon after Hubel's arrival some 10 s° neurons. The claim they made, one was forced to realize that each cell had in the laboratory, but it was not until 1962, that the appropriate stimulus could be found its own very specific stimulus require- after Kuffler had moved his laboratory to for any single neuron in the primary visual ments, and consequently when it was active Harvard and at least 4 years after the work cortex whose activity they could reliably each cell 'said' something specific about the had started, that the major paper on the cat's detect, therefore had a major impact. nature of the image in its particular region cortex appeared. As far as cortical Others had successfully recorded from cot- of the visual field. neurophysiology was concerned it was the dawn of a new day, for the preceding night had been illuminated chiefly by the murky glow of evoked potentials and electro- encephalography. They reported three main results: first, they believed that neurons in primary visual cortex could always be excited by light if the appropriate patterned stimulus was found; second, the majority of neurons were binocular and could be excited through either eye; and third, they showed that cells of a given type were clustered according to a columnar microstructure. In their developmental work they showed both a high degree of ontogenetic determination of the features of the visual pathway, and striking plasticity resulting from abnormalities of experience. All of these findings were new, and all have been amply confirmed, though with certain qualifications and additions. If we consider these four problems, i.e. pattern selectivity, Fig. I. David Hubel (left) and Torsten Wiesel(right). (Photographwas taken at the Cold Spring Harbor binocularity, columnar microstructure and Symposiumin 1975by Colin Blakemore.) ~ Elsevier Biomedical Press /9~2 LI37~ 5~I2,~2,'(~K~ i~t,$o2 75 BarlowHB (1982). David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel. Their contribution towards understanding the primary visual cortex. Trends in Neuroscience, 5, 145-152 146 17N,S' ,~I.~/~2 Different cells differed from each other in many ways. Some preferred one orienta- A C <~,/4/ O ~ ~9/" tiom some preferred another, and similarly m with the size of the bar, and whether it was w bright or dark, and the direction it was moved in. But there was also a major di~ 8 ,, tinction into two types they called simple and complex. The former is the type shown ~" ~, ~, ~" ,~ ._.~g,¢ 44 < ,d, ,va ~- .p in Fig. 2 and these cells responded to pat- " ~" " terned stimuli in a way that could be roughly predicted from the shape and struc- ture of their receptive fields, as plotted with E a small spot of light turned on and off. "a.~-a,x~n"/~ Others, however, had larger receptive ", fields and often gave both 'on' and 'off "~_.~/a4.~ 4"X4" responses to a small spot over large areas. ~--n ..~ X -r ~" ,z::7 4- "~ / . 4 "~* a',.'/'b these other cells also preferred one orienta- tion, and often one direction of movement, ve~ just like the simple type. They named them 4. complex and suggested that they were acti- Fig. 2. Examples of receptive fields plotted from isolated units in the primary visual cortex of the cat. When a vated by a collection of neurons of the spot oflight was aimed at a position marked by a cross it caused a response when turned on. whereas for a simple type that all shared the property of position marked by a triangle the response occurred when it was turned off Summation occurred when light preferring one particular orientation, as fell simultaneously in regions marked alike, whereas light falling in an oppositely marked region reduced the shown in the lower half of Fig. 3. Since response. The top left fields (11,and B) are from fibres arriving at the cortex from the geniculate. The remainder are from the simple type of cortical neuron, and the response to patterns of light could be they had larger receptive fields they effec- predicted qualitatively from the arrangement of on and off areas of the receptive fields. (This Lt not true for tively generalized the detection of orienta- complex cells.) (FromRef. 13. ) tion over a range of positions; thus, they might provide a rudimentary example of the physiological mechanism lor positional invariance - the ability to recognize the same feature anywhere in the visual field. In later studies they investigated other cortical areas ~ and extended the work to monkeys ~7 as well as cats. This part of their work is a superb example of what might be called the natural history approach to neurophysiology: they observed what their single cells responded best to and classified them, but they made little attempt to ana- lyse the mechanism whereby pattern selec- tivity was achieved, or to fathom the sig- nificance of the types of selectivity F--~--~----/--- / observed. It must be said that their two major suggestions about mechanism have not stood up well. Their idea was that pat- tern selectivity was achieved by summation / from a selected subset of inputs: geniculate afferents from a row ofretinal neurons give I~ ~ ~:i a cortical neuron its orientational prefer- h,i,P' i~lJ~. ~1 ence' they suggested' as sh°wn at the t°P °f [~]~ ~ ~ Fig. 3. It now appears much more likely that inhibition plays the major role in mak- I ing cortical neurons selective~4, just as it I does in retina*, for drugs antagonizing I inhibitory transmitters remove pattern [ ,selectivity. Similarly their suggestion about the hierarchical arrangement of cortical neurons has been undermined by the dis- Fig. 3, The mechanisms for the selectivity of simple and complex cells suggested by Hubel and Wiesel. At the covery of parallel pathways in the visual top tej~ the receptive fields of three retinal ganglion cells are shown. The messages from a large number of system~7, by the demonstration that at least such cells, all lying in a row, are lm~ssed through lateral geniculate neurons and converge on a simple cell. which conSequcntly has a linear receptive field. Below are shown the receptive fields of three dmple type some of the complex cells are directly cortical neurons whose axons converge on a complex cortical neuron. Nowiulays it is recognized thai a excited by geniculate afferentsZ% and by the simple cortical neuron often has very few excitatory geniculate afferents, and that inhibition plays an important discovery of visual patterns that are ineffec- part in their pattern selectivity.
Recommended publications
  • The Creation of Neuroscience
    The Creation of Neuroscience The Society for Neuroscience and the Quest for Disciplinary Unity 1969-1995 Introduction rom the molecular biology of a single neuron to the breathtakingly complex circuitry of the entire human nervous system, our understanding of the brain and how it works has undergone radical F changes over the past century. These advances have brought us tantalizingly closer to genu- inely mechanistic and scientifically rigorous explanations of how the brain’s roughly 100 billion neurons, interacting through trillions of synaptic connections, function both as single units and as larger ensem- bles. The professional field of neuroscience, in keeping pace with these important scientific develop- ments, has dramatically reshaped the organization of biological sciences across the globe over the last 50 years. Much like physics during its dominant era in the 1950s and 1960s, neuroscience has become the leading scientific discipline with regard to funding, numbers of scientists, and numbers of trainees. Furthermore, neuroscience as fact, explanation, and myth has just as dramatically redrawn our cultural landscape and redefined how Western popular culture understands who we are as individuals. In the 1950s, especially in the United States, Freud and his successors stood at the center of all cultural expla- nations for psychological suffering. In the new millennium, we perceive such suffering as erupting no longer from a repressed unconscious but, instead, from a pathophysiology rooted in and caused by brain abnormalities and dysfunctions. Indeed, the normal as well as the pathological have become thoroughly neurobiological in the last several decades. In the process, entirely new vistas have opened up in fields ranging from neuroeconomics and neurophilosophy to consumer products, as exemplified by an entire line of soft drinks advertised as offering “neuro” benefits.
    [Show full text]
  • Socity the Physiologicalsociety Newsletter
    p rVI i~ ne Pal Newsette Socity The PhysiologicalSociety Newsletter Contents 1 Physiological Sciences at Oxford - Clive Ellory 2 Neuroscience Research at Monash University - Uwe Proske 4 Committee News 4 Grants for IUPS Congress, Glasgow, 1993 4 COPUS - Committee on the Public Understanding of Science 4 Nominations for election to Membership 5 Computers in Teaching Initiative 5 Membership Subscriptions for 1993 5 Benevolent Fund 6 Wellcome Prize Lecturer 6 Retiring Committee Members 8 Letters & Reports 8 Society's Meetings 9 Animal Research - Speaking in Schools 9 Colin Blakemore - FRS 9 Happy 80th Birthday 10 Talking Point in the Biological Sciences - Simon Brophy,RDS 11 Chance & Design 11 Views 11 Muscular Dystrophy Group - SarahYates 14 The Multiple Sclerosis Society - John Walford 14 British Diabetic Association - Moira Murphy 15 Biomedical Research in the SERC - Alan Thomas 16 Cancer Research Campaign - TA Hince 18 The Wellcome Trust - JulianJack 22 Articles 22 Immunosuppression in Multiple Sclerosis - A N Davison 23 Hypoxia - a regulator of uterine contractions in labour? - Susan Wray 25 Pregnancy and the vascular endothelium - Lucilla Poston 28 Society Sponsored Events 28 IUPS Congress 93, list of themes 32 Notices 35 Tear-Out Forms 35 Affiliates 37 Grey Book Updates Administrations & Publications Office, P 0 Box 506, Oxford, OXI 3XE Tel: (0865) 798498 Fax: (0865) 798092 Produced by Kwabena Appenteng, Heather Dalitz and Clare Haigh The PhysiofogicafSociety 9ewsfetter Physiological Sciences at Oxford The two year interval since the last meeting of the Society in Lecturer in the department for some time, has been appointed to Oxford corresponds with the time I have been standing in for a university lectureship, in association with Balliol College.
    [Show full text]
  • Network AW 2004.Qxd 10/11/2004 13:40 Page 1
    Network AW 2004.qxd 10/11/2004 13:40 Page 1 network Autumn/Winter 2004 News, views and information from the Medical Research Council In this issue... MRC integral to UK clinical Funding update research drive News of competition for MRC funding in Speaking at the Academy of Medical Sciences (AMS) in 2004/05 and the application June, Lord Warner of Brockley announced the formation review process of the UK Clinical Research Collaboration (UKCRC) – a page 2 multi-partner initiative launched to give a major boost to clinical research in the UK.The Government has also set up a new MRC/Health Departments (HDs) Joint Health Francis Crick Delivery Group to increase the strategic coordination of 1916-2004 publicly funded medical research and support the UKCRC. A colleague pays tribute to one of the founding fathers UKCRC of molecular biology The UKCRC was a key recommendation of the Research page 4 for Patient Benefit Working Party set up in response to "We welcome Chancellor Gordon Brown's budget influential reports in Autumn 2003 by the AMS and the announcement of a cash injection for medical research Government's Bioscience Innovation and Growth Team. over the next four years, and will be working even more Basic Technology With a mission to coordinate and transform clinical closely with the Government and the Health Departments November showcase for research in the UK, the partnership will involve the MRC, to address national health priorities in the years ahead." progress by MRC scientists Government, the NHS, academia, medical charities, Colin Blakemore, MRC Chief Executive in the Research Councils UK industry and the public.
    [Show full text]
  • The Use of Non-Human Primates in Research in Primates Non-Human of Use The
    The use of non-human primates in research The use of non-human primates in research A working group report chaired by Sir David Weatherall FRS FMedSci Report sponsored by: Academy of Medical Sciences Medical Research Council The Royal Society Wellcome Trust 10 Carlton House Terrace 20 Park Crescent 6-9 Carlton House Terrace 215 Euston Road London, SW1Y 5AH London, W1B 1AL London, SW1Y 5AG London, NW1 2BE December 2006 December Tel: +44(0)20 7969 5288 Tel: +44(0)20 7636 5422 Tel: +44(0)20 7451 2590 Tel: +44(0)20 7611 8888 Fax: +44(0)20 7969 5298 Fax: +44(0)20 7436 6179 Fax: +44(0)20 7451 2692 Fax: +44(0)20 7611 8545 Email: E-mail: E-mail: E-mail: [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Web: www.acmedsci.ac.uk Web: www.mrc.ac.uk Web: www.royalsoc.ac.uk Web: www.wellcome.ac.uk December 2006 The use of non-human primates in research A working group report chaired by Sir David Weatheall FRS FMedSci December 2006 Sponsors’ statement The use of non-human primates continues to be one the most contentious areas of biological and medical research. The publication of this independent report into the scientific basis for the past, current and future role of non-human primates in research is both a necessary and timely contribution to the debate. We emphasise that members of the working group have worked independently of the four sponsoring organisations. Our organisations did not provide input into the report’s content, conclusions or recommendations.
    [Show full text]
  • Torsten Wiesel (1924– ) [1]
    Published on The Embryo Project Encyclopedia (https://embryo.asu.edu) Torsten Wiesel (1924– ) [1] By: Lienhard, Dina A. Keywords: vision [2] Torsten Nils Wiesel studied visual information processing and development in the US during the twentieth century. He performed multiple experiments on cats in which he sewed one of their eyes shut and monitored the response of the cat’s visual system after opening the sutured eye. For his work on visual processing, Wiesel received the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine [3] in 1981 along with David Hubel and Roger Sperry. Wiesel determined the critical period during which the visual system of a mammal [4] develops and studied how impairment at that stage of development can cause permanent damage to the neural pathways of the eye, allowing later researchers and surgeons to study the treatment of congenital vision disorders. Wiesel was born on 3 June 1924 in Uppsala, Sweden, to Anna-Lisa Bentzer Wiesel and Fritz Wiesel as their fifth and youngest child. Wiesel’s mother stayed at home and raised their children. His father was the head of and chief psychiatrist at a mental institution, Beckomberga Hospital in Stockholm, Sweden, where the family lived. Wiesel described himself as lazy and playful during his childhood. He went to Whitlockska Samskolan, a coeducational private school in Stockholm, Sweden. At that time, Wiesel was interested in sports and became the president of his high school’s athletic association, which he described as his only achievement from his younger years. In 1941, at the age of seventeen, Wiesel enrolled at Karolinska Institutet (Royal Caroline Institute) in Solna, Sweden, where he pursued a medical degree and later pursued his own research.
    [Show full text]
  • BY COLIN BLAKEMORE and RICHARD C. VAN SLUYTERS* from the Physiological Laboratory, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 20 August 1973)
    J. Physiol. (1974), 237, pp. 195-216 195 With 8 text-figures Printed in Great Britain REVERSAL OF THE PHYSIOLOGICAL EFFECTS OF MONOCULAR DEPRIVATION IN KITTENS: FURTHER EVIDENCE FOR A SENSITIVE PERIOD BY COLIN BLAKEMORE AND RICHARD C. VAN SLUYTERS* From the Physiological Laboratory, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 20 August 1973) SUMMARY 1. It was confirmed that suturing the lids of one eye (monocular deprivation), until only 5 weeks of age, leaves virtually every neurone in the kitten's visual cortex entirely dominated by the other eye. On the other hand, deprivation of both eyes causes no change in the normal ocular dominance of cortical neurones, most cells being clearly binocularly driven. 2. Kittens were monocularly deprived until various ages, from 5 to 14 weeks, at which time reverse suturing was performed: the initially deprived right eye was opened and the left eye closed for a further 9 weeks before recording from the visual cortex. 3. Reverse suturing at 5 weeks caused a complete switch in ocular dominance: every cell was dominated by the initially deprived right eye. Reverse suturing at 14 weeks, however, had almost no further effect on ocular dominance: most cells were still driven solely by the left eye. Animals reverse sutured at intermediate ages had cortical neurones strongly dominated by one eye or the other, and they were organized into clear columnar groups according to ocular dominance. 4. Thus, between 5 weeks and 4 months of age, there is a period of declining sensitivity to both the effects of an initial period of monocular deprivation and the reversal of those effects by reverse suturing.
    [Show full text]
  • MRC Annual Review 2004/05 1 the MRC Works Across All Medical Research Introduction Disciplines and with a Wide Range of Stakeholders to Improve Human Health
    Working together to improve human health 2004/05 Annual Review The Medical Research Council (MRC) is the UK’s leading publicly funded biomedical research organisation. Our mission is to: ● Encourage and support high-quality research with the aim of improving human health. ● Produce skilled researchers, and to advance and disseminate knowledge and technology to improve the quality of life and economic competitiveness in the UK. ● Promote dialogue with the public about medical research. Welcome 1 Introduction 2 Working in partnership 4 Accelerating research 8 A sense of achievement 12 14 Cancer 16 Cardiovascular disease and stroke 18 Respiratory disease 20 Infectious disease 22 Neurosciences and mental health 24 The ageing population 26 Obesity, diet and diabetes 28 Health inequalities 30 Clinical investigation and trials 32 From discovery to treatments Welcome From Professor Colin Blakemore MRC Chief Executive In this Annual Review you will read about some of the many achievements of MRC-funded scientists in 2004/05 and the partnerships that have made much of this vital work possible. The research that we fund is aimed at tackling major challenges to human health. These range from potentially devastating infections such as AIDS and avian flu to the gradual increase in ageing-related diseases resulting from longer life expectancy. But it is not enough for us simply to support and carry out the highest-quality biomedical science. We must relate what we are doing in the laboratory to clinical research involving patients and the work of the NHS. By focussing on medical research ‘in the round’, as a continuum involving interaction and two-way feedback between a variety of disciplines, we can ensure that our scientists’ discoveries are translated into health benefits for people as soon as possible.
    [Show full text]
  • Medicine COVID-19 and RACISM
    Columbia 2019–2020 ANNUAL REPORT Medicine Columbia University Vagelos College of Physicians & Surgeons FIGHTING TWO VIRUSES: COVID-19 AND RACISM Features: 4 12 20 At a Crossroads: All Hands on Deck The Bioethics of Medicine and Genomics and Justice the Movement Pivot was the action verb that describes how VP&S clinicians, A new ethics division is Work toward health equality researchers, and students expanding VP&S scholarship has begun at VP&S and all confronted COVID-19 when the into the ethical, legal, and of academic medicine as early virus arrived in New York City. social implications of precision summer protests revealed From redeploying clinicians to medicine. The 360-degree the health disparities in areas outside their specialty to perspective includes “studying medicine in general and graduating fourth-year students the studies” as researchers especially in COVID-19 cases. early, VP&S helped “bend the continue to unleash the Students and faculty share curve” at the epicenter of the power to treat, prevent, and their own perspectives on nation’s pandemic. cure disease. The effort also the intersection of COVID-19 provides an opportunity to and Black Lives Matter. bring social science to bear on bioethical questions. On the Cover Steven McDonald, MD, a 2014 VP&S graduate, is one of five faculty members and medical students who share their perspectives on the Black Lives Matter movement. Read about the five and the VP&S plans to promote racial justice, Page 4. Photograph by Jörg Meyer ColumbiaMedicine | 2019–2020 Annual Report Issue
    [Show full text]
  • THE PSYCHOLOGIST Is the Official Monthly Bulletin of the British Psychological Society
    The British THE PSYCHOLOGIST is the official monthly Bulletin of The British Psychological Society. It will publish official statements on behalf of the Psychological Society when appropriate, and from time to time. Society It will also provide a forum for discussion and controversy among The British Psychological Society was members of the Society. As a consequence, views expressed in any founded in 1901, and incorporated by section of this journal which are signed by the writer are the views Royal Charter in 1965. Its principal exclusively of that writer: publication in this journal does not constitute objects are "to promote the endorsement by the Society of the views so expressed. This is in no way advancement and diffusion of a affected by the right reserved by the Managing Edttor to edit all copy knowledge of psychology pure and published. applied and especially to promote the Equally, publication of advertisements in THE PSYCHOLOGIST is not efficiency and usefulness of Members of an endorsement of the advertiser nor of the products and services the Society by setting up a high standard advertised. Advertisers may not incorporate in a subsequent of professional education and advertisement or promotional piece the fact that a product or service has knowledge; to maintain a Code of been advertised in THE PSYCHOLOGIST. The Society reserves the right Conduct for the guidance of Members to cancel or reject any advertisement without notice. and Contributors, and to compel the observance of strict rules of professional conduct as a condition of membership; Information for Contributors to maintain ... a Register of Chartered The Managing Editor welcomes Psychologists".
    [Show full text]
  • Psychoanalytic Knowledge
    Psychoanalytic Knowledge Edited by Man Cheung Chung and Colin Feltham 0333_973917_01_pre.qxd 9/15/2003 1:18 PM Page i Psychoanalytic Knowledge 0333_973917_01_pre.qxd 9/15/2003 1:18 PM Page ii 0333_973917_01_pre.qxd 9/15/2003 1:18 PM Page iii Psychoanalytic Knowledge Edited by Man Cheung Chung Principal Lecturer in Psychology, The University of Plymouth, UK and Colin Feltham Reader in Counselling, Sheffield Hallam University, UK 0333_973917_01_pre.qxd 9/15/2003 1:18 PM Page iv © Editorial Matter & Selection © Man Cheung Chung & Colin Feltham 2003 Chapter 1 © Man Cheung Chung 2003 Chapter 10 © Suzanne Kirschner Remaining Chapters © Palgrave 2003 All rights reserved. No reproduction, copy or transmission of this publication may be made without written permission. No paragraph of this publication may be reproduced, copied or transmitted save with written permission or in accordance with the provisions of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, or under the terms of any licence permitting limited copying issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency, 90 Tottenham Court Road, London W1T 4LP. Any person who does any unauthorised act in relation to this publication may be liable to criminal prosecution and civil claims for damages. The authors have asserted their rights to be identified as the authors of this work in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. First published 2003 by PALGRAVE MACMILLAN Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire RG21 6XS and 175 Fifth Avenue, New York, N.Y. 10010 Companies and representatives throughout the world PALGRAVE MACMILLAN is the global academic imprint of the Palgrave Macmillan division of St. Martin’s Press, LLC and of Palgrave Macmillan Ltd.
    [Show full text]
  • From Controlling Elements to Transposons: Barbara Mcclintock and the Nobel Prize Nathaniel C
    454 Forum TRENDS in Biochemical Sciences Vol.26 No.7 July 2001 Historical Perspective From controlling elements to transposons: Barbara McClintock and the Nobel Prize Nathaniel C. Comfort Why did it take so long for Barbara correspondence. From these and other to prevent her controlling elements from McClintock (Fig. 1) to win the Nobel Prize? materials, we can reconstruct the events moving because their effects were difficult In the mid-1940s, McClintock discovered leading up to the 1983 prize*. to study when they jumped around. She genetic transposition in maize. She What today are known as transposable never had any inclination to pursue the published her results over several years elements, McClintock called ‘controlling biochemistry of transposition. and, in 1951, gave a famous presentation elements’. During the years 1945–1946, at Current understanding of how gene at the Cold Spring Harbor Symposium, the Carnegie Dept of Genetics, Cold activity is regulated, of course, springs yet it took until 1983 for her to win a Nobel Spring Harbor, McClintock discovered a from the operon, François Jacob and Prize. The delay is widely attributed to a pair of genetic loci in maize that seemed to Jacques Monod’s 1960 model of a block of combination of gender bias and gendered trigger spontaneous and reversible structural genes under the control of an science. McClintock’s results were not mutations in what had been ordinary, adjacent set of regulatory genes (Fig. 2). accepted, the story goes, because women stable alleles. In the term of the day, they Though subsequent studies revealed in science are marginalized, because the made stable alleles into ‘mutable’ ones.
    [Show full text]
  • Public Perceptions of S&T in Brazil, Funding Crisis and the Future
    Public perceptions of S&T in Brazil, funding crisis and the future Interest in science and technology The interest in science and technology increased 15% in Brazil between the first and the more recent survey CGEE, 2015 Interest in science and technology European Union (2013) x Brazil (2015) 26% of the Brazilians are very interested in S&T, against 13% of the people interviewed in the European Union European Union 2013 Brazil 2015 Not interested Low interested Interested Very interested DK DA CGEE, 2015 Who is interested? DA DN Very interested Interested Not very interested Not interested Illiterate Elementary school Elementary High school University (incomplete) school (complete) degree (complete) CGEE, 2015 Those who have more formal education have more interest in S&T Do you remember... Do you know any Brazilian institution dedicated to scientific research? Do you remember the name of a Brazilian scientist? No Yes DA CGEE, 2015 50% of Brazilians think scientists are smart people who do useful things for humanity Science brings only benefits: 1987–12% 2006–29% 2010–38% 2015–54% What inspires the scientists? Helping humanity (34%), contributing to the advancement of knowledge (17%), contributing to the scientific and technological development of the country (15%) People in the government should follow guidelines developed by scientists Partially agree - 41% Completely agree - 18% Scientific and technological developments will lead to less social inequalities Partially agree - 35% Completely agree - 17% Considering the surveys... - People
    [Show full text]