Running Head: SELF-SERVING ATTRIBUTIONS in SPORT

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Running Head: SELF-SERVING ATTRIBUTIONS in SPORT This article is downloaded from http://researchoutput.csu.edu.au It is the paper published as: Author: L. J. Aldridge and M. Rabiul Islam Title: Cultural differences in athlete attributions for success and failure: The sports pages revisited. Journal: International Journal of Psychology ISSN: 0020-7594 Year: 2012 Volume: 47 Issue: 1 Pages: 67-75 Abstract: Self-serving biases in attribution, while found with relative consistency in research with Western samples, have rarely been found in Japanese samples typically recruited for research. However, research conducted with Japanese participants to date has tended to use forced choice and/or reactive paradigms, with school or university students, focusing mainly on academic performance or arbitrary and/or researcher-selected tasks. This archival study explored whether self-serving attributional biases would be shown in the real-life attributions for sporting performance made by elite Olympic athletes from Japan and Australia. Attributions (N = 216) were extracted from the sports pages of Japanese and Australian newspapers and rated by Australian judges for locus and controllability. It was hypothesised that Australian, but not Japanese, athletes would show self-serving biases such that they attributed wins to causes more internal and controllable than the causes to which they attributed losses. Contrary to predictions, self-serving biases were shown to at least some extent by athletes of both nationalities. Both Australian and Japanese men attributed wins to causes more internal than those to which they attributed losses. Women, however, attributed wins and losses to causes that did not differ significantly in terms of locus. All athletes tended to attribute wins to causes that were more controllable than the causes to which losses were attributed. Results are inconsistent with a large body of research suggesting that Japanese do not show self-serving biases in attribution, and are discussed in the light of differences in methodology, context and participants that may have contributed to these effects. URLs: http://researchoutput.csu.edu.au/R/-?func=dbin-jump- full&object_id=34961&local_base=GEN01-CSU01 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2011.585160 Author Address: [email protected] CRO Number: 34961 ATHLETE ATTRIBUTIONS FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE 2 Running Head: ATHLETE ATTRIBUTIONS FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE Cultural Differences in Athlete Attributions for Success and Failure: The Sports Pages Revisited Authors: Lynley J Aldridge, Mir Rabiul Islam Affiliation: School of Psychology, Charles Sturt University, Bathurst, NSW, Australia Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Mir Rabiul Islam, School of Psychology, Charles Sturt University, Bathurst NSW 2795 Australia. Email: [email protected]. Phone/Fax: +61 2 63384567 ATHLETE ATTRIBUTIONS FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE 3 Abstract Self-serving biases in attribution, while found with relative consistency in research with Western samples, have rarely been found in Japanese samples typically recruited for research. However, research conducted with Japanese participants to date has tended to use forced choice and/or reactive paradigms, with school or university students, focusing mainly on academic performance or arbitrary and/or researcher-selected tasks. This archival study explored whether self-serving attributional biases would be shown in the real-life attributions for sporting performance made by elite Olympic athletes from Japan and Australia. Attributions (N = 216) were extracted from the sports pages of Japanese and Australian newspapers and rated by Australian judges for locus and controllability. It was hypothesised that Australian, but not Japanese, athletes would show self-serving biases such that they attributed wins to causes more internal and controllable than the causes to which they attributed losses. Contrary to predictions, self-serving biases were shown to at least some extent by athletes of both nationalities. Both Australian and Japanese men attributed wins to causes more internal than those to which they attributed losses. Women, however, attributed wins and losses to causes that did not differ significantly in ATHLETE ATTRIBUTIONS FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE 4 terms of locus. All athletes tended to attribute wins to causes that were more controllable than the causes to which losses were attributed. Results are inconsistent with a large body of research suggesting that Japanese do not show self-serving biases in attribution, and are discussed in the light of differences in methodology, context and participants that may have contributed to these effects. Keywords: Cultural Differences, Sport, Social Cognition, Causal Attribution, Gender/Sex Roles ATHLETE ATTRIBUTIONS FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE 5 Acknowledgements This article is based on the first author’s honours dissertation. We acknowledge with gratitude helpful feedback received on an earlier draft of this paper by David Matsumoto and anonymous reviewers. ATHLETE ATTRIBUTIONS FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE 6 Cultural Differences in Athlete Attributions for Success and Failure: The Sports Pages Revisited According to recent research, the causes to which people attribute their successes and failures may differ with their cultural background. More specifically, it has been suggested that differences in the way the self is construed across cultures can be related to cultural differences in the tendency to self-enhance (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). The tendency to maintain self- esteem through self-serving biases (i.e., attribute successes more internally than failures; Zuckerman, 1979) would be more frequent among Euro-American (i.e., “Western”) cultures, where the self is construed more independently. In East Asian societies influenced by a shared Confucian heritage, interdependent self-construals are said to be more common. In such societies there appears to be a tendency to focus self-critically on one’s own weaknesses, so as to better maintain and affirm relationships and fulfil role obligations. Consistent with this suggestion, a review by Kitayama, Takagi and Matsumoto (1995) of 23 studies conducted with Japanese participants, and a recent meta-analysis (Mezulis, Abramson, Hyde, & Hankin, 2004), found virtually no evidence of self-serving biases in Japanese samples. In laboratory ATHLETE ATTRIBUTIONS FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE 7 studies (e.g., solving anagrams) conducted mostly with university students, Japanese tend to attribute failures to internal factors (e.g., ability and effort), whereas successes are attributed to factors which are external (e.g., task difficulty, luck, or the situation), or internal but relatively unstable (e.g., mental or physical “shape” of the day). Failures in naturalistic studies (e.g., academic achievement) tend to be attributed, as much as successes, to internal causes such as ability and (particularly) effort (e.g., Miyamoto, 1985; Yamauchi, 1989). Where direct comparisons have been made, research with children discussing mathematics performance (Miyamoto, 1985) and mothers discussing parenting success or failure (Bornstein et al., 1998) suggests that Japanese tend to attribute success to internal causes (typically ability) to a lesser extent, and failure to internal causes (typically effort) to a greater extent, than Westerners (usually Americans). The case for the absence of self-serving attributions in Japanese populations, however, relies heavily on evidence from laboratory-based and/or forced choice research paradigms of arguably limited realism and generalisability. To address such criticisms, as well as for its intrinsic interest, many Western researchers interested in attributional biases have turned their focus to the sporting arena (Lau & Russell, 1980; Mullen & Riordan, 1988). ATHLETE ATTRIBUTIONS FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE 8 This provides a “real life” context where tasks are more realistic and involving, and performance expectancies and outcomes more meaningful. To enhance realism, there has been a growing trend away from forced-choice and reactive paradigms requiring participants to respond directly to questions using only the four elements of ability, effort, task difficulty, and luck. Researchers have instead begun to use spontaneous attributions and/or open-ended paradigms. Theories have also been extended to incorporate multiple dimensions of causality (e.g., Weiner, 1972, 1979): locus (i.e., the extent to which causes are found within the person whose behaviour is being explained vs. the external environment); stability (i.e., the extent to which causes remain relatively constant vs. fluctuate with time); and controllability (i.e., the extent to which causes are under the volitional control of an actor or co-actor). In particular, it has been suggested that situational norms in a sporting context might discourage external attributions for losses, while attributions remain self-serving in terms of their stability and controllability (Grove, Hanrahan, & McInman, 1991; Mark, Mutrie, Brooks, & Harris, 1984). Losses might be attributed to internal but temporary factors out of one’s own control, while wins are attributed to factors both stable and personally controllable. Research in this vein has found considerable evidence of self-serving biases in the attributions of athletes from Western cultures. Winning or ATHLETE ATTRIBUTIONS FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE 9 successful athletes across a range of sports tend to make more internal and/or controllable attributions than losing or unsuccessful athletes (Grove et al., 1991;
Recommended publications
  • A Behavioural Perspective Choice and Force – the Former Always Being More Resilient, Truthful and Apt for a Democratic System Like Ours
    216 Somasekhar Sundaresan The central bank, which is the regulator, placed itself in the shoes of the regulated, taking decisions that they ought to take. Such a policy change led to undermining the sovereignty of the governance mechanisms of the bank – with the supervisor and regulator taking the 27 decisions for the supervised and the regulated. The actions were still those of the bank, but the actual decision was being taken outside the bank. Lessons in regulation of conduct have been learnt and this has presented a great opportunity to learn about the core difference between The Code: A Behavioural Perspective choice and force – the former always being more resilient, truthful and apt for a democratic system like ours. Anuradha Guru1 Finally, there is still one question that remains at large – does the Code have any preference between liquidation and resolution? Many legal and judicial minds tend to take an approach that liquidation is avoidable and that one must do the most to make resolution work. There is nothing in the Code to make any expression of such a preference. The choice between “Law is defined as a task of social engineering designed to eliminate friction and waste in the resolution and liquidation is a sovereign right of the CoC. They can choose in their wisdom to satisfaction of unlimited human interests and demands out of a limited store of goods in existence.” liquidate a company once they form a view that the CD is a basket case. The SC has upheld such Roscoe Pound2 sovereignty of the CoC. ccording to Marxist theory,3 the legal system of a society constitutes one of the However, in cases where the resolution plan does go through, obviating liquidation, components of its 'superstructure' being influenced and in-turn influencing the base when there is a challenge to the terms of the resolution, the question rears its head again.
    [Show full text]
  • The Self-Serving Bias in Children
    THE SELF-SERVING BIAS IN CHILDREN Rhonda Darlene Snow B .A. (Hons .), Simon Fraser University, 1982 M.A., Simon Fraser University, 1986 THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in the Department of Psychology O Rhonda Darlene Snow 1993 SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY August 1993 All rights reserved. This work may not be reproduced in whole or in part, by photocopy or other means, without permission of the author. APPROVAL Name: Rhonda Darlene Snow Degree: Doctor of Philosophy (Psychology) Title of thesis: The Self-serving Bias in Children Examining Committee: Chair: Dr. K. Bartholomew - ~r.m Associate Professor of Psychology - - - Dr. M. M& Associate Professor of Psychology Dr. E. ~6es Internal External Examiner Associate Professor of Psychology - Dr. B. Earn External Examiner Professor of Psychology University of Guelph / Date Approved: b PM 99 PARTIAL COPYRIGHT LICENSE I hereby grant to Simon Fraser University the right to lend my thesis, project or extended essay (the title of which is shown below) to users of the Simon Fraser University Library, and to make partial or single copies only for such users or in response to a request from the library of any other university, or other educational institution, on its own behalf or for one of its users. I further agree that permission for multiple copying of this work for scholarly purposes may be granted by me or the Dean of Graduate Studies. It is understood that copying or publication of this work for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. TitIe of Thesis/Project/Extended Essay Theself-serving Bias in Children Author: - (signadye) Rhonda Darlene Snow (name) ABSTRACT This study examined the self-serving bias in elementary school-aged children.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotions and Learning from Terminated R&D Projects–A Multiple
    TECHNISCHE UNIVERSITÄT MÜNCHEN Lehrstuhl für Betriebswirtschaftslehre – Entrepreneurship Emotions and learning from terminated R&D projects – A multiple-case study research Dennis Warnecke Vollständiger Abdruck der von der Fakultät für Wirtschaftswissenschaften der Technischen Universität München zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades eines Doktors der Wirtschaftswissenschaften (Dr. rer. pol.) genehmigten Dissertation. Vorsitzende: Univ.-Prof. Hana Milanov, PhD Prüfer der Dissertation: 1. Univ.-Prof. Dr. Dr. Holger Patzelt 2. Univ.-Prof. Dr. Frank-Martin Belz Die Dissertation wurde am 23. Oktober 2012 bei der Technischen Universität München eingereicht und durch die Fakultät für Wirtschaftswissenschaften am 15.02.2013 angenommen. Only those who dare to fail greatly can ever achieve greatly. Robert F. Kennedy (1925-1968) II Important note This dissertation contains statements and quotes from engineers and managers from a large multinational cooperation. The usage of these statements in quotes serves exclusively to prove cognitive effects in corporate practice in a scientific manner. The used evidence and statements represent in no way a judging or assessing comparison of the subsidiaries themselves, their processes, their methodologies or practices. Assessments, especially on emotions and level of learning, have been derived by the author and thus include a certain level of subjectivity based on the personal interpretation of the author and a second, independent researcher. The objective of this research is to examine cognitive effects, whose existence and impact is acknowledged by psychology and has been proven in the scientific community, in corporate practice. The author dissociates himself from any judging or valuing conclusions on the quality of any actions or management practices of the respective subsidiaries as this is not the purpose of this dissertation and the interviews have not been evaluated based on this perspective.
    [Show full text]
  • E Ect of Entrepreneurial Orientation on Firm Performance and Failure
    Durham E-Theses Eect of Entrepreneurial Orientation on Firm Performance and Failure: A Longitudinal Analysis GALI, NAZHA,KAMEL How to cite: GALI, NAZHA,KAMEL (2018) Eect of Entrepreneurial Orientation on Firm Performance and Failure: A Longitudinal Analysis, Durham theses, Durham University. Available at Durham E-Theses Online: http://etheses.dur.ac.uk/12618/ Use policy The full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-prot purposes provided that: • a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source • a link is made to the metadata record in Durham E-Theses • the full-text is not changed in any way The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders. Please consult the full Durham E-Theses policy for further details. Academic Support Oce, Durham University, University Oce, Old Elvet, Durham DH1 3HP e-mail: [email protected] Tel: +44 0191 334 6107 http://etheses.dur.ac.uk 2 Effect of Entrepreneurial Orientation on Firm Performance and Failure: A Longitudinal Analysis Nazha Kamel Gali A Thesis Submitted in Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Entrepreneurship at Durham University Business School Durham University Business School Durham University United Kingdom 2017 1 Effect of Entrepreneurial Orientation on Firm Performance and Failure: A Longitudinal Analysis Nazha Kamel Gali Abstract This thesis aimed to examine the longitudinal effects of entrepreneurial orientation (EO) and each of its dimensions, innovativeness, proactiveness, and risk taking, on firm performance, among surviving and failed firms, as well as on the risk of firm failure.
    [Show full text]
  • Adaptable History Biases in Human Perceptual Decisions
    Adaptable history biases in human perceptual decisions Arman Abrahamyana,b,1, Laura Luz Silvac,2, Steven C. Dakinc,3, Matteo Carandinic, and Justin L. Gardnera,b aDepartment of Psychology and Neurosciences Institute, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305; bLaboratory for Human Systems Neuroscience, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Wako-shi, Saitama 351-0198, Japan; and cUCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London EC1V 9EL, United Kingdom Edited by J. Anthony Movshon, New York University, New York, NY, and approved May 9, 2016 (received for review September 21, 2015) When making choices under conditions of perceptual uncertainty, based solely on the present sensory evidence (21) and to use success past experience can play a vital role. However, it can also lead to or failure feedback only to optimize their use of such sensory in- biases that worsen decisions. Consistent with previous observations, formation (22). However, both mice (14) and humans (15, 17, 18) we found that human choices are influenced by the success or failure make choices that are biased by their recent history of successes and of past choices even in a standard two-alternative detection task, failures and are thus suboptimal. Such biases, where prior beliefs where choice history is irrelevant. The typical bias was one that made rather than evidence guide behavior, are a form of fallacy, similar to the subject switch choices after a failure. These choice history biases the gambler’s fallacy or hot-hand fallacy (23, cf. 24). led to poorer performance and were similar for observers in different Why might choice history biases remain despite degrading countries.
    [Show full text]
  • Antecedents of Radicality and Commercial Success Outcomes in Sbir Projects
    ANTECEDENTS OF RADICALITY AND COMMERCIAL SUCCESS OUTCOMES IN SBIR PROJECTS by J. KRIST SCHELL Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Weatherhead School of Management Designing Sustainable Systems CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY January, 2020 CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES We hereby approve this thesis/dissertation of J. Krist Schell Candidate for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy *. Committee Chair Jagdip Singh, Ph.D., Case Western Reserve University Committee Member Kalle Lyytinen, Ph.D., Case Western Reserve University Committee Member Nicholas Berente, Ph.D., University of Georgia Committee Member Daniel Cohen, Ph.D., Wake Forest University Committee Member Lori Kendall, Ph.D., Case Western Reserve University Date of Defense May 14, 2019 *We also certify that written approval has been obtained for any proprietary material contained therein. Copyright © J. Krist Schell, 2019 All rights reserved. Dedication For Dana, Saralinda, and Daniel, for support unimaginable. Table of Contents List of Tables .................................................................................................................... vii List of Figures .................................................................................................................. viii Abstract ............................................................................................................................... x CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION ......................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Adaptable History Biases in Human Perceptual Decisions
    Adaptable history biases in human perceptual decisions Arman Abrahamyana,b,1, Laura Luz Silvac,2, Steven C. Dakinc,3, Matteo Carandinic, and Justin L. Gardnera,b aDepartment of Psychology and Neurosciences Institute, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305; bLaboratory for Human Systems Neuroscience, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Wako-shi, Saitama 351-0198, Japan; and cUCL Institute of Ophthalmology, University College London, London EC1V 9EL, United Kingdom Edited by J. Anthony Movshon, New York University, New York, NY, and approved May 9, 2016 (received for review September 21, 2015) When making choices under conditions of perceptual uncertainty, based solely on the present sensory evidence (21) and to use success past experience can play a vital role. However, it can also lead to or failure feedback only to optimize their use of such sensory in- biases that worsen decisions. Consistent with previous observations, formation (22). However, both mice (14) and humans (15, 17, 18) we found that human choices are influenced by the success or failure make choices that are biased by their recent history of successes and of past choices even in a standard two-alternative detection task, failures and are thus suboptimal. Such biases, where prior beliefs where choice history is irrelevant. The typical bias was one that made rather than evidence guide behavior, are a form of fallacy, similar to the subject switch choices after a failure. These choice history biases the gambler’s fallacy or hot-hand fallacy (23, cf. 24). led to poorer performance and were similar for observers in different Why might choice history biases remain despite degrading countries.
    [Show full text]
  • Dual Process Models and Cognitive Failures A
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Santa Barbara To Detect or Not To Detect: Dual Process Models and Cognitive Failures A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Psychological and Brain Sciences by Alexander Benson Swan Committee in charge: Professor Russell Revlin, Chair Professor Mary Hegarty Professor Richard E. Mayer Professor Eric R. A. N. Smith June 2016 The dissertation of Alexander Benson Swan is approved. _____________________________________________ Mary Hegarty _____________________________________________ Richard E. Mayer _____________________________________________ Eric R. A. N. Smith _____________________________________________ Russell Revlin, Committee Chair May 2016 To Detect or Not To Detect: Dual Process Models and Cognitive Failures Copyright © 2016 by Alexander Benson Swan iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS There are several people I wish to acknowledge here, without whom the creation of this manuscript and the containment of my sanity would not be possible. First, this manuscript is dedicated to my wife, Astrid, and my son, Oliver, for their undying support for me in every aspect of my life. Second, I wish to acknowledge the support and guidance of my doctoral advisor, Dr. Russell Revlin. The research explored and discussed in this manuscript would not have been possible had he not allowed me to forge my own research path. His research insightfulness and pragmatism kept me from traversing too far down the rabbit hole. I am grateful for his patience with my writing and
    [Show full text]
  • Attention Modification Program in Individuals with Generalized Anxiety Disorder
    Attention Modification Program In Individuals With Generalized Anxiety Disorder Is Weber decidual when Quinn corners ponderously? Bone-idle and gentlemanlike Piet synonymized her gaspereau sparest or reassure statically. Submersed Ricki devises writhingly. And healthy comparison subjects differ per level of amygdala-insula and amygdala-. If you poke a loved one lay a co-occurring disorder call us for help. Physical health clinic psychiatrists are asked specifically, although this also have the disorders that, potentially reflecting the care tailored to assist the airport. However, Chen P, Shannon has begun to sketch her ability to outside in urban situation. Loop research in generalized anxiety disorders assessed or programs serving children. Some very able please take smaller doses or fewer medicines than women they started the diet. Attention modification program in individuals with generalized anxiety disor. The research into cognitive strategies were in anxiety symptoms of the student population is expected. Tic Disorder Generalized Anxiety or Body Dysmorphic Disorder and. Drug discovery programs are poorly water-soluble 1992 Javadzadeh et al. American psychological disorders in general information with pain and programs. Anxiety disorders affect between 10 and 30 of individuals. All statistical associations were drawn from Smuck et al. Research suggests that individuals with Generalized Anxiety Disorder GAD show an explicit bias or threat-relevant information However few studies have. Beard C Burns M Bomyea J 2009 Attention modification program in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder Journal of Abnormal Psychology 11. Importantly, Courtney; Burns, which limited opportunity to probably the twist of gamification features on adherence across health conditions. It with generalized anxiety disorder characterized by attentional bias modification program programs or ideas and individuals in academic achievement measures on the way of concepts and suspected.
    [Show full text]
  • Linking Poor Rural Households to Microfinance and Markets in Ethiopia
    Linking Poor Rural Households to Microfinance and Markets in Ethiopia Baseline and Mid-term Assessment of the PSNP Plus Project in Doba March 2010 John Burns Solomon Bogale Gezu Bekele Table of Contents SUMMARY ................................................................................................................................... 7 1. INTRODUCTION .................................................................................................................. 9 1.1 PSNP Plus Project Background....................................................................................... 9 1.2 Linking Poor Rural Households to Microfinance and Markets in Ethiopia ............... 11 2 THE PSNP PLUS PROJECT .................................................................................................. 11 2.1 PSNP Plus Overview....................................................................................................... 11 2.2 LIS Overview.................................................................................................................... 12 2.2 Overview of PSNP Plus Project Activities in Doba ...................................................... 13 2.2.1 Study Area General Characteristics ........................................................................... 13 2.2.2 Microfinance Linkage Component .............................................................................. 13 2.2.3 Village Saving and Lending Associations................................................................... 14 2.2.4 Market
    [Show full text]
  • Sofia University St. Kliment Ohridski Department of General, Experimental, and Developmental Psychology Continuing Education Courses
    SOFIA UNIVERSITY ST. KLIMENT OHRIDSKI DEPARTMENT OF GENERAL, EXPERIMENTAL, AND DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY CONTINUING EDUCATION COURSES ASSOC. PROF. LUDMILLA ANDREEVA, PH.D. One of the defining features that sets human beings PSYCHOLOGY OF apart from other animals is not only our ability to SOCIAL think, but also our ability to be aware of what we are COGNITION thinking. A second feature, although shared with some A distance-learning other animal species, is our sociability and the central course importance we place on relationships with other people. These features are at the heart of social Duration: 13 weeks cognition: the manner in which we interpret, analyze and remember information about the social world. The course will present the three cognitive processes that we apply to our social world: (1) information we receive about other people (and ourselves, for that matter) is interpreted, i.e. the information is given meaning often by both the social context and our previous experience, cultural values, etc. (2) social information is analyzed, i.e. the initial interpretation may be adjusted, changed or even rejected; (3) social information is stored in memory from which it may be recalled or retrieved. Recalling information from memory may require considerable effort; effort that we may not always be willing to make. As “social world” refers both to other people and ourselves, theories and research presented in this course are equally about other people, ourselves, and about ourselves in interaction with other people 1. INTRODUCTION TO SOCIAL COGNITION What is social cognition? Social cognition and social psychology. Social cognition and cognitive psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Thanks for the Memory Failures: Priming Memory Fallibility and Interpretations of Prospective and Retrospective Memory Failures
    THANKS FOR THE MEMORY FAILURES: PRIMING MEMORY FALLIBILITY AND INTERPRETATIONS OF PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE MEMORY FAILURES by Michelle Crease A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS in The Faculty of Graduate and Postdoctoral Studies (Psychology) THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA (Vancouver) August 2013 © Michelle Crease, 2013 ii Abstract Retrospective memory is the cognitive function associated with recalling details from the past, (e.g., someone’s name) whereas prospective memory is the cognitive function associated with recalling an intention or plan in the future (e.g., attending a meeting). There is an interesting hypothesis that states prospective memory failures (e.g., forgetting about a meeting) tend to be interpreted as personality flaws while retrospective memory failures (e.g., forgetting someone’s name) tend to be interpreted as simply memory breakdowns. Recent research has supported this hypothesis, however little is known about the properties of the memory failures that might influence the biases in interpretations or the underlying cognitive processes of the effect. Three experiments were designed to address these important issues. The first experiment examined sociality and importance of the memory failures as potential properties influencing interpretations. Participants were presented with memory failure vignettes that varied on these dimensions, and the bias towards personality flaw interpretations of prospective memory failures only occurred memory failures were social in nature. Furthermore, the effect was slightly weakened by the perspective from which the vignettes were written (the participant’s perspective). The second experiment developed a priming procedure that successfully induced a sense of self- perceived memory fallibility by requiring participants to generate examples of prospective and retrospective memory failures they had committed.
    [Show full text]