Synesthesia for Manual Alphabet Letters and Numeral Signs in Second- Language Users of Signed Languages

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Synesthesia for Manual Alphabet Letters and Numeral Signs in Second- Language Users of Signed Languages See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/304573227 Synesthesia for manual alphabet letters and numeral signs in second- language users of signed languages Article in Neurocase · June 2016 DOI: 10.1080/13554794.2016.1198489 CITATIONS READS 0 118 5 authors, including: Joanna Atkinson David M Eagleman UCL Stanford University 22 PUBLICATIONS 189 CITATIONS 131 PUBLICATIONS 3,519 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE Bencie Woll University College London 200 PUBLICATIONS 3,316 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Assessing deaf children's narrative skills View project Sign lanaguage, perception and cognition View project All content following this page was uploaded by Joanna Atkinson on 28 November 2018. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. Neurocase The Neural Basis of Cognition ISSN: 1355-4794 (Print) 1465-3656 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/nncs20 Synesthesia for manual alphabet letters and numeral signs in second-language users of signed languages Joanna Atkinson, Tanya Lyons, David Eagleman, Bencie Woll & Jamie Ward To cite this article: Joanna Atkinson, Tanya Lyons, David Eagleman, Bencie Woll & Jamie Ward (2016): Synesthesia for manual alphabet letters and numeral signs in second-language users of signed languages, Neurocase, DOI: 10.1080/13554794.2016.1198489 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13554794.2016.1198489 Published online: 28 Jun 2016. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 15 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=nncs20 Download by: [University of London] Date: 02 July 2016, At: 12:53 NEUROCASE, 2016 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13554794.2016.1198489 Synesthesia for manual alphabet letters and numeral signs in second-language users of signed languages Joanna Atkinsona, Tanya Lyonsb, David Eaglemanc, Bencie Wolla and Jamie Wardb,d aDCAL Deafness, Cognition and Language Research Centre, University College London, London, UK; bSchool of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK; cBaylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas, USA; dSackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, Brighton, UK ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY Many synesthetes experience colors when viewing letters or digits. We document, for the first time, an Received 24 June 2015 analogous phenomenon among users of signed languages who showed color synesthesia for finger- Accepted 1 June 2016 spelled letters and signed numerals. Four synesthetes experienced colors when they viewed manual KEYWORDS letters and numerals (in two cases, colors were subjectively projected on to the hands). There was a Synesthesia; fingerspelling; correspondence between the colors experienced for written graphemes and their manual counterparts, British sign language; suggesting that the development of these two types of synesthesia is interdependent despite the fact American sign language; that these systems are superficially distinct and rely on different perceptual recognition mechanisms in deaf the brain. Introduction grammars, and are unrelated to the spoken and written lan- guages that surround them (Sutton-Spence & Woll, 1999). Synesthesia is a phenomenon in which perceptual experi- Numeral signs are considered as part of the lexicons of signed ences, such as colors, tastes, or smells are elicited by stimuli languages and they show variation between and within that are not usually associated with such experiences (see national sign languages (Anderson, 1979; Zeshan, Escobedo Ward & Mattingley, 2006; or Cytowic & Eagleman, 2009; for Delgado, Panda, & De Vos, 2013). Unlike sign languages, man- reviews). It affects around 4% of population (Simner et al., ual alphabets were developed specifically to provide an inter- 2006) and is thought to arise from maturational neurodeve- face with written language. Fingerspelling is primarily used for lopmental differences in the brain, which lead to cross-activa- names of people and places, or to represent words borrowed tion in neural areas that do not usually interact (Hubbard, from written language into signed language. Brang, & Ramachandran, 2011). The most widely documented How might fingerspelling synesthesia relate to existing expla- form is grapheme→color synesthesia, in which written letters, nations of synesthesia such as Ramachandran & Hubbard’s cross- numbers, and words induce color (Simner et al., 2006). For activation theory and adjacency principle (2001a; 2001b: Hubbard example, seeing a letter “j” might induce a concurrent sensa- et al., 2011)? They suggest that genetic factors might lead to failure tion of the color “red”. Letters spoken aloud can also trigger of neurodevelopmental synaptic pruning, such that adjacent brain synesthetic colors (Baron-Cohen, Harrison, Goldstein, & Wyke, regions remain connected into adulthood leading to cross-activa- 1993) but the present study is the first documentation of tion (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001a; 2001b). They state that Downloaded by [University of London] at 12:53 02 July 2016 equivalent synesthesia in the visuomotor language modality, written letters/numbers act as synesthetic inducers because with colors induced by manual fingerspelled letters and V4 color processing is adjacent to the Visual Word Form Area numeral signs. (VWFA,(Cohen & Dehaene, 2004)), which has a key role in mapping Manual alphabets represent orthographic letters of a writ- orthographic input onto lexical representations. Crucially, the ing system using hand configurations to represent each letter, VWFA is cross-modal and activated, during functional magnetic which are combined in sequences of movements to finger- resonance imaging, by any orthographic input whether printed spell written words. The British manual alphabet uses two- letters/words, fingerspelling (Waters et al., 2007), or Braille (Reich, handed configurations, while most other systems, including Szwed, Cohen, & Amedi, 2011). Studies of both BSL and ASL have the American manual alphabet, are one-handed (Figures 1 and reported that watching fingerspelling activates a neural circuit that 2). In all manual alphabets, the hand configurations have been closely overlaps with reading print. Signed languages are strongly designed to represent some visual features of the correspond- left lateralized, harnessingareassuchastheleftinferiorfrontal ing orthographic symbols in at least some letters (see the gyrus, the VWFA, and the lexico-semantic areas of the left middle forms for D in the American Sign Language (ASL) and British temporal gyrus (Emmorey, McCullough, & Weisberg, 2015; Sign Language (BSL) manual alphabets in Figures 1 and 2) but Waters et al., 2007). The perception of fingerspelling is, however, the strength of the relationship varies from letter to letter and additionally linked to activity in bilateral regions dedicated to is sometimes entirely absent (cf. A, E, I, O, U in the BSL alphabet). motion perception and the hand and arm regions of motor cortex Signed languages, used by Deaf communities around the (Waters et al., 2007). Given the extremely close correspondence of world, have evolved naturally, with their own phonologies and CONTACT Joanna Atkinson [email protected] © 2016 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group 2 J. ATKINSON ET AL. Figure 2. American manual alphabet and ASL numerals (Image: http://en.wiki pedia.org/wiki/American_manual_alphabet, Wikipedia Commons Free Media Repository). and the written letter A both elicited the same color) suggest- ing developmental transfer from letter names to the names of Figure 1. British manual alphabet and BSL numerals (South West region) notes despite the arbitrary nature of the conceptual link. Downloaded by [University of London] at 12:53 02 July 2016 (Images: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Fingerspelling, Wikipedia Commons Free The form, the corresponding sound, and the semantic Media Repository, http://www.rod-parrott.pwp.blueyonder.co.uk/site/numbers). meaning of Braille characters can all act as triggers for synes- thetic color. Steven and Blakemore (2004) described the brain regions involved in processing print and fingerspelling, we experience of color synesthesia in six blind participants with predict that color experiences for fingerspelling are equally phe- prior experience of color vision. Several participants had nomenologically possible. Despite this, there are no previously strong color responses for Braille characters, which were documented cases of manual alphabet/numeral sign→color induced either through touching the dots or imagining them synesthesia as far as we are aware. when they heard or thought about a letter, number, or musi- It is also unclear how this type of synaesthesia, if it exists, is cal note. For some individuals, the colors seen were in Braille related to grapheme→color synaesthesia although we formation like an LED display, while others saw blocks of hypothesise that there would be a correspondence of colors associative colors. One participant (JF) had tactile-color across the different modalities. There is substantial evidence synesthesia exclusively for Braille, but not for other textures, that synesthetic colors can be transferred from one kind of or for dots arranged in unrelated spatial arrangements. symbolic representation to another. This can happen at multi- Interestingly,
Recommended publications
  • A Student's Pocket Guide to Deaf Community Terminology
    A Student’s Pocket Guide to Deaf Community Terminology Start ASL, the leading online resource for ASL and Deaf Culture, has created this easy-to-use and convenient Terminology Pocket Guide. If you’re on the go, or whenever you simply need to make a quick reference, you’ll find this tiny yet practical book to be a great friend in your immersion into American Sign Language and the Deaf community. This handy little Pocket Guide contains definitions to those not-so-familiar terms that you may encounter at any time, anywhere. For anyone wanting to truly understand and communicate within the ASL community, we highly recommend having Start ASL’s Terminology Pocket Guide on hand for all situations… Never leave home without it! Beginning when she was only thirteen years old, Michelle Jay has understood her passion and love for ASL. As a hearing person in the Deaf community, Michelle was determined to master everything she could about ASL… not just “how to sign,” but the very best ways to learn to sign as well! As the founder of StartASL.com, the leading online resource for ASL and Deaf Culture, Michelle has tremendous insight into this unique community. Michelle earned a Bachelor’s Degree in Deaf Studies, with an emphasis in teaching, from one of the nation’s premier programs at California State University, Northridge. When not writing books or tending her website, Michelle contributes thought- provoking articles to academic publishers such as Greenhaven Press. Her unique articles have done much to support Deaf Culture, and have been printed in resource publications such as American Chronicle and Perspectives on Diseases & Disorders: Deafness & Hearing Impairments.
    [Show full text]
  • Sign Space As Paralinguistic Device and the ABC Story in American Sign Language
    University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 1996 Sign space as paralinguistic device and the ABC story in American Sign Language William Robert Hay The University of Montana Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Hay, William Robert, "Sign space as paralinguistic device and the ABC story in American Sign Language" (1996). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 7803. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/7803 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Maureen and Mike MANSFIELD LIBRARY The University ofIVIONTANA Permission is granted by the author to reproduce tliis material in its entirety, provided that tliis material is used for scholarly purposes and is properly cited in published works and reports. * * Please check "Yes" or "No" and provide signature * ’ Yes, I grant permission No, I do not grant permission Author’s Signature / / / /X V Date I ^ j ^ ip_____ Any copying for commercial purposes or financial gain may be undertaken only with tlie author's explicit consent. Reproduced with permission of the copyright
    [Show full text]
  • MRID-Self-Paced-Manu
    Minnesota Registry of interpreters for the Deaf Self-paced Modules for Educational Interpreter Skill Development These self-paced learning modules are designed to be tools that educational interpreters can use to enhance their skills in pursuit of certification. Successful completion of this module series does not guarantee the interpreter will successfully obtain certification from the National Registry of Interpreters for the Deaf (RID) or any other certifying body, nor does it guarantee employment in an educational setting. Any interpreter working with these modules must understand that the Minnesota RID, RID and/or Minnesota Department of Children, Families and Learning are not responsible for an interpreter's success or failure in taking any national certification test or in his or her pursuit of employment. MRID, 1996 Fingerspelling Table of Contents I. Goals and Objectives ...................................... 4 I1. Equipment and Materials ................................ 5 /I/. Time.lllllllllIIIImIIIImIm..IIIIII.m.m.m............... 6 lkI Pre-Test ........................................................... 7 V; Overview and Ac fivities A. Uses of fingerspelling.........................I.....m........... f2 k? . Types of fingerspelling ......................................... f3 C. English to ASL ........m.......I1.....................n........... 16 DmTransliteration ...................................................... 20 E. ASL to English ....................................................... 22 Vl. Post-Test ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~.~~~~~.~~~~~~..~~~~~~.~.~~~~29
    [Show full text]
  • American Sign Language
    U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES ∙ National Institutes of Health NIDCD Fact Sheet | Hearing and Balance American Sign Language What is American Sign Language? American Sign Language (ASL) is a complete, natural language that has the same linguistic properties as spoken languages, with grammar that differs from English. ASL is expressed by movements of the hands and face. It is the primary language of many North Americans who are deaf and hard of hearing, and is used by many hearing people as well. Is sign language the same in other countries? There is no universal sign language. Different sign languages are used in different countries or regions. For example, British Sign Language (BSL) is a different A young boy signs “I love you.” language from ASL, and Americans who know ASL may not understand BSL. Some countries adopt features of ASL in their sign languages. LSF are distinct languages. While they still contain some Where did ASL originate? similar signs, they can no longer be understood by each other’s users. No person or committee invented ASL. The exact beginnings of ASL are not clear, but some suggest that it How does ASL compare with spoken arose more than 200 years ago from the intermixing of language? local sign languages and French Sign Language (LSF, or Langue des Signes Française). Today’s ASL includes some ASL is a language completely separate and distinct elements of LSF plus the original local sign languages; from English. It contains all the fundamental features over time, these have melded and changed into a rich, of language, with its own rules for pronunciation, word complex, and mature language.
    [Show full text]
  • Speech-Language-Communication 02/04/2013 14:21
    YouTube Auto-Channels - Speech-Language-Communication 02/04/2013 14:21 YouTube Auto-Channels: Speech-Language-Communication Collated by Liz Panton MRCSLT 22 Sept 2012 http://salt-mine.net/blog/2012/09/22/youtube-auto-generated-channels/ and http://www.youtube.com/user/SaLTmineTV Channels auto generated by YouTube. The information below has been copied and pasted from the YouTube Blog: http://j.mp/PTnLlZ "Channels auto generated by YouTube are channels created by algorithms to collect trending and popular videos by topic. Auto generated channels act like user channels in that you can subscribe to them and stay updated on new videos. 1. Auto generated Channels FAQ: How does YouTube determine when to create an auto generated channel? An auto generated channel is created when YouTube algorithmically identifies a topic to have a significant presence on the site. It might be because there are a minimum number of videos or watch views about this topic. We also determine if the quality of the set of videos in that channel meets some thresholds. How does YouTube auto generate channels? YouTube algorithmically determines the central topics in a video and then uses that information to develop great collections of videos for any topic of interest. These channels do not convey any editorial opinion. How do I get my channels or videos to show up in an auto generated channel? You don’t need to do anything, just follow the normal best practices for discovery of a video - select really good title and description. Remember to focus on quality of tags rather than the quantity.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 6 Signed Languages
    Chapter 6 Signed Languages A few years ago, the noted linguist David Perlmutter published an essay that began with the fol- lowing story,1 told to him by one of his students: The young man told of how, when he was a child, he had a friend with whom he had many good times, although for some reason this girl never talked to him. Once, he went home with her and met her mother and had a very surprising experience. The mother came down the stairs, and it suddenly became obvious to the boy that his friend and her mother were communicating somehow, although he couldn't see how they were managing to do so. When he returned to his own home, he told his mother about it, and his mother explained, ‘They're different from us. They speak. We sign.’ And she did her best to explain to him that there is something called ‘sound’ and something called ‘hearing’, and that his friend and her mother were communicating by means of sound, rather than gesture. As the boy struggled to understand these strange concepts,2 he said, ‘Wow! Are there other people like that?’ And his mother said, ‘Yes. Most people speak. We are the ones who are different.’ For the past few weeks we've been examining the sounds that are characteristic of spoken lan- guage, as well as some of the ways spoken language can be represented in writing. But we should all admit that we have been ignoring an important segment of human linguistic activity. Not all human language is spoken.
    [Show full text]
  • Spring 2013 American Sign Language 100
    Spring 2013 American Sign Language 100, CRN: 20770 January 14, 2013 – May 10, 2013 Tuesday &Thursday Lecture 10:15 AM – 11:30 AM Lab 11:40 AM– 12:45 PM Imperial Valley College Room 2736 Division: Arts and Letters Phone: 760-886-5479 cell Instructor: Alejandra Galaviz Email: [email protected] Office Hours: By appointment only Required Texts: Tennant, R. A. & Brown, M. G. (2010). The American Sign Language Handshape Dictionary (2nd Ed.). Gallaudet University Press. ISBN: 9781563684449 Humphries, T. & Padden, C. (2004). Learning American Sign Language (2nd Ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc. ISBN: 9780205275533 Prerequisites: None I. Course Description: An introduction to American Sign Language and fingerspelling. The course will focus on conversational skills, grammar, and vocabulary as it is used in the Deaf community. Deaf culture will be examined. II. Attendance Policy: • Attendance is mandatory. Any student who misses the first class will be dropped. In order to acquire a new language, you must be present. The final grade in this class will be affected by active participation, including attendance, as follows: Students are allowed a total of three absences; on the fourth the student will be dropped. Arriving late or leaving early will count as half an absence. If you accumulate absences after the drop date, you must make-up* the absences in order to pass this course. • It is the instructor’s discretion to withdraw a student after the add/drop deadline due to excessive absences. 1 | Page • Students who talk during the signing portion of class will be asked to leave and it will count as an absence.
    [Show full text]
  • SASL Journal, Volume 2, Number 2
    Society for American Sign Language Journal Volume 2 Number 2 Article 1 September 2018 SASL Journal, Volume 2, Number 2 Follow this and additional works at: https://tigerprints.clemson.edu/saslj Part of the American Sign Language Commons, and the Education Commons Recommended Citation (2018) "SASL Journal, Volume 2, Number 2," Society for American Sign Language Journal: Vol. 2 : No. 2 , Article 1. Available at: https://tigerprints.clemson.edu/saslj/vol2/iss2/1 This Full Issue is brought to you for free and open access by TigerPrints. It has been accepted for inclusion in Society for American Sign Language Journal by an authorized editor of TigerPrints. For more information, please contact [email protected]. et al.: SASL Journal, Volume 2, Number 2 SASL Journal Volume 2, N umber 2 Fall/Winter 2018 A Society for American Sign Language Publication ISSN: 2474-8277 (online) Published by TigerPrints, 2018 1 Society for American Sign Language Journal, Vol. 2, No. 2 [2018], Art. 1 Society for American Sign Language Journal Volume 2, Number 2 Editor-in-Chief Jody H. Cripps Clemson University Department of Languages – American Sign Language 717 Strode Tower Clemson, SC 29634 [email protected] Fax: 864-656-0258 Copyeditors Betsy McDonald Laura A. Blackburn Georgetown University (Retired) Tidewater Community College Stephen Fitzmaurice Anita Small Clemson University small LANGUAGE CONNECTIONS Society for American Sign Language - Board of Directors Samuel J. Supalla - President Andrew P. J. Byrne - Secretary University of Arizona Framingham State University Harvey Nathanson - Treasurer Karen Alkoby Austin Community College Gallaudet University Gabriel Arellano Jody H. Cripps Georgetown University Clemson University Ronald Fenicle Russell S.
    [Show full text]
  • 111-166-Course-Descriptions.Pdf
    Course Descriptions 112 Course Descriptions Cuyamaca College Catalog 2021-2022 EXPLANATION OF 126 AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE WITH 130 INTRODUCTION TO PHYSICAL SCHOOL AGE CHILDREN 1 UNIT ANTHROPOLOGY 3 UNITS ABBREVIATIONS AND 1 hour lecture C-ID ANTH 110 Explore the methods and benefits of using 3 hours lecture COURSE NOTES American Sign Language (ASL) with hearing People’s place in nature; physical and behavioral school age children. Areas emphasized will characteristics of primates; principles of Courses which meet the requirements for be methods, benefits, and philosophies of evolution and basic outline of human genetics; General Education for the Associate Degree, teaching school age children to communicate description of the record of early humans and CSU GE, and the Intersegmental General using ASL. Upon completion, students will explanation of fossils; present day variability Education Transfer Curriculum (IGETC) are be able to introduce these techniques in among human populations. identified after each course description. The elementary school classrooms and/or at home. AA/AS GE, CSU, CSU GE, IGETC, UC CSU and UC indicators are also included and CSU mean that the courses transfer for at least 140 INTRODUCTION TO elective credit to these two public systems of 130 AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE: ARCHAEOLOGY 3 UNITS higher education in California. FINGERSPELLING 3 UNITS C-ID ANTH 150 Prerequisite: “C” grade or higher or “Pass” in ASL 3 hours lecture If you would like more information on how 120 or equivalent ability to sign This course is an introduction to the field of courses meet your specific degree or transfer 3 hours lecture archaeology; its concepts, theories, data and objectives, please see a counselor.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Fingerspelling and Lexicalized Signs (Stringham)
    Understanding Fingerspelling and Loan Signs Interpreting I (ASL 3310) American Sign Language Utah Valley State College After this discussion, you will Better understand: 1) a brief history, the roles and purposes of fingerspelling in ASL; 2) how to spot, avoid, and correct common errors in fingerspelling production; 3) and understand the existence and usage of fingerspelled loan signs. 1) a. Where did fingerspelling Sign language history, unfortunately, is not • c.1750s: The Abbe de L’Epee founds the come from? well-documented. What is known: world’s first school for the Deaf; uses fin- • 300–800 AD: Despite a vow of silence, gerspelling to teach students French Benedictine monks develop a manual • c.1900s: Educators use the “Rochester system to communicate Method;” essentially fingerspelling and • c.1550: Pedro Ponce de León creates a speech only—no accompanying signs gestural system to aid student-teacher interaction; is first conventionalized sign Why is this history important? language system? • Did Deaf people already have access to • c.1600: Juan Pablo Martin Bonet “claims” or even create a manual alphabet on to invent a system intending to associate their own? Historical evidence seems to handshapes with sound; Myth: Bonet show that they did not. invented the manual alphabet, Fact: he • Fingerspelling’s initial intended usage never used it was to teach speech to Deaf people. 1) b. So why do we use • Although it has been debated for many Generally, Deaf people/ASL signers use fin- fingerspelling? What is it? years whether or not fingerspelling actually gerspelling to communicate proper nouns: What is it for? is ASL, Deaf people/ASL signers do use • personal names • streets the manual alphabet on a regular basis.
    [Show full text]
  • Baldwin-Wallace College 1998-2000 Catalog
    Baldwin-Wallace College 1998-2000 Catalog CollegeSource Visit Career Guidance Foundation at http://www.collegesource.org Copyright & Disclaimer You may: Information •print copies of the information for your own personal use, © Copyright 1994, 1995, 1996, 1997, 1998 •store the files on your own computer for per- Career Guidance Foundation sonal use only, or •reference this material from your own docu- CollegeSource digital catalogs are derivative ments. works owned and copyrighted by Career Guid- ance Foundation. Catalog content is owned The Career Guidance Foundation reserves the and copyrighted by the appropriate school. right to revoke such authorization at any time, and any such use shall be discontinued immedi- While the Career Guidance Foundation pro- ately upon written notice from the Career Guid- vides information as a service to the public, ance Foundation. copyright is retained on all digital catalogs. Disclaimer This means you may NOT: CollegeSource digital catalogs are converted from either the original printed catalog or elec- •distribute the digital catalog files to others, tronic media supplied by each school. Although every attempt is made to ensure accurate con- •“mirror” or include this material on an version of data, the Career Guidance Founda- Internet (or Intranet) server, or tion and the schools which provide the data do •modify or re-use digital files not guarantee that this information is accurate or correct. The information provided should be without the express written consent of the used only as reference and planning tools. Final Career Guidance Foundation and the appropri- decisions should be based and confirmed on ate school. data received directly from each school.
    [Show full text]
  • WHEN FINGERSPELLING THROWS a CURVEBALL by JUDITH ALLEN
    WHEN FINGERSPELLING THROWS A CURVEBALL by JUDITH ALLEN OLIVER (Under the Direction of William A. Kretzschmar) ABSTRACT Inquiry into Deaf adolescent use of sign language is an understudied subject. The major focus of this research was to provide a linguistic description of fingerspelling used by twelve Deaf teenagers in grades 9 through 12 at a residential school for the Deaf. The aim was to provide an account of how these Deaf adolescents use fingerspelling when it comes to frequency of use, word-class, abbreviations, compounds, lexicalizations, and to examine patterns and variations that appear in adolescent’s fingerspelling. There is a great deal of research on acquisition of fingerspelling in early childhood as well as its use in the adult years but there is a limited look at adolescent fingerspelling behavior. In bridging the gap in literature, a sociolinguistic interview was used to gather corpus data, which was analyzed with the aid of the digital tool, ELAN. Using a complex systems framework called The Linguistics of Speech a frequency distribution was carried out and the most frequently produced fingerspellings emerged. Quantitative measures of the distribution of fingerspelled tokens were further expressed visually on the A-curve. The hallmark of this theoretical model is, first, that it has never been used in the study of Sign Language. Second, it considers the individual speaker’s agency in use of variants and provides a way to deal with the considerable amount of variation that exists in fingerspelling. While nouns are the most commonly occurring fingerspellings they are not the most frequent recurring in the language of these adolescents.
    [Show full text]