<<

Women’s Suffrage

Capel R M 1992 El Sufragio femenino en la Segunda RepuT blica. Van Wingerden S A 1999 The Women’s Suffrage MoŠement in Comunidad de Madrid, Madrid Britain, 1866–1928. Macmillan, Houndmills, UK Cohen Y, The! baud F (eds.) 1998 FeT minismes et identiteT s Ward G C, Saxton M, Gordon A D, Dubois E C, Burns K 1999 nationales: Les Processus d’inteT gration des femmes au politique. Not for OurselŠes Alone: The Story of Elizabeth Cady Stanton Centre Jacques Cartier, Lyon, France and Susan B. Anthony. An Illustrated History. Knopf, New Corbin A, Lalouette J, Riot-Sarcey M (eds.) 1997 Les Femmes York [Accompanies the documentary film] dans la citeT . Cre! aphis, Gra# ne, France Daley C, Nolan M (eds.) 1994 Suffrage and Beyond: International K. Offen Feminist PerspectiŠes. Auckland University Press, Auckland; New York University Press, New York; Pluto Press, London DuBois E C 1997 Harriot Stanton Blatch and the Winning of Woman Suffrage. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT DuBois E C 1998 Woman Suffrage and Women’s Rights. New York University Press, New York Classes and Parts of Speech Fagoaga C 1985 La Voz y el Šoto de las mujeres: El sufragismo en Espang a, 1877–1931. Icaria, Barcelona, Spain There is a long tradition of classifying , for the Faure! C 1997 EncyclopeT die politique et historique des femmes: purpose of grammatical description, into the ten word Europe, Amerique de Nord. Presses Universitaires de France, classes (or parts of speech) , , , Paris , , preposition, , numeral, Grimshaw P 1987 [orig. 1972] Women’s Suffrage in New Zealand. , . While each of these terms is useful, Auckland University Press, Auckland; Oxford University and they are indispensable for practical purposes, their Press, Oxford, UK Hardemeier S 1997 FruW he Frauenstimmrechtsbewegung in der status in a fully explicit description of a language or in Schweiz (1890–1930): Argumente, Strategien, Netzwerk und general grammatical theory remains disputed. Al- Gegenbewegung. Chronos, Zurich, Switzerland though most of the traditional word class distinctions Hause S C, Kenney A R 1984 Women’s Suffrage and Social can be made in most languages, the cross-linguistic Politics in the French Third Republic. Princeton University applicability of these notions is often problematic. Press, Princeton, NJ Here I focus primarily on the major word classes Holton S S 1986 Feminism and Democracy: Women’s Suffrage noun, verb, and adjective, and on ways of dealing with and Reform Politics in Britain, 1900–1918. Cambridge Uni- the cross-linguistic variability in their patterning. versity Press, Cambridge, UK Holton S S 1996 Suffrage Days: Stories from the Women’s Suffrage MoŠement. Routledge, London Joannou M, Purvis J (eds.) 1999 The Women’s Suffrage 1. The Classification of Words Š Š Mo ement: New Feminist Perspecti es. Manchester University Words can be classified by various criteria, such as Press, Manchester, UK phonological properties (e.g., monosyllabic vs. poly- Lavrin A 1995 Women, Feminism, and Social Change in Argentina, Chile, and Uruguay, 1890–1940. University of syllabic words), social factors (e.g., general vs. Nebraska Press, Lincoln, NB technical vocabulary), and language history (e.g., MacKenzie M 1988 Shoulder to Shoulder: A Documentary. The loanwords vs. native words). All of these are classes Stirring History of the Militant Suffragettes. Vintage Books, of words, but as a technical term, word class refers to New York [Accompanies the documentary film] the ten traditional categories below (plus perhaps a Miller F 1991 Latin American Women and the Search for Social few others), most of which go back to the Greek and Justice. University Press of New England, Hanover and Roman grammarians. In addition to the terms, a few London examples are given of each word class. Murphy C 1989 The Women’s Suffrage MoŠement and Irish Society in the Early Twentieth Century. Temple University Noun book, storm, arriŠal Press, Philadelphia Offen K M 2000 European Feminisms, 1700–1950: A Political Verb push, sit, know History. Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA Adjective good, blue, Polish Oldfield A 1992 Woman Suffrage in Australia: a Gift or a Struggle. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK Adverb quickly, Šery, fortunately Prentice A, Bourne P, Brandt G C, Light B, Mitchinson W, Pronoun you, this, nobody Black N 1988 Canadian Women: A History. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Toronto Preposition\adposition on, for, because of Rupp L J 1997 Worlds of Women: The Making of an International Women’s MoŠement. Princeton University Press, Princeton, Conjunction and, if, while NJ Numeral one, twice, third Sepe C, Izzi Di Paolo P (eds.) 1997 Il Šoto alle donne cinquant’anni dopo. Ufficio Progretti Donna, Commune di Roma Article the, a Smith P 1996 Feminism and the Third Republic: Women’s Interjection ouch, tsk Political and CiŠil Rights in France, 1918–1945. Clarendon Press, Oxford, UK

16538 Word Classes and Parts of Speech

(In this article, the more general term ‘adposition’ will are content words, and adpositions, conjunc- be used rather than preposition, because many lan- tions, and articles, as well as auxiliaries and words guages have postpositions rather than prepositions, classified as ‘particles’ are function words. While there and word order is irrelevant in this context.) is sometimes disagreement over the assignment of The special status of the classification above derives words and even entire word classes to these two broad from the fact these are the most important classes categories, their usefulness and importance is not in of words for the purpose of grammatical description, doubt. classes are generally open (i.e., equally relevant for morphology, , and lexical they accept new members in principle) and large . This makes the classification more interest- (comprising hundreds or thousands of words), and ing, but also more complex and more problematic content words tend to have a specific, concrete than other classifications of words. Besides the term meaning. They tend to be fairly long (often disyllabic word class, the older term (Latin pars or longer), and their text frequency is fairly low. By orationis) is still often used, although it is now quite contrast, classes are generally closed opaque (originally it referred to constituents). and small, and function words tend to have abstract, The term word class was introduced in the first half of general meaning (or no meaning at all, but only a the twentieth century by structuralist . An- grammatical function in specific constructions). They other roughly equivalent term, common especially in tend to be quite short (rarely longer than a syllable), Chomskyan linguistics is ‘’ (al- and their text frequency is high. This is summarized in though technically this refers not only to lexical Table 1. categories such as and , but also to phrasal The reason why auxiliaries are not included in the categories such as noun phrases and verb phrases). traditional list of word classes is probably merely that The main two problems with the maximal word- they are not prominent in Greek and Latin , class above are (a) that some of the classes intersect but in many languages these ‘function verbs’ are very (e.g., the English word ‘there’ is both a pronoun and important (English examples are be, haŠe, can, must, an adverb), and (b) that the different classes do not will, should ). The class ‘particle’ is really only a waste- have equal weight; while most languages have hun- basket category: function words that do not fit into dreds of verbs and thousands of nouns, there are far any of the other classes are usually called particles fewer and conjunctions, and often only a (e.g., ‘focus particles,’ such as only and also, ‘question handful of adpositions and articles. The solution that particles,’ such as Polish czy in Czy moT wisz po polsku? is often adopted explicitly for the second problem is to ‘Do you speak Polish?,’ or ‘discourse particles’ such as make a further subdivision into major word classes German ja in Das ist ja schoW n! ‘That’s nice! (expressing (nouns, verbs, , adverbs) and minor word surprise).’ classes (all others). (Alternative terms for major and The precise delimitation of function words and minor classes are content words\function words and, content words is often difficult. For instance, while the especially in Chomskyan linguistics, lexical cate- conjunctions if, when, as, and because are unequivo- gories\functional categories.) This distinction is dis- cally function words, this is less clear for words like cussed further in Sect. 2. The solution to the first suppose, proŠided that, granted that, assuming that. problem that is implicit in much contemporary work is And while the adpositions in, on, of, at are clearly that pronouns and numerals are not regarded as word function words, this is less clear for concerning, classes on a par with nouns, verbs, prepositions, and considering, in Šiew of. In the case of adpositions, so on. Instead, they are regarded as semantically linguists sometimes say that there are two subclasses, highly specific subclasses of the other classes. For ‘function adpositions’ and ‘content adpositions,’ anal- instance, there are nominal pronouns (e.g., he, who), ogous to the distinction between content verbs and adjectival pronouns (e.g., this, which, such) and ad- function verbs (l auxiliaries). Another widespread verbial pronouns (e.g., here, thus). Similarly, there are view is that word-class boundaries are not always adjectival numerals ( fiŠe, fifth), adverbial numerals sharp, and that there can be intermediate cases (twice), and nominal numerals (a fifth, a fiŠe). Some between full verbs and auxiliaries, between nouns and languages also have verbal pronouns and verbal adpositions, and between nouns\verbs and conjunc- numerals. Accordingly, this article will not deal with tions. Quite generally, function words arise from pronouns (see Pronouns) and numerals (see Numeral content words by the diachronic process of gram- Systems). maticalization (see ), and since grammaticalization is generally regarded as a gradual diachronic process, it is expected that the resulting function words form a gradient from full content 2. Content Words and Function Words words to clear function words. When grammatic- alization proceeds further, function words may In all languages, words (and entire word classes) can become and finally affixes, and again we often be divided into the two broad classes of content words find intermediate cases which cannot easily be classi- and function words. Nouns, verbs, adjectives, and fied as words or word-parts.

16539 Word Classes and Parts of Speech

3. Defining Nouns, Verbs, and AdjectiŠes the predominant practice in Western grammar has been to give priority to the syntactic criterion. For In the following, the emphasis will be on the content instance, adjectives in German have a characteristic word classes nouns, verbs, and adjectives (for adverbs, pattern of inflection that makes them quite unlike a problematic class, see Section 5.3 below). The nouns, and this morphological pattern could be used properties of the function words are more appro- to define the class (e.g., roter\rote\rotes ‘red (mascu- priately discussed in other contexts (e.g., auxiliaries in line\feminine\neuter)’). However, a few property the context of tense and aspect, conjunctions in the words are indeclinable and are always invariant (e.g., context of subordinate , and so on). rosa,asindie rosa Tapete ‘the pink wallpaper’). These Before asking how nouns, verbs, and adjectives are words would not be adjectives according to a strictly defined, it must be made clear whether a definition of morphological definition, but in fact everybody re- these word classes in a particular language (e.g., gards words like rosa as adjectives, because they can English or Japanese) is intended, or whether we want occur in the same syntactic environments as other a definition of these classes for language in general. adjectives. The widely known and much-maligned definitions of Thus, there is universal agreement among linguists nouns as denoting ‘things, persons, and places,’ of that language-particular word classes need to be verbs as denoting ‘actions and processes,’ and adjec- defined on morphosyntactic grounds for each indi- tives as denoting ‘properties’ is, of course, hopelessly vidual language. However, two problems remain. (a) simplistic from the point of view of a particular The generality problem: how should word classes be language. In most languages, it is easy to find nouns defined for language in general? Morphological pat- that do not denote persons, things, or places (e.g., terns and syntactic constructions vary widely across word, power, war), and verbs that do not denote languages, so they cannot be used for cross-lin- actions or processes (e.g., know, lack, exist), and many guistically applicable definitions. (b) The subclass languages also have adjectives that do not denote Š problem: which of the classes identified by language- properties (e.g., urban, celestial, ehicular). However, particular criteria count as word classes, and which if the goal is to define nouns, verbs, and adjectives in only count as subclasses? For instance, English has general terms that are not restricted to a particular some property words that can occur in the context is language, these simplistic notional definitions do not more … than, e.g., beautiful, difficult, interesting. An- fare so badly. other group of semantically similar words (e.g., pretty, In the first part of the twentieth century, the tough, nice) does not occur in this context. Nobody structuralist movement emphasized the need for rig- takes this as evidence that English has two different orous language-particular definitions of grammatical word classes where other languages have just a single notions, and notionally based definitions of word class (adjectives), but it is not clear why it does not classes were rejected because they patently did not count as sufficient evidence. work for individual languages or were hard to apply The solution to the generality problem that is rigorously. Instead, preference was given to mor- usually adopted (often implicitly, but cf. Schachter phological and syntactic criteria, e.g., ‘if an English 1985 and Wierzbicka 2000) is that one defines word word has a plural in –s, it is a noun,’ or ‘if a word classes on a language-particular basis, and then the occurs in the context the…book, it is an adjective.’ word class that includes most words for things and But of course this practice was not new, because words persons is called ‘noun,’ the word class that includes like power and war have always been treated as nouns most words for actions and processes is called ‘verb,’ on morphological and syntactic grounds. Some older and the word class that includes most words for grammarians, neglecting syntax, defined nouns, verbs, properties is called ‘adjective.’ However, the subclass and adjectives exclusively in morphological terms, and problem has not been solved or even addressed as a result nouns and adjectives were often lumped satisfactorily, and the use of word-class notions in a together in a single class in languages like Latin and general or cross-linguistic sense remains problematic. Greek, where they do not differ morphologically. But

Table 1 Content words and function words Content words Function words word classes nouns, verbs, adpositions, conjunctions, adjectives, adverbs articles, auxiliaries, particles class membership open closed class size large small meaning concrete, specific abstract, general (or none) text frequency low high

16540 Word Classes and Parts of Speech

4. Characterizing Nouns, Verbs, and AdjectiŠes progressive), mood (indicative, imperative, optative, subjunctive, etc.), polarity (affirmative, negative), va- Despite the theoretical problems in defining word lence-changing operations (passive causative, see classes in general, in practice it is often not difficult to Valency and Argument Structure in Syntax), and the agree on the use of these terms in a particular language. person\number of subject and object(s) (see Gram- This is because nouns, verbs, and adjectives show great matical Agreement). Semantic notions that are more similarities in their behavior across languages. Their rarely expressed morphologically are spatial orienta- most common characteristics are briefly summarized tion and instrument. Some examples follow. in this section. (a) Tense. Panyjima (Australia) wiya-lku 4.1 Nouns ‘sees,’ wiya-larta ‘will see,’ wiya-rna ‘saw.’ In many languages, nouns have affixes indicating (b) Subject person\number. Hungarian laT t- Grammatical number (singular, plural, dual, see T T \ Number), case (e.g., nominative, accusative, ergative, ‘I see,’ lat-sz ‘you see,’ lat ‘s he sees.’ \ dative), possessor person number (‘my,’ ‘your,’ ‘his,’ (c) Valence-changing. Turkish unut- ‘forget,’ etc.), and definiteness. Some examples follow. unut-ul- ‘be forgotten’ (passive), unut-tur- (a) Number. Khanty (Western Siberia) xot ‘house,’ ‘make forget’ (causative). xot-yyn ‘two houses’ (dual), xot-yt (d) Spatial orientation. Russian Šy-letat’ ‘fly out,’ ‘houses’ (plural). Š- letat’ ‘fly in,’ pere-letat’ ‘fly over ,’ Šz-letat’ (b) Case. Classical Arabic al-kitaab-u ‘the book’ (nominative), al-kitaab-i ‘fly up.’ ‘the book’s’ (genitive), al-kitaab-a ‘the book (accusative).’ Syntactically, verbs generally take between one and (c) Possessor person\number. Somali xoolah-ayga three nominal arguments, e.g., fall (1: patient), dance ‘my herd,’ xoolah-aaga ‘your herd,’ xoleh-eeda (1: agent), kill (2: agent, patient), see (2: experiencer, stimulus), giŠe (3: agent, patient, recipient). Nouns ‘her herd,’ xooli-hiisa ‘his herd,’ etc. and adjectives may also take arguments, but they are not nearly as rich as verbs, and nouns that correspond to verbs often cannot take arguments in the most Syntactically, nouns can always be combined with direct way (compare Plato defined beauty vs. *Plato (e.g., that house) and often with defin- definition beauty (impossible); additional coding is iteness markers (the house), and they can occur in the required: Plato’s definition of beauty. Verbs always syntactic function of argument (subject, object, etc.) occur as predicates without additional coding, whereas without additional coding. Thus, in a simple two- nouns and adjectives often need additional function- argument we can have the child caused the indicating coding when they occur as predicates, N namely a copular verb (cf. Halim works vs. *Halim a accidentN, but not *smokeV causes illA. (Here and in the V following, the subscripts N, V and A indicate nouns, workerN (impossible), *Halim hard-workingA (impos- verbs and adjectives.) Verbs like smoke and adjectives sible); here the is is required). like ill need additional function-indicating coding to occur in argument function (smok-ing causes ill-ness). Because reference is primarily achieved with nouns, it is nouns that can serve as antecedents for pronouns 4.3 AdjectiŠes (compare Albania’s destruction of itself vs. *the In a fair number of languages, adjectives have affixes Albanian destruction of itself (impossible)). Finally, indicating comparison (comparative degree, superla- nouns are often divided into a number of gender tive degree, equative degree), and in a few languages, classes which are manifested in grammatical agree- adjectives are inflected for agreement with the noun ment (see Grammatical Gender). they modify. Some examples follow.

(a) Comparison. Latin audax ‘brave,’ audac-ior 4.2 Verbs ‘braver’ (comparative), audac-issimus In many languages, verbs have affixes indicating tense (present, past, future), aspect (imperfective, perfective, ‘bravest’ (superlative).

16541 Word Classes and Parts of Speech

(b) Comparison. Tagalog (Philippines) mahal But even more strikingly, many languages appear to expensive;’ sing-mahal ‘as expensive as.’ lack adjectives entirely, expressing all property con- cepts by words that look like verbs or like nouns. For (c) Agreement. Hindi acchaa ‘good’ (masculine instance, in Korean, property concepts inflect for tense singular), acchee (masculine plural), and mood like verbs in predication structures, and they require a relative suffix (see RelatiŠe Clauses) acchii (feminine singular\plural). when they modify a noun, again like verbs (cf. (b) (i), (ii)) below. (a) Predication In many languages, adjectives show no inflectional (i) Event properties of their own. Syntactically, a peculiarity of salam-i mek-ess-ta adjectives is that they can typically occur in com-    parative constructions (whether they show affixes person- eat- - marking comparison or not), and they can be com- ‘the person ate’ bined with degree modifiers of various kinds that do not co-occur with verbs and nouns (e.g., Šery hot , too (ii) Property Š A difficultA, cf. *workV ery,*too mistakeN (impossible)). san-i noph-ess-ta Adjectives generally occur as nominal modifiers with- hill- high-- out additional coding (cf. a baldA man), whereas nouns and verbs mostly need additional function-indicating ‘the hill was high’ coding when they occur as modifiers (*a beard man\a Š \ N Š man with a beard,*a sha eN man a man who sha es). (b) Modification (i) Event 5. Difficulties of Classification mek-un salam The general properties of nouns, verbs, and adjectives eat- person that were sketched in Sect. 5 are sufficient to establish these classes without much doubt in a great many ‘a person who ate’ languages. However, again and again linguists report (ii) Property on languages where such a threefold subdivision does not seem appropriate. Particularly problematic are noph-un san adjectives (Sect. 5.1) but languages lacking a noun- high- hill –verb distinction are also claimed to exist (Sect. 5.2), and Sect. 5.3 discusses adverbs, which present dif- ‘a high hill’ ficulties in all languages.

While languages where all property words can be 5.1 The UniŠersality of AdjectiŠes classified as verbs are very common, languages where all property concepts are nouns are less widely In contrast to nouns and verbs, adjectives are some- attested. A language for which such a claim has been times like function words in that they form a rather made is Ecuadorian Quechua: in this language, prop- small, closed class. For instance, Tamil (southern erty concept words can occur in argument position India) and Hausa (northern Nigeria) have only about and take the same inflection as nouns (cf. (a)(i), (ii) a dozen adjectives. Interestingly, in such languages the below), and nouns can occur as modifiers without concepts that are denoted by adjectives in the small additional coding, like property words (cf. (b) (i), (ii) class coincide to a large extent (Dixon 1977): di- below). mension (‘large,’ ‘small,’ ‘long,’ ‘short,’ etc.), age (‘new,’ ‘young,’ ‘old,’ etc.), value (‘good,’ ‘bad’), color (a) argument position (‘black,’ ‘white,’ ‘red,’ etc.). Other concepts for which English has adjectives (e.g., human propensity con- (i) Thing cepts such as ‘happy,’ ‘clever,’ ‘proud,’ ‘jealous,’ and wambra-ta-mi physical property concepts such as ‘soft,’ ‘heavy,’   ‘hot’) are then expressed by verbs or by nouns. For child- - instance, in Tamil, ‘heavy man’ is ganam-ulla manusan, c c c wajta-rka literally ‘weight-having man,’ and in Hausa, ‘intel-    ligent person’ is mutum mai hankali, literally ‘person hit- .3 . having intelligence.’ ‘he hit the child’

16542 Word Classes and Parts of Speech

(ii) Property (a) Predication jatun-ta-mi (i) Thing big-- sa fo` ma’i le fafine wajta-rka  doctor the woman hit-.3. ‘the woman was a doctor’ ‘he hit the big one’ (ii) Event (b) modification sa alu le fafine (i) Thing  go the woman rumi wasi ‘the woman went’ stonehouse (a) Argument ‘stone house’ (i) Thing (ii) Property e lelei le fo` ma’i jatun wambra  good the doctor big child ‘the doctor is good’ ‘big child’ (ii) Event e lelei le alu o le Šasi Thus, it is often said that while nouns and verbs are  good the go of the bus virtually universal, adjectives are often lacking in languages. However, it is generally possible to find i Apia features that differentiate a property subclass within to Apia the larger class to which property words are assigned. For instance, Korean property verbs do not take the ‘it’s good that the bus goes to Apia’ present-tense suffix -nun, and Ecuadorian Quechua thing words do not combine with the manner adverb suffix -ta (e.g., sumaj-ta ‘beautifully,’ but not *duktur- ta ‘in a doctor’s manner’). Here the subclass problem Clearly, the similarity of thing-words and event- arises: on what grounds do we say that Korean has words in such languages is quite striking and differs two classes of verbs (non-property verbs vs. property dramatically from what is found in the better-known verbs), rather than two word classes (verbs and European languages. But thing-words and event- adjectives)? Since this question is difficult to answer, words do not behave exactly alike in Samoan; the some linguists have claimed that most languages have pattern above is asymmetrical in that fo` ma’i means ‘be adjectives after all, but that adjectives have a strong a doctor’ and ‘person who is a doctor,’ but alu does not tendency to be either verb-like or noun-like (e.g., mean both ‘go’ and ‘person who goes,’ but rather ‘the Wetzer 1996). fact of going.’ Upon closer examination, it has usually turned out that major word classes which can be called nouns and verbs can be distinguished even in the problematic languages. 5.2 The UniŠersality of the Noun–Verb Distinction For a few languages, it has been claimed that there is no (or only a very slight) distinction between nouns 5.3 The Problem of AdŠerbs and verbs, for instance for several North American languages of the Wakashan, Salishan, and Iroquoian Adverbs are the most problematic major word class families, as well as for a number of Polynesian because they are extremely heterogeneous in all lan- languages. For instance, in Samoan (a Polynesian guages, and unlike for nouns, verbs, and adjectives, no language), full words referring to events and things semantic prototype can be identified easily for them show intriguingly similar behavior. Both thing (or (cf. Ramat and Ricca 1994). The most that can be said person) words and event words seem to occur in the in general about adverbs is that they serve to modify same predication structures (a) and in argument non-nominal constituents (verbs or verb phrases, positions (b) below. adjectives, other adverbs, sentences). Perhaps the

16543 Word Classes and Parts of Speech concept of adverb should not be taken too seriously, Toward the end of the twentieth century, linguists because there are very few properties that adverbs of (especially functionalists) became interested in word different kinds share. Five broad subclasses of adverbs classes again. Wierzbicka (1986) proposed a more are often distinguished: setting adverbs (locative: here, sophisticated semantic characterization of the differ- there, below, abroad; temporal: now, then, yesterday, ence between nouns and adjectives (nouns categorize always), manner adverbs (quickly, carefully, beauti- referents as belonging to a kind, adjectives describe fully), degree adverbs (Šery, too, extremely), linking them by naming a property), and Langacker (1987) adverbs (therefore, howeŠer, consequently), and sen- proposed semantic definitions of noun (‘a region in tence adverbs (perhaps, fortunately, frankly) (see Quirk some domain’) and verb (‘a sequentially scanned et al. 1985 for the most comprehensive semantic process’) in his framework of Cognitive Grammar. classification of adverbs). Hopper and Thompson (1984) proposed that the Setting adverbs, degree adverbs, and linking ad- grammatical properties of word classes emerge from verbs are relatively small, closed classes, and they their discourse functions: ‘discourse-manipulable par- often share properties with function words. Sentence ticipants’ are coded as nouns, and ‘reported events’ are adverbs are rare in most languages, and their great coded as verbs. elaboration is probably a peculiarity of the written There is also a lot of interest in the cross-linguistic languages of Europe (Ramat and Ricca 1998). The regularities of word classes, cf. Dixon (1977), Bhat only sizable subclass of adverbs that has equivalents in (1994) and Wetzer (1996) for adjectives, Walter (1981) many languages is the class of manner adverbs. Many and Sasse (1993a) for the noun–verb distinction, languages have a productive way of forming manner Hengeveld (1992b) and Stassen (1997) for non-verbal adverbs from adjectives (e.g., English warm\warmly, predication. Hengeveld (1992a) proposed that major French lent ‘slow,’ lentement ‘slowly’). But this also word classes can either be lacking in a language (then makes manner adverbs problematic as a major word it is called rigid) or a language may not differentiate class, because one could argue that adjective-derived between two word classes (then it is called flexible). manner adverbs are just adjectives which occur with a Thus, ‘languages without adjectives’ (cf. Sect. 6) are special inflectional marker to indicate that they are not either flexible in that they combine nouns and adjec- used in their canonical noun-modifying function. This tives in one class (N\Adj), or rigid in that they lack point of view is non-traditional, but it seems quite adjectives completely. Hengeveld claims that besides reasonable, and it is strengthened by the fact that in the English type, where all four classes (VkNk quite a few languages, adjectives can be used as manner AdjkAdv) are differentiated and exist, there are only adverbs without any special marking. three types of rigid languages (VkNkAdj, e.g., One of the main features that unifies the various Wambon; VkN, e.g., Hausa; and V, e.g., Tuscarora), subclasses of adverbs in languages like English and and three types of flexible languages (VkNkAdj\ French is that four of the five classes contain adjective- Adv, e.g., German; VkN\Adj\Adv, e.g., Quechua; derived words ending in -ly\-ment (only setting ad- V\N\Adj\Adv, e.g., Samoan). verbs are almost never of this type). This is certainly no The most comprehensive theory of word classes and accident, but it should also be noted that this is their properties is presented in Croft (1991). Croft probably a feature typical of European languages that notes that in all the cross-linguistic diversity, one can is hardly found elsewhere. find universals in the form of markedness patterns; universally, object words are unmarked when func- tioning as referring arguments, property words are 6. Theoretical Approaches unmarked when functioning as nominal modifiers, and action words are unmarked when functioning as While the identification and definition of word classes predicates. While it is not possible to define cross- was regarded as an important task of descriptive and linguistically applicable notions of noun, adjective, theoretical linguistics by classical structuralists (e.g., and verb on the basis of semantic and\or formal Bloomfield 1933), Chomskyan generative grammar criteria alone, it is possible, according to Croft, to simply assumed (contrary to fact) that the word classes define nouns, adjectives, and verbs as cross-linguistic of English (in particular the major or ‘lexical’ cate- prototypes on the basis of the universal markedness gories noun, verb, adjective, and adposition) can be patterns. carried over to other languages. Without much ar- For a sample of recent work on word classes in a gument, it has generally been held that they belong to cross-linguistic perspective, see Vogel and Comrie the presumably innate substantive universals of lan- (2000), and the bibliography in Plank (1997). Other guage, and not much was said about them (other than overviews are Sasse (1993b), Schachter (1985), and that they can be decomposed into the two binary further collections of articles are Tersis-Surugue (1984) features [pN] and [pV]: [jN, kV] l noun, and Alpatov (1990). [kN, jV] l verb, [jN, jV] l adjective, [kN, kN] l adposition) (see Linguistics: Theory of Prin- See also: Lexical Processes (Word Knowledge): Psy- ciples and Parameters). chological and Neural Aspects; Speech Production,

16544 Word, Linguistics of

Neural Basis of; Speech Production, Psychology of; Word, Linguistics of Word, Linguistics of; Word Meaning: Psychological Aspects; Word Recognition, Cognitive Psychology of In thinking about words, the first question is how to divide an utterance up into them. The simple answer is that words are demarcated by spaces, just as they are Bibliography on this page. But this simple answer depends on the v \ existence of writing. In speech, we do not normally Alpatov V M (ed.) 1990 Casti reci: Teorija i tipologija. Nauka, leave a space or pause between words. Most languages Moscow Bhat D N S 1994 The AdjectiŠal Category: Criteria for Differ- throughout history have not been written down. entiation and Identification. Benjamins, Amsterdam Surely we do not want to say that only written Bloomfield L 1933 Language. Holt, New York languages have words, and, even with written lan- Croft W 1991 Syntactic Categories and Grammatical Relations: guages, spacing does not provide an entirely satisfying the CognitiŠe Organization of Information. The University of answer. For example, English compounds can be Chicago Press, Chicago spelled in three ways: open, closed, or hyphenated, Dixon R M W 1977 Where have all the adjectives gone? Studies and some items can be spelled in any of these three in Language 1: 19–80 ways without being affected in any detectable way: Hengeveld K 1992a Parts of speech. In: Fortescue M, Harder P, birdhouse, bird-house, bird house. We do not want to Kristoffersen L (eds.) Layered Structure and Reference in a Functional PerspectiŠe. Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp. 29–56 say that the first spelling is one word, the last spelling Hengeveld K 1992b Non-Šerbal Predication: Theory, Typology, two words, and the middle one neither one word nor Diachrony. de Gruyter, Berlin two, which we would have to do if we accepted spaces Hopper P J, Thompson S A 1984 The discourse basis for lexical as criterial. The better conclusion is that spelling con- categories in . Language 60: 703–52 ventions are not a completely reliable clue to whether Langacker R W 1987 Nouns and verbs. Language 63: 53–94 something is a word or not. Plank F 1997 Word classes in typology: recommended readings Some linguists avoid the problem by claiming that (a bibliography). Linguistic Typology 1: 185–92 the whole notion ‘word’ is theoretically invalid, just an Quirk R, Greenbaum S, Leech G, Svartvik J 1985 A Com- artifact of spelling. In their favor is the fact, which prehensiŠe Grammar of the English Language. Longman, London people are always surprised to learn, that not all Ramat P, Ricca D 1994 Prototypical adverbs: On the scalarity\ languages have a word for ‘word.’ The classical radiality of the notion of adverb. RiŠista di Linguistica 6: languages, biblical Hebrew, classical Greek, and clas- 289–326 sical Latin, for example, all have terms that are Ramat P, Ricca D 1998 Sentence adverbs in the languages of systematically ambiguous among ‘speech,’ ‘word,’ and Europe. In: van der Auwera J (ed.) AdŠerbial Constructions in ‘utterance’ but none has terms that distinguish clearly the Languages of Europe. de Gruyter, Berlin, pp. 187–275 among these notions and certainly none has a special Sasse H-J 1993a Das Nomen—eine universale Kategorie? Š term that means just ‘word.’ Even in the opening verse Sprachtypologie und Uni ersalienforschung 46: 187–221 of the Gospel of John, ‘In the beginning was the Sasse H-J 1993b Syntactic categories and subcategories. In: Word,’ it is still not clear just what is the meaning of Jacobs J et al. (eds.) Syntax: An International Handbook λοT γο| of Contemporary Research. de Gruyter, Berlin, Vol. 1, the Greek word [logos] that we conventionally pp. 646–86 translate as ‘word.’ Many scholars believe that the best Schachter P 1985 Parts-of-speech systems. In: Shopen T (ed.) translation is ‘thought’ or ‘reason.’ Language Typology and Syntactic Description. Cambridge The classical languages are not alone. The anthro- University Press, Cambridge, UK, Vol. 1, pp. 3–61 pologist Bronislaw Malinowski declared that the Stassen L 1997 IntransitiŠe Predication. Oxford University Press, distinction between word and utterance was not an Oxford Š obvious one to most peoples, that ‘isolated words are Tersis-Surugue N (ed.) 1984 L’opposition erbo-nominale dans in fact only linguistic figments, the products of an diŠerses langues du monde. (Special issue of ModeZ les linguisti- ques VI.1.), Presses Universitaires de Lille, Lille, France advanced linguistic analysis’ (Malinowski 1935, p. 11), Vogel P M, Comrie B (eds.) 2000 Approaches to the Typology of suggesting that there was no reason for most languages Word Classes.(Empirical Approaches to Language Typology, to distinguish between words and utterances. None- Vol. 23.) de Gruyter, Berlin theless, most modern linguists believe that all lan- Walter H 1981 Studien zur Nomen-Verb-Distinktion aus typo- guages do have words, whether their speakers are logischer Sicht. Fink, Mu$ nchen aware of the units or not. The question then becomes Wetzer H 1996 The Typology of AdjectiŠal Predication.de how to figure out what a word is and how to identify Gruyter, Berlin words in a way that is valid for all languages, written Wierzbicka A 1986. What’s in a noun? (Or: How do nouns differ or spoken, and, since language is first and foremost in meaning from adjectives?) Studies in Language 10: 353–89 Wierzbicka A 2000 Lexical prototypes as a universal basis for spoken, our answer must not depend on writing. cross-linguistic identification of ‘parts of speech’. In: Vogel The earliest explicit discussion that we have of the P M, Comrie B (eds.) Approaches to the Typology of Word notion ‘word’ and of words in the speech stream is in Classes. de Gruyter, Berlin, pp. 285–317 the work of Aristotle. Aristotle made a distinction between an utterance or sentence, for which he used M. Haspelmath the term λοT γο| [logos] (confusingly, the term that we

16545

Copyright # 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences ISBN: 0-08-043076-7