Abstracts Booklet

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Abstracts Booklet The Nineteenth Manchester Phonology Meeting 19 f«Èn•l«dZi ABSTRACTS BOOKLET Thursday 19th - Saturday 21st May 2011 Held at Hulme Hall, Manchester Organised by a collaboration of phonologists at the University of Edinburgh, the University of Manchester, and elsewhere. This booklet contains the abstracts for all the papers presented at the nineteenth Manchester Phonology Meeting, held at Hulme Hall, Manchester, in May 2011. The abstracts are arranged in alphabetical order by the surname of the (first named) presenter. Unfortunately, one presenter did not submit the non-anonymous version of their abstract in time for inclusion in this booklet; otherwise all abstracts should be here. The abstracts for the oral paper sessions are presented first, followed by the abstracts for the poster paper sessions, and the booklet concludes with abstracts for the special session. All sessions for papers listed in this booklet will take place in either the Old Dining Hall, the JCR or the dining hall area in Hulme Hall. The opening and closing addresses and the special session will be held in the Old Dining Hall. The final programme, included in your registration pack and available on the 19mfm website, gives the details of which papers are in which room, and at which times. Oral papers Pretonic unstressed syllables in English Katalin Balogné Bérces PPCU, Piliscsaba, Hungary [email protected] The paper aims to contribute to the study of phonological strength. It is well-known that certain positions (e.g., stressed vowels, word-initial or post-coda consonants) are stronger, that is, more resistant to lenition (weakening, incl. all types of reduction and deletion) than others (e.g., unstressed vowels, word-final or coda consonants) both synchronically and diachronically. As a typical representative of the Germanic pattern, English exhibits a stress- sensitive system in its consonants, i.e., there is a difference between their pre-stress and post- stress behaviour. In particular, consonants immediately preceding full vowels are strong (note the aspirate in atomic and vehicular) but weak otherwise (cf. atom and vehicle). This extends to vowels, as only schwas are subject to deletion (syncope – battery vs. batt'ry). The present paper investigates the strength of pretonic unstressed syllables. First, word-initially their consonant differs from their vowel: in words of the potato type, the consonant of the first syllable is strong while the vowel is weak, even deletable. Therefore, the evaluation of the strength of that syllable as a whole is ambivalent. We take this observation as argument for abandoning the syllable as a theoretical tool in the discussion of phonological strength and as evidence for the need to treat the consonant and the vowel separately, as is done in CVCV phonology (Lowenstamm 1996, Scheer 2004). Second, pretonic unstressed syllables do not exhibit the same degree of phonological strength/weakness in different positions: word-initially their consonants are generally stronger (as in potato, with almost as much aspiration on the /p/ as on the first /t/) than medially (as in Winnepesaukee), while the reverse is true for their vowels (the schwa being more prone to syncopate in potato/suppose than in nationalize). From this, the paper concludes that foot- based adjunction analyses, propagated in Jensen (2000), Davis (2003), etc. are inadequate either because they predict the same amount of aspiration in Winnepesaukee as in potato, or because they allow for a reduced vowel in a monosyllabic foot. Therefore, in the present paper the claim is made that (i) the phonological strength of consonants and vowels should be evaluated separately, a consequence of which is that (ii) stress is a property of vowels (rather than syllables) which may percolate to neighbouring consonants, and that (iii) prominence relations can be reduced to a system of V-to-V and V- to-C interactions. It follows, then, that a theoretical framework based on Ségéral and Scheer's (1999) and Dienes and Szigetvári's (1999) definitions of government and licensing as two antagonistic lateral forces (cf. Scheer 2004, Scheer and Ziková 2010) is a suitable model. With a CVCV skeleton, it is capable of expressing the relative weakness/strength of vocalic and consonantal positions separately. This way, it accounts for the above observations avoiding the debatable notion of the syllable and without making reference to foot structure. Voicing contrast in Hungarian fricatives Zsuzsanna Bárkányi Katalin Mády HAS, Reaserch Institute for Linguistics Institute of Phonetics and Speech Processing, and ELTE, University of Budapest University of Munich bzsu at nytud dot hu mady at phonetik dot uni-muenchen dot de This presentation aims to explore voicing contrast in Hungarian fricatives especially in word- and utterance-final position. Hungarian is not a final-devoicing language, i.e. there are minimal pairs which differ in the voicing quality of the word-final obstruent: mé[z] 'honey' - mé[s] 'lime'. According to the literature this contrast is cued by phonetic voicing. Recent studies (Kiss-Bárkányi 2006, Bárkányi-Kiss 2010) on the double-faced phonological behavior of [v] in Hungarian show that v in utterance-final position is devoiced due to the aerodynamic conflict involved in the realization of voiced fricatives (e.g. Stevens 1998, Johnson 2003, Jansen 2004, Fuchs-Brunner 2005). Based on this we hypothesize that the other voiced fricatives [z] and [ʒ] are also devoiced in final position. The following questions arise: Is there contrast preservation between voiced and voiceless fricatives in this position? If so, which phonetic parameters might encode the difference when phonation is lacking? Even if there are measurable differences between the voiced- voiceless contexts are they robust enough to be perceived by speakers? How much voicing is needed for a fricative to be perceived as voiced? We realized acoustic and perception experiments in order to answer these questions. In Experiments 1 and 2 we examined (with 6 native speakers of Hungarian) the acoustic realization of /s/-/z/ and /ʃ/-/ʒ/, respectively, in utterance-final position. Our results confirm that voiced sibilants are highly unphonated - over 70% in the fricative interval. However, even compeletely unphonated voiced and voiceless sibilants are reliably disinguished by the length of the fricative and the length of the preceding vowel (and their ratio) - in accordance with the literature on the issue (e.g. Wells 2000). (There are other parameters like intensity of the fircative, preaspiration of the preceding vowel, which we did not measure in this study, but seem to contribute to partial contrast preservation. We also observed that there is considerable inter-speaker variation, but the same speaker usually realizes the voiced-voiceless pairs with different phonation.) An important finding is that when our test words appeared in sentence-medial position we did not find any differences in the length of the preceding vowel. This means that in a phonetically impoverished context, when phonation is lost, a "secondary" cue is enhanced to prevent neutralization. So phonetic features that have been thought to be redundant in the phonology of Hungarian are actually crucial for maintaining the voicing contrast in phonetically unfavorable position. Experiment 3 aims to find out whether speakers really perceive voiced fricatives as such in final position. Unphonated stimuli (over 90% of devoicing in both the phonologically voiced and phonologically voiceless case) were chosen from the acoustic studies. Data was presented as a forced choice test to 20 native speakers of Hungarian through stereo headphones with the help of Praat's ExperimentMFC. Our preliminary results show that if the whole final syllable is presented, both voiced voiceless fricatives are recognized correctly most of the time although with voiced fricatives performing significantly worse. When a lone consonant is presented, voiceless fricatives are recognized correctly, while voiced fricatives are recognized in less than 50% of the cases. They are perceived voiceless. This corroborates that the role of the preceding vowel is crucial in this impoverished position and shows that there is only partial contrast preservation. In Experiment 4 stimuli were chosen from Experiment 1 with different degrees of phonation from 0-100% of voicing in 10% steps and were presented as in Experiment 3. Our results so far show that recognition considerably improves if around 30% of the fricative is realized with phonation. Contrary to our expectations, we did not find a statistically significant difference in the voicing of compact and diffuse voiced fricatives in final position. This entails a further question: can we really explain the phonological behaviour of /v/ by its phonetic properties? We will show that while the articulatory target of /v/ is a labiodental narrow approximant (as Bárkányi-Kiss 2010 based on Padgett 2002) claim, the articulatory target of [z] and [ʒ] is an alveolar and post-alveolar voiced fricative, respectively, and this has phonological consequences. Prosodic faithfulness: evidence from poetic meter Lev Blumenfeld, Carleton University 〈[email protected]〉 In this talk I use evidence of prosodic similarity in poetic meter to propose a general correspondence theory for prosodic structure. The notion of contrast in Optimality Theory is connected with correspondence: faithfulness constraints only refer to those aspects of representation which can be used
Recommended publications
  • Phonological Processes
    Phonological Processes Phonological processes are patterns of articulation that are developmentally appropriate in children learning to speak up until the ages listed below. PHONOLOGICAL PROCESS DESCRIPTION AGE ACQUIRED Initial Consonant Deletion Omitting first consonant (hat → at) Consonant Cluster Deletion Omitting both consonants of a consonant cluster (stop → op) 2 yrs. Reduplication Repeating syllables (water → wawa) Final Consonant Deletion Omitting a singleton consonant at the end of a word (nose → no) Unstressed Syllable Deletion Omitting a weak syllable (banana → nana) 3 yrs. Affrication Substituting an affricate for a nonaffricate (sheep → cheep) Stopping /f/ Substituting a stop for /f/ (fish → tish) Assimilation Changing a phoneme so it takes on a characteristic of another sound (bed → beb, yellow → lellow) 3 - 4 yrs. Velar Fronting Substituting a front sound for a back sound (cat → tat, gum → dum) Backing Substituting a back sound for a front sound (tap → cap) 4 - 5 yrs. Deaffrication Substituting an affricate with a continuant or stop (chip → sip) 4 yrs. Consonant Cluster Reduction (without /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (grape → gape) Depalatalization of Final Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the end of a word (dish → dit) 4 - 6 yrs. Stopping of /s/ Substituting a stop sound for /s/ (sap → tap) 3 ½ - 5 yrs. Depalatalization of Initial Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the beginning of a word (shy → ty) Consonant Cluster Reduction (with /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (step → tep) Alveolarization Substituting an alveolar for a nonalveolar sound (chew → too) 5 yrs.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Treatment of Consonant Cluster Reduction
    ON THE TREATMENT OF CONSONANT CLUSTER REDUCTION Young-Seok Kim In this paper we will consider a theory of Consonant Cluster Reduction (CCR) without deletion rules. Through its analysis of CCR and closely related phenome,1a in the phonology of Korean, this paper explains why CCR should be dealt with by a general convention which we shall refer to as Stray Erasure Principle. It will be shown that, though primarily based on the Korean language, this theory makes correct predic­ tions about similar phenomena in other typologically unrelated languages such as Menomini. 1. The Syllable Structure of Korean The phonemic inventory of Standard Korean comprises 9 (or 10) vowels and 21 consonants including glides. 1 At the phonetic level, only a limited number of con­ sonants (i.e. [p=], [t=], [k=], [1], [m], [n] and [IJ] are allowed in the syllable-final position because of Obstruent Neutralization. However, all the consonants can in principle occur both morpheme-initially2 and morpheme-finally provided we admit that there are some accidental gaps for some reason or other. True consonants can also occur before an on-glide y or w. The canonical syllable structure of Korean would then look like this: (1) ~ (C) (G) V (C) Since we do not find evidence supporting the syllable-internal hierarchical struc­ ture, we will take no account of the possibility of assigning G either to the onset (cf. B.-G. Lee 1982) or to the nucleus, which in turn is dominated by an upper node, namely the rime. Instead, we will adopt the basic idea of Clements and Keyser (1983), which is minimally different from the theory developed by Kahn (1976) in that it recognizes an intermediate CV tier between the syllable (0) and the phonemic melody 1 It has been taken for granted by many grammarians that vowel length is distinctive in the Seoul dialect (H.-B.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction: Constraints in Phonological Acquisition
    1 Introduction: constraints in phonological acquisition René Kager, Joe Paten and Wim Zonneveld This volume presents ten studies in phonological first languageacquisition, an area of research that has become one of fast-growing importance in recent years. The reason for this is not just the fruitfulness and linguistic interest of this type of study per se: it is also thecase that the more we come to know about phonological development, by the analysis of growingnumbers of data collections and increasingly sophisticated experiments, themore the field has complied with the notion that acquisition research liesat the heart of the modern study of language. One of the aims of this introductionis to illustrate and discuss these developments. In line with them, thepast decade in phonology in particular has witnessed an upswell of productiveinteraction between empirical acquisition research and theory development. With thearrival and rise of constraint-based models, in particular Prince and Smolensky's(1993) Optimality Theory, phonological theory now providesa framework that meets the desiderata expressed more than two decadesago by Lise Menn (1980: 35-36), who is also a contributor to this volume: () ... The child'slonguetiedness',that overwhelming reality which Stampe and Jakobson both tried to capture with their respective formal structures, could be handled more felicitously if one represented the heavy articulatory limitations of the child by the formal device of output constraints I...1The child's gradual mastery of articulation then is formalized as a relaxation of those constraints. The rapid emergence of acquisition studies within OptimalityTheory reflects the general suitability of constraints for the formalisation ofdevelopmental limitations, as well as the usefulness of constraint ranking for expressingthe relaxation of these limitations.
    [Show full text]
  • Lenition in Persian Phonological System Aliye Kambuziya1* Mahmoud Mobaraki2 1
    Lenition in Persian Phonological System Aliye Kambuziya1* Mahmoud Mobaraki2 1. Associate Professor, Linguistics Department, Faculty of Humanities, Tarbiat Modares University, Iran 2. Ph.D. Student, Linguistics Department, Faculty of Humanities, Tarbiat Modares University, Iran * E-mail of the corresponding author: [email protected] Abstract This study deals with lenition processes according to the theoretical framework of generative phonology to answer the cited questions: How phonological processes are applied in Persian phonological system as lenition? In other words, how do the data support the application of lenition processes in Persian? In which contexts do lenition processes apply in Persian? Synthetic process typology of phonological processes is investigated according to the phonological pattern of Persian; finally the most frequent lenition processes are selected. To see how these processes are applied in Persian as lenition, Standard Persian and six dialects out of twenty five dialects which show these processes are selected. The data are gathered in field study. Then, each of the lenition processes is probed on the Persian varieties to find the alternatives and underlying forms which are important to decide how the lenition processes are applied; and to find the positions in which lenition processes take place. The collected data support the lenition processes in Persian. The data show that the lenition processes tend to occur in postvocalic, intervocalic and final positions; and the final position has the highest frequency for lenition processes to occur. This support Kenstowichz‟s idea that mentions word final is the typical position for lenition. Keywords: lenition processes, generative phonology, synthetic process typology, Persian phonological system 1.Introduction 1.1The Current Approaches to Lenition In traditional approach, the typology of phonological processes is dualistic.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Phonological Processes on the Speech Intelligibility of Young Children
    Portland State University PDXScholar Dissertations and Theses Dissertations and Theses 10-20-1994 The Effects of Phonological Processes on the Speech Intelligibility of Young Children Susanne Shotola-Hardt Portland State University Follow this and additional works at: https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds Part of the Speech and Rhetorical Studies Commons Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Shotola-Hardt, Susanne, "The Effects of Phonological Processes on the Speech Intelligibility of Young Children" (1994). Dissertations and Theses. Paper 4780. https://doi.org/10.15760/etd.6664 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations and Theses by an authorized administrator of PDXScholar. Please contact us if we can make this document more accessible: [email protected]. THESIS APPROVAL The abstract and thesis of Susanne Shotola-Hardt for the Master of Science in Speech Communication: Speech and Hearing Sciences were presented October 20, 1994, and accepted by the thesis committee and the department. COMMITTEE APPROVALS: .. \\ ....___ ____ ; --J6~n McMahon Sheldon Maron Representative, Office of Graduate Studies DEPARTMENT APPROVAL: ""' A * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * ACCEPTED FOR PORTLAND STATE UNIVERSITY BY THE LIBRARY b on £!luve-4* ~ /99</: . ABSTRACT An abstract of the thesis of Susanne Shotola-Hardt for the Master of Science in Speech Communication: Speech and Hearing Sciences presented October 20, 1994. Title: The Effects of Phonological Processes on the Speech Intelligibility of Young Children. The purpose of this study was to explore the relationship between occurrence of 10 phonological processes, singly and in groups, with mean percentage of intelligibility of connected speech samples.
    [Show full text]
  • Sounds Difficult? Why Phonological Theory Needs 'Ease of Articulation'
    SOAS Working Papers in Linguistics Vol. 14 (2006): 207-226 Sounds difficult? Why phonological theory needs ‘ease of articulation’ David Shariatmadari [email protected] Introduction In this paper I will try to show that theories of phonological structure must incorporate some measure of phonetic naturalness, specifically the idea that there exists a tendency to conserve energy in the use of the articulatory organs, with ‘easy’ sounds being those that require less physical effort to produce on the part of the speaker. I will call this the Ease of Articulation Hypothesis (EoA) 1. A strong form of EoA would state that articulatory phonetics is the sole motivating factor for sound patterns including the structure of phonemic inventories, phonotactics and morphophonemic variation. This is clearly false. If it were the case, phonology would be indistinguishable from phonetics. There would be no reason for a given phonological process not to apply in all languages, since all human beings have the same vocal apparatus and what is phonetically natural for a speaker of Turkish must also be phonetically natural for a speaker of Japanese. And yet, there are clearly many phonological differences between these two, and among all other languages. In contrast, a weak version of EoA might hold that articulation is one of a number of pressures competing for influence over the shape of the speech string. Some of the time it will win out. Whether or not it does depends, crucially, on the structure of the language concerned. Since every language has a different structure, the way in which phonetic influence is felt will be different for every language.
    [Show full text]
  • PHONETICS and PHONOLOGY of REGRESSIVE VOICING ASSIMILATION in RUSSIAN NATIVE and NON-NATIVE SPEECH by Natalya Y. Samokhina
    Phonetics and Phonology of Regressive Voicing Assimilation in Russian Native and Non-native Speech Item Type text; Electronic Dissertation Authors Samokhina, Natalya Publisher The University of Arizona. Rights Copyright © is held by the author. Digital access to this material is made possible by the University Libraries, University of Arizona. Further transmission, reproduction or presentation (such as public display or performance) of protected items is prohibited except with permission of the author. Download date 29/09/2021 04:59:37 Link to Item http://hdl.handle.net/10150/194543 PHONETICS AND PHONOLOGY OF REGRESSIVE VOICING ASSIMILATION IN RUSSIAN NATIVE AND NON-NATIVE SPEECH by Natalya Y. Samokhina _____________________ Copyright © Natalya Y. Samokhina 2010 A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of the GRADUATE INTERDISCIPLINARY PROGRAM IN SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND TEACHING In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements For the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY In the Graduate College THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA 2010 2 THE UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA GRADUATE COLLEGE As members of the Dissertation Committee, we certify that we have read the dissertation prepared by Natalya Y. Samokhina entitled Phonetics and Phonology of Regressive Voicing Assimilation in Russian Native and Non-native Speech and recommend that it be accepted as fulfilling the dissertation requirement for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy _______________________________________________________________________ Date: Diane Ohala _______________________________________________________________________
    [Show full text]
  • An Apparent Example from Basque Juliette Blevins
    Unexpected obstruent loss in initial obstruent–sonorant clusters: an apparent example from Basque Juliette Blevins (CUNY) & Ander Egurtzegi (IPS-LMU) To be published as: Blevins & Egurtzegi. 2017. “Unexpected obstruent loss in initial obstruent–sonorant clusters: an apparent example from Basque”, Phonology 34, 507-522. Abstract The apparent loss of initial obstruents in Basque borrowings from Romance (e.g. laru ≪ Lat. claru) is striking. While Proto-Basque is generally reconstructed as lacking initial clusters, the expected repair in loans, based on typology, phonology and phonetics, is copy-vowel epenthesis, not obstruent loss. Indeed, there is evidence for a vowel-copy process in Basque in other loans with obstruent–sonorant clusters (e.g. gurutze ≪ Lat. cruce). We suggest that initial obstruent loss before /l/ but not /r/ is related to Romance developments. In the Romance varieties in contact with Basque, /fl pl bl kl gl/ all show evidence of neutralisation to /ʎ/ word-initially. We hypothesise that obstruent loss in words like Basque laru reflects influence from local Romance languages at a time when Basque lacked /ʎ/. In contrast, vowel copy conforming to Basque syllable structure was the norm in Romance loanwords with clusters not affected by this process. 1. The preference for OR onset clusters The majority of work on tautosyllabic consonant clusters explains a cross- linguistic preference for obstruent-sonorant (OR) onset clusters over other cluster types in terms of the sonority hierarchy (Sievers 1881; Steriade 1982; Clements 1990; Blevins 1995; Berent 2013). Whether the sonority hierarchy adopted is the seemingly universal one shown in (1), or a more language-specific scale like those argued for in Steriade (1982) or Parker (2002), phonologists are largely in agreement on two points.
    [Show full text]
  • EPENTHESIS in CHILDREN's CONSONANT CLUSTER PRODUCTIONS: a PERCEPTUAL and ACOUSTICAL STUDY by MARTA KELCEY EVESON B.Sc, the Unive
    EPENTHESIS IN CHILDREN'S CONSONANT CLUSTER PRODUCTIONS: A PERCEPTUAL AND ACOUSTICAL STUDY \ by MARTA KELCEY EVESON B.Sc, The University of Victoria, 1991 A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE in THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES (School of Audiology and Speech Sciences) We accept this thesis as conforming to the required standard THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA April 1996 © Marta Kelcey Eveson, 1996 In presenting this thesis in partial fulfilment of the requirements for an advanced degree at the University of British Columbia, I agree that the Library shall make it freely available for reference and study. I further agree that permission for extensive copying of this thesis for scholarly purposes may be granted by the head of my department or by his or her representatives. It is understood that copying or publication of this thesis for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. Department of Aud^nq^ qp A ^Sy^fj^V) Soe^ceS The University of British Columbia Vancouver, Canada DE-6 (2/88) ABSTRACT The purpose of the present study was to examine epenthesis in children's consonant cluster productions from phonological and phonetic perspectives. The following questions were investigated: (1) Do consonant clusters produced with an epenthetic vowel differ in duration from those without? (2) Is the epenthetic vowel in the consonant cluster consistent in length and quality, or do co-articulatory effects occur? (3) Is the epenthetic vowel dependent in terms of duration on the phrasal context or the duration of the syllable nucleus? The subjects, S_i (Charles) and S2 (Blair), were two of six subjects in a doctoral research study investigating the application of a nonlinear phonological framework to the assessment and remediation of phonological disorders.
    [Show full text]
  • Final Devoicing Principles and Parameters
    Final Devoicing Principles and Parameters Wiebke Gertrud Brockhaus University College London University of London October 1991 Thesis submitted to the University of London in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy ProQuest Number: 10610943 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a com plete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. uest ProQuest 10610943 Published by ProQuest LLC(2017). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States C ode Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106- 1346 ABSTRACT This thesis addresses the problem of how to deal with the phonological event of final obstruent devoicing (FOD) in a theoretical framework based on principles and parameters rather than rules. The data used comes almost exclusively from German (Hochlautung). The first chapter presents the 'raw facts' of FOD. Its purpose is to provide an outline of the sort of data to be accounted for in the remainder of the thesis. Previous treatments of FOD in German are discussed and evaluated in the second chapter. All of them are shown to be associated with a number of problems, many of which are artifacts of inadequate theoretical frameworks. The third and fourth chapters address the questions of what FOD is and where it occurs.
    [Show full text]
  • Download This PDF File
    Pobrane z czasopisma New Horizons in English Studies http://newhorizons.umcs.pl Data: 29/09/2021 23:18:28 New Horizons in English Studies 2/2017 LANGUAGE • Maciej Tomaka JAGIELLONIAN UNIVERSITY IN KRAKÓW [email protected] On Reduction in English: What the English Don’t Say Abstract. The aim of this article is to examine which sounds are most often omitted in official oral pub- lic performances by native speakers of British English. Such terms as reduction, elision (and its types), and connected speech are explained; the literature cited is concerned with elision of vowels (triphthong smoothing being treated separately), of consonants, and of whole syllables. The study presents the results of an analysis conducted on selected material available on the Internet – this comprises three British English oral performances of the total length of approximately 20 minutes. With regard to the nature of the data, they were divided into scripted, semi-scripted, and unscripted samples. The research has shown that earlier preparation significantly lowers the number of phonetic reduction phenomena. Furthermore, if the speech is given in front of an audience rather than being pre-recorded, the number of reduction phenomena isUMCS higher. Besides, the analysis has shown a few important tendencies in mod- ern British pronunciation, such as omission of /t/, realisation of final /t/ as ʔ[ ], omission of final /t/ in contractions, which therefore are realised as [n] instead of [nt]. Keywords: reduction, elision, connected speech, British English, oral performace 1. Reduction, elision, and clipping In order to specify the topic of the article,1 let us consider two meanings of reduction: reduce (v.) (1) A term used in the phonological classification of vowel sounds, referring to a vowel which can be analysed as a centralized variant of a vowel in a related form.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Development: Does Misperception Play a Role in Children's Misarticulations? Montreal Working Papers in Linguistics, Vol. 1
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 105 740 FL 006 780 AUTHOR Golick, Margie TITLE Phonological Development: Does Misperception Play a Role in Children's Misarticulations? Montreal Working Papers in .Linguistics, Vol. 1. INSTITUTION McGill Univ., Montreal (Quebec).; Montreal Univ. (Quebec).; Quebec Univ., Montreal. PUB DATE Mar 74 NOTE 15p. EDRS PRICE MF-$0.76 HC-$1.58 PLUS POSTAGE DESCRIPTORS *Articulation (Speech); Auditory Perception; Aurally Handicapped; *Child Language; Language Ability; *Languag Development; Perception Tests; Perceptual Development; Perceptually Handicapped; Phonetics; Phonology; Pronunciation; *Psycholinguistics; Retarded Speech Development; Syllables; Vocabulary ABSTRACT Smith (1973), Stampe (1972), and Braine (1973) believe that by the time the child speaks his perception is well-developed, and that any discrepancy between child forms and adult forms are due to organizational and production difficulties. Other linguists believe immature perception determines the form of child speech. This paper suggests that children with articulation and/Or reading and spelling dfficulties have failed to learn the phonological system of English and that in many cases the deviant phonological system reflects delayed or disordered perceptual abilities. Two kinds of tests, the oral verbal intelligence tests and the "phonic" spelling tests, are suggested as ways of gaining insight into the nature of possible misperceptions. It is further suggested that a child's success in verbal development depends on his ability to perceive the discrepancy between his forms and adult forms, and that the role of perceptual constraints should be considered in the investigation of deviant phonological systems. Studies of children with delayed language skills enable linguists to study phenomena that pass by too rapidly to to studied in most children.
    [Show full text]