Neurobiology of Language

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Neurobiology of Language NEUROBIOLOGY OF LANGUAGE Edited by GREGORY HICKOK Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA STEVEN L. SMALL Department of Neurology, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA AMSTERDAM • BOSTON • HEIDELBERG • LONDON NEW YORK • OXFORD • PARIS • SAN DIEGO SAN FRANCISCO • SINGAPORE • SYDNEY • TOKYO Academic Press is an imprint of Elsevier SECTION C BEHAVIORAL FOUNDATIONS This page intentionally left blank CHAPTER 12 Phonology William J. Idsardi1,2 and Philip J. Monahan3,4 1Department of Linguistics, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA; 2Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA; 3Centre for French and Linguistics, University of Toronto Scarborough, Toronto, ON, Canada; 4Department of Linguistics, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada 12.1 INTRODUCTION have very specific knowledge about a word’s form from a single presentation and can recognize and Phonology is typically defined as “the study of repeat such word-forms without much effort, all speech sounds of a language or languages, and the without knowing its meaning. Phonology studies the laws governing them,”1 particularly the laws govern- regularities of form (i.e., “rules without meaning”) ing the composition and combination of speech (Staal, 1990) and the laws of combination for speech sounds in language. This definition reflects a seg- sounds and their sub-parts. mental bias in the historical development of the field Any account needs to address the fact that and we can offer a more general definition: the study speech is produced by one anatomical system (the of the knowledge and representations of the sound mouth) and perceived with another (the auditory system of human languages. From a neurobiological system). Our ability to repeat new word-forms, or cognitive neuroscience perspective, one can such as “glark,” is evidence that people effortlessly consider phonology as the study of the mental model map between these two systems. Moreover, new for human speech. In this brief review, we restrict word-forms can be stored in both short-term and ourselves to spoken language, although analogous long-term memory. As a result, phonology must concerns hold for signed language (Brentari, 2011). confront the conversion of representations (i.e., data Moreover, we limit the discussion to what we con- structures) between three broad neural systems: mem- sider the most important aspects of phonology. ory, action, and perception (the MAP loop; Poeppel & These include: (i) the mappings between three sys- Idsardi, 2011). Each system has further sub-systems tems of representation: action, perception, and long- that we ignore here. The basic proposal is that this term memory; (ii) the fundamental components of is done through the use of phonological primitives speech sounds (i.e., distinctive features); (iii) the laws (features), which are temporally organized (chunked, of combinations of speech sounds, both adjacent and grouped, coordinated) on at least two fundamental long-distance; and (iv) the chunking of speech sounds time scales: the feature or segment and the syllable into larger units, especially syllables. (Poeppel, 2003). To begin, consider the word-form “glark.” Given this string of letters, native speakers of English will have an idea of how to pronounce it and what it 12.2 SPEECH SOUNDS would sound like if another person said it. They would AND THE MAP LOOP have little idea, if any, of what it means.2 The meaning of a word is arbitrary given its form, and it could The alphabet is an incredible human invention, but mean something else entirely. Consequently, we can its ubiquity overly influences our ideas regarding the 1Longman Dictionary of Contemporary English. 2Urban Dictionary (http://www.urbandictionary.com/) states that it means “to slowly grasp the meaning of a word or concept, based on the situation in which it is used” (i.e., almost grokking a concept). Neurobiology of Language. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00012-2 141 © 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 142 12. PHONOLOGY basic units of speech. This continues to this day and is Each of these structures has some degrees of free- evident in the influence of the International Phonetic dom of movement, which we describe in terms of Alphabet (IPA; http://www.langsci.ucl.ac.uk/ipa/) their deflection from a neutral posture for speaking. for transcribing speech. Not all writing systems are The position for the mid-central vowel schwa, [ə], is alphabetic, however. Some languages choose ortho- considered to be the neutral posture of the speech graphic units larger than single sounds (moras, sylla- articulators. In most structures, two opposite directions bles) and a few, such as Bell’s Visible Speech (Bell, of movement are possible, yielding three stable regions 1867) and the Korean orthographic system Hangul of articulation, that is, the tongue dorsum can be put (Kim-Renaud, 1997), decompose sounds into their into a high, mid (neutral), or low position. In the neu- component articulations, all of which constitute impor- tral posture the velum is closed, but it can be opened tant, interconnected representations for speech. to allow air to flow through the nose, and such speech sounds are classified as nasal (as in English “m” [m]). 12.2.1 Action or Articulation of Speech The lips can deflect from the neutral posture by being rounded (as in English “oo” [u]) or drawn back (as in The musculature of the mouth has historically been English “ee” [i]). The tongue tip can be curled conca- somewhat more accessible to investigation than audi- vely or convexly either along its length (yielding retro- tion or memory, and linguistic phonetics has often dis- flex and laminal sounds, respectively) or across its played a bias toward classifying speech sounds in terms width (yielding grooved and lateral sounds, respec- of the actions needed to produce them (i.e., the articula- tively). The tongue dorsum (as mentioned) can be tion of the speech sounds by the mouth). For example, moved vertically (high or low) and horizontally (front the standard IPA charts for consonants and vowels or back), and the tongue root can be moved horizon- (Figure 12.1) are organized by how speech sounds are tally (advanced or retracted). articulated. The columns in Figure 12.1A arrange conso- The larynx (Figure 12.3) is particularly complex and nants with respect to where they are articulated in the can be moved along three different dimensions: modi- mouth (note: right to left corresponds to anterior to fying its vertical position (raised or lowered), modify- posterior position within the oral cavity), and the rows ing its tilt (rotated forward to slacken the vocal folds correspond to how they are articulated (i.e., their or rotated backwards to stiffen them), and changing manner of articulation). The horizontal dimension in the degree of separation of the vocal folds (adducted Figure 12.1B represents the relative frontness-backness or abducted). Furthermore, the lips and the tongue of the tongue, and the vertical dimension represents the blade and dorsum can close off the mouth to dif- aperture of the mouth during production. These are the ferent degrees (termed the “manner” of production): standard methods for organizing consonant and vowel completely closed (stops), nearly closed with turbulent inventories in languages. airflow (fricatives), or substantially open (approxi- Within the oral cavity, there are several controllable mants). Taken together, these articulatory maneuvers structures used to produce speech sounds. These include describe how to make various speech sounds. For the larynx, the velum, the tongue (which is further example, an English [s], as in “sea”, is an abducted divided into three relatively independently moveable (voiceless, high glottal airflow) grooved fricative. sections: the tongue blade, the tongue dorsum, and the Furthermore, as described in Section 12.3, the antago- tongue root), and the lips (see Figure 12.2 reproduced nistic relationships between articulator movements from Bell, 1867; for more detail see Zemlin, 1998). serve as the basis for the featural distinctions (whether (A) (B) Place of articulation Front Central Back Labio- Alveo- Bilabial Interdental Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal dental palatal Close Stop Fricative Close-mid Affricate Nasal Lateral Open-mid Manner of articulation Retroflex Glide Open FIGURE 12.1 IPA charts for American English (A) consonants and (B) vowels. C. BEHAVIORAL FOUNDATIONS 12.2 SPEECH SOUNDS AND THE MAP LOOP 143 monovalent, equipollent, or binary; see Fant, 1973; 12.2.2 Perception or Audition of Speech Trubetzkoy, 1969) that have proven so powerful in understanding not only the composition of speech A great deal of the literature regarding speech per- sounds but also the phonology of human language. ception deals with how “special” speech is (Liberman, 1996) or is not. Often, this is cast as a debate between the motor theory of speech perception (Liberman & Mattingly, 1985) and speech as an area of expertise 4 within general auditory perception (Carbonnell & Lotto, 2014). The motor theory of speech perception 7 3 6 posits speech-specific mechanisms that recover the 5 7 intended articulatory gestures that produced the phys- 2 8 ical auditory stimulus. General auditory perception models, however, posit that the primary representa- tional modality of speech perception is auditory and the mechanisms used during speech perception are 1 the same as those responsible for nonspeech auditory perception. This dichotomy, in some ways, parallels debates about face perception (Rhodes, Calder, Johnson, & Haxby, 2011). Since the development of the 1. The larynx 5. The back of the tongue sound spectrograph (Potter, Kopp, & Green, 1947)and 2. The pharynx 6. The front of the tongue the Haskins pattern playback machine (Cooper, 3. The soft palate 7. The point of the tongue Liberman, & Borst, 1951), it has been known that it is 4. The action of the soft palate in 8. The lips closing the nasal passage technologically feasible to analyze and accurately reproduce speech with timeÀfrequencyÀamplitude FIGURE 12.2 Speech articulators.
Recommended publications
  • The Even Language: Studying, Teaching and Linguistic Ecology Challenges
    Journal of Siberian Federal University. Humanities & Social Sciences 2021 14(6): 822–833 DOI: 10.17516/1997–1370–0763 УДК 811.51 The Even Language: Studying, Teaching and Linguistic Ecology Challenges Alexander A. Petrova, с and Veronica A. Razumovskayab aHerzen State Pedagogical University of Russia St. Petersburg, Russian Federation bSiberian Federal University Krasnoyarsk, Russian Federation сM.K. Ammosov North- Eastern Federal University Yakutsk, Russian Federation Received 05.03.2021, received in revised form 21.04.2021, accepted 21.05.2021 Abstract. The article is devoted to the history of the study and teaching of the Even (Lamut) language in Russia in the period from the 17th – to the beginning of the 21st century. Special attention is paid to the problem of linguistic ecology. The fundamental and applied researches in the synchronic and diachronic aspects, as well as issues of teaching the Even language in families, pre-school educational institutions, colleges and higher educational institutions of Russia are studied. Ethnolinguistics is considered as a possible tool of language preservation and development, i. e., the Even language teaching in the close connection with traditional and modern material and spiritual culture. In this way, the role of vocabulary reflecting the song and dance creative activity, decorative and applied art, ceremonies (of life cycle: birth, wedding, funeral) and economy (hunting, fishing, reindeer breeding), taboo and euphemisms, folk knowledge (metrology, meteorology, space orientation, medicine, calendar, food and cuisine, etc.), as well as the words of religious beliefs (animism, shamanism, Christianity) in the formation of a language picture of the world and the Evens’ mentality is being determined.
    [Show full text]
  • Resource Manual EL Education Language Arts Curriculum
    Language Arts Grades K-2: Reading Foundations Skills Block Resource Manual EL Education Language Arts Curriculum K-2 Reading Foundations Skills Block: Resource Manual EL Education Language Arts Curriculum is published by: EL Education 247 W. 35th Street, 8th Floor New York, NY 10001 www.ELeducation.org ISBN 978-1683622710 FIRST EDITION © 2016 EL Education Inc. Except where otherwise noted, EL Education’s Language Arts Curriculum is published under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) License. To view a copy of this license, visit https://creativecommons.org/ licenses/by/4.0/. Licensed third party content noted as such in this curriculum is the property of the respective copyright owner and not subject to the CC BY 4.0 License. Responsibility for securing any necessary permissions as to such third party content rests with parties desiring to use such content. For example, certain third party content may not be reproduced or distributed (outside the scope of fair use) without additional permissions from the content owner and it is the responsibility of the person seeking to reproduce or distribute this curriculum to either secure those permissions or remove the applicable content before reproduction or distribution. Common Core State Standards © Copyright 2010. National Governors Association Center for Best Practices and Council of Chief State School Officers. All rights reserved. Common Core State Standards are subject to the public license located at http://www.corestandards.org/public-license/. Cover art from “First Come the Eggs,” a project by third grade students at Genesee Community Charter School. Used courtesy of Genesee Community Charter School, Rochester, NY.
    [Show full text]
  • Response Generalization in Individual Participants Receiving Constraint-Induced Aphasia Therapy
    THE EFFECT OF CONSTRAINT-INDUCED APHASIA THERAPY ON NAMING AND DISCOURSE IN INDIVIDUALS WITH APHASIA by JESSICA DAWN RICHARDSON (Under the Direction of Anne Bothe and Rebecca Shisler Marshall) ABSTRACT Participation in aphasia therapy generally results in positive outcomes. Constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) researchers in particular make bold claims about the efficacy of the approach, but pervasive methodological problems throughout the literature detract from the impact of those claims. The study reported in this dissertation was designed to determine the effect of CIAT on standardized measures of language ability, functional communication, and quality of life. In addition, continuous assessment of dependent variables occurred to ensure that improvements in naming and discourse behaviors could be attributed to CIAT and not to other extraneous factors. Six adults with aphasia participated in this modified single-subject, multiple- baseline across individuals design consisting of a baseline, treatment, and maintenance phase. Results provide the new information that the CIAT protocol utilized in this study resulted in a reduction in activity and participation limitations. Furthermore, this study demonstrated the effect of CIAT on naming of trained items and on untrained discourse tasks though the stability criteria used in this study did not prevent the occurrence of accelerating trends in baseline data and therefore reduces the impact of these claims. Results also supply needed information about treatment elements and preliminary
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the Influence of Cognitive Factors on Category-Specific Phonology
    The Influence of Cognitive Factors on Category-Specific Phonology Jonathan Manker University of California, Berkeley A growing body of literature has documented an array of phonological patterns--- either static inventories or active rules--- which are sensitive to the grammatical category of the words they govern. Here I consider grammatical categories as including both syntactic classes such as nouns, verbs, and adjectives, and broad classes such as functional and content morphemes. A parallel body of literature has shown psycholinguistic and neurological differences in how different grammatical categories are treated. These independent observations in both the phonological and cognitive literature suggest a currently unexplored question: Are the observed category-specific phonological asymmetries the reflection of asymmetries in the mental organization or processing of these categories? A review of the literature reveals little solid evidence that there are either innate biases in category-specific phonology or that categories form neighborhoods that facilitate the spread of phonological patterns in the lexicon, when other possible factors, such as syntax and semantics, are considered. However, observations from the dual-stream model of speech perception suggest there may be emergent differences in how more predictable, functional categories are processed as opposed to less predictable, content categories. 1. Introduction In recent decades, linguists have begun to explore how many aspects of grammar interact with phonological patterns in language. In particular, studies coming from a wide range of approaches have documented and analyzed the effect of grammatical categories on sound patterns. Here I will use the term grammatical categories liberally to include classes based on syntactic function (nouns, verbs, adpositions, determiners, etc.), and the broader classes including content (nouns, verbs, adjectives) and functional morphemes (determiners, prepositions, inflectional affixes, etc.).
    [Show full text]
  • Hand-To-Hand Combat, Or Mouth-To-Mouth Resuscitation?
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2003) 26, 199–260 Printed in the United States of America From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness Michael C. Corballis Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Auckland, New Zealand. [email protected] Abstract: The strong predominance of right-handedness appears to be a uniquely human characteristic, whereas the left-cerebral dom- inance for vocalization occurs in many species, including frogs, birds, and mammals. Right-handedness may have arisen because of an association between manual gestures and vocalization in the evolution of language. I argue that language evolved from manual gestures, gradually incorporating vocal elements. The transition may be traced through changes in the function of Broca’s area. Its homologue in monkeys has nothing to do with vocal control, but contains the so-called “mirror neurons,” the code for both the production of manual reaching movements and the perception of the same movements performed by others. This system is bilateral in monkeys, but pre- dominantly left-hemispheric in humans, and in humans is involved with vocalization as well as manual actions. There is evidence that Broca’s area is enlarged on the left side in Homo habilis, suggesting that a link between gesture and vocalization may go back at least two million years, although other evidence suggests that speech may not have become fully autonomous until Homo sapiens appeared some 170,000 years ago, or perhaps even later. The removal of manual gesture as a necessary component of language may explain the rapid advance of technology, allowing late migrations of Homo sapiens from Africa to replace all other hominids in other parts of the world, including the Neanderthals in Europe and Homo erectus in Asia.
    [Show full text]
  • Vowel Harmony in Two Even Dialects Dialects
    400 220 Natalia Aralova Natalia Aralova Natalia Aralova Vowel harmony in two Even Vowel harmony in two Even dialects dialects Production and perception Production and perception This dissertation analyzes vowel systems in two dialects of Even, an endangered Northern Tungusic language spoken in Eastern Siberia. The data were collected during fieldwork in the Bystraia district of Central Kamchatka and in the village of Sebian-Küöl in Yakutia. The focus of the study is the Even system of vowel harmony, which in previous literature has been assumed to be robust. The central question concerns the number of vowel oppositions and the nature of the feature underlying the opposition between harmonic sets. The results of an acoustic study show a consistent pattern for only one acoustic parameter, namely F1, which can harmony in two Even dialects Vowel be phonologically interpreted as a feature [±height]. This acoustic study is supplemented by perception experiments. The results of the latter suggest that perceptually there is no harmonic opposition for high vowels, i.e., the harmonic pairs of high vowels have merged. Moreover, in the dialect of the Bystraia district certain consonants function as perceptual cues for the harmonic set of a word. In other words, the Bystraia Even harmony system, which was previously based on vowels, is being transformed into new oppositions among consonants. ISBN 978-94-6093-180-2 Vowel harmony in two Even dialects: Production and perception Published by LOT phone: +31 30 253 6111 Trans 10 3512 JK Utrecht e-mail: [email protected] The Netherlands http://www.lotschool.nl Cover illustration: Even reindeer herder Anatoly Afanasyevich Solodikov, Central Kamchatka.
    [Show full text]
  • The Ethno-Linguistic Situation in the Krasnoyarsk Territory at the Beginning of the Third Millennium
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Siberian Federal University Digital Repository Journal of Siberian Federal University. Humanities & Social Sciences 7 (2011 4) 919-929 ~ ~ ~ УДК 81-114.2 The Ethno-Linguistic Situation in the Krasnoyarsk Territory at the Beginning of the Third Millennium Olga V. Felde* Siberian Federal University 79 Svobodny, Krasnoyarsk, 660041 Russia 1 Received 4.07.2011, received in revised form 11.07.2011, accepted 18.07.2011 This article presents the up-to-date view of ethno-linguistic situation in polylanguage and polycultural the Krasnoyarsk Territory. The functional typology of languages of this Siberian region has been given; historical and proper linguistic causes of disequilibrum of linguistic situation have been developed; the objects for further study of this problem have been specified. Keywords: majority language, minority languages, native languages, languages of ethnic groups, diaspora languages, communicative power of the languages. Point Krasnoyarsk Territory which area (2339,7 thousand The study of ethno-linguistic situation in square kilometres) could cover the third part of different parts of the world, including Russian Australian continent. Sociolinguistic examination Federation holds a prominent place in the range of of the Krasnoyarsk Territory is important for the problems of present sociolinguistics. This field of solution of a number of the following theoretical scientific knowledge is represented by the works and practical objectives: for revelation of the of such famous scholars as V.M. Alpatov (1999), characteristics of communicative space of the A.A. Burikin (2004), T.G. Borgoyakova (2002), country and its separate regions, for monitoring V.V.
    [Show full text]
  • Download the Devils Horn Free Ebook
    THE DEVILS HORN DOWNLOAD FREE BOOK Michael Segell | 336 pages | 18 Sep 2006 | Picador USA | 9780312425579 | English | New York, NY, United States The Devil's Horn: The Story of the Saxophone, from Noisy Novelty to King of Cool Fall TV Frank Zappa can be seen jokingly making the gesture in the film Baby Snakes in response to the audience, commenting, "That's right, spindle twice. Many of its members in the lower ranks are simply not aware of the true purpose of the organization. Lovecraft Country: Season 1. Starting in earlyCoven concerts always began and ended with Dawson giving the sign on The Devils Horn. Add Article. The Devils Horn 14, Brian rated it it was amazing. It was a symbol that I thought was reflective of what that band was supposed to be all about. Larry Weinstein's tribute to the saxophone doesn't know when to stop wailing, but there's enough fascinating material here to appeal to the ordinary viewer, not just woodwind aficionados. August Learn how and when to remove this template message. For the most part I The Devils Horn that, so there's the good. Back to School Picks. And perhaps it is this split personality-- jazz and legit, brass and woodwind, sweet and devilish-- that appeals to me, a gemini in many senses, in The Devils Horn history of this magnificent instrument. Error rating book. Learn how and when to remove these template messages. Beginning The Devils Horn the early s, the horns were known as the "P-Funk sign" to fans of Parliament-Funkadelic.
    [Show full text]
  • About Mentoring
    ALL ABOUT MENTORINGA PUBLICATION OF SUNY EMPIRE STATE COLLEGE Issue 41 • Summer 2012 ALL ABOUT MENTORING ABOUT ALL Issue 41 • Summer 2012 1 Union Ave. Saratoga Springs, NY 12866-4391 518-587-2100 www.esc.edu Printed by SUNY Empire State College Print Shop ALL ABOUT MENTORING ISSUE 41 SUMMER 2012 Alan Mandell College Professor of Adult Learning and Mentoring Editor Karen LaBarge Faculty Development Projects Coordinator Associate Editor Gael Fischer Designer Debra Park Phyllis M. Cunningham: 1927 - 2012 Copy Editor P H O T O G R A P H Y “Education is not simply about attaining knowledge, Photos courtesy of Stock Studios, education is about the politics of knowledge. and faculty and staff of SUNY Empire State College, Education is not about the preservation of status unless otherwise noted. and elitism; education is about democratization of Cover photo by Suzanne Orrell, “Palindrome” (detail). power relationships.” P R O D U C T I O N Kirk Starczewski Phyllis M. Cunningham, Director of Publications “Let’s Get Real: A Critical Look at the Ron Kosiba Print Shop Supervisor Practice of Adult Education,” Janet Jones Journal of Adult Education, 22(1), Keyboard Specialist College Print Shop fall 1993, pp. 3 - 15 Send comments, articles or news to: All About Mentoring c/o Alan Mandell SUNY Empire State College 325 Hudson St., 5th Floor New York, NY 10013-1005 646-230-1255 [email protected] 1 Table of Contents Editorial – Against Teaching . 2 The Obama Administration as History: First Waves of Interpretation Alan Mandell A Sabbatical Report . 77 Wayne Willis, Genesee Valley Center Rewards and Challenges of Community Engagement Through Service Learning .
    [Show full text]
  • The Post-Syntactic Morphology of the Albanian Pre-Posed Article: Evidence for Distributed Morphology
    The Post-Syntactic Morphology of the Albanian Pre-Posed Article: Evidence for Distributed Morphology Jochen Trommer Institut f¨urSemantische Informationsverarbeitung Universit¨atOsnabr¨uck D-49069 Osnabr¨uck Abstract The so-called "preposed article" (PA) is a functional morpheme that plays a central role in the morphosyntax of Albanian DPs but is poorly understood theoretically. In this paper I present an analysis of PAs morphology along the lines of Distributed Morphology (DM; Halle and Marantz, 1993). PA is taken to be a post-syntactically inserted agreement morpheme. It is argued that a correct account of the in- sertion contexts of PA and of its allomorphy presupposes reference to surface syntactic configurations thus supporting the conceptual frame- work of DM. The claim that PA is in certain instances a constitutive element of word formation (cf. e.g. Buchholz and Fiedler, 1987) is rejected. 1 Some Basic Empirical Facts 1.1 Occurrence The Albanian Preposed Article (PA) mainly occurs in two contexts1. It marks a dative DP as possessor of a complex DP and it occurs with most adjectives used attributively or predicatively: 1Besides other noun-modifying functional words (possessive pronouns, ordinal numbers, etc.) it also occurs with certain relative pronouns and nouns. For a more complete overview see (Buchholz and Fiedler, 1987, pg. 199) 1 (1) a. klas-a e kapitalist¨e-ve class-def PA capitalists-dat `The class of capitalists' b. vajz-a e bukur girl-def PA nice `the nice girl' c. vajz-a ¨esht¨e e bukur girl-def is PA nice `the girl is nice' While it functions as a grammatical marker in its first use, since it occurs with possessors perfectly regularly , and constitutes the only overt difference between dative verb complements and possessors, (2) a.
    [Show full text]
  • A Longitudinal Study on the Role of Lexical Stress and Motivation in the Perception and Production of L2 Spanish Stop Consonants
    A LONGITUDINAL STUDY ON THE ROLE OF LEXICAL STRESS AND MOTIVATION IN THE PERCEPTION AND PRODUCTION OF L2 SPANISH STOP CONSONANTS A Dissertation submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences of Georgetown University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Spanish By Charles Nagle, M.S. Washington, DC October 31, 2014 Copyright 2014 by Charles Nagle All Rights Reserved ii A LONGITUDINAL STUDY ON THE ROLE OF LEXICAL STRESS AND MOTIVATION IN THE PERCEPTION AND PRODUCTION OF L2 SPANISH STOP CONSONANTS Charles Nagle, M.S. Thesis Advisor: Cristina Sanz, Ph.D. & Alfonso Morales-Front, Ph.D. ABSTRACT This study investigated the perception and production of two L2 stop consonants, examining the importance of lexical stress and motivation to learners’ ability to discriminate and produce Spanish /b/ and /p/. Longitudinal data was collected from 26 English-speaking adults enrolled in a second-semester Spanish course at the time of recruitment. On five occasions over a year-long period spanning nearly three semesters of Spanish instruction, participants completed a discrimination task and two production tasks: a sentence formation task and a sentence reading task. Once per semester, they also completed a language contact questionnaire, a quantitative motivation questionnaire operationalizing aspects of the L2 Motivational Self System (Dörnyei, 2009), and a qualitative questionnaire eliciting information on their language learning goals and beliefs. For each of four contrasts crossing stress and position in the word, I calculated d’ as a measure of participants’ ability to discriminate the stops (Macmillan & Creelman, 2005). On the production data, I took VOT measurements from word-initial targets and C:V intensity ratio measurements, which index degree of lenition (Hualde, Simonet, & Nadeu, 2011), from word- medial targets using Praat software (Boersma & Weenik, 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Phonetics and Phonology Seminar Introduction to Linguistics, Andrew
    Phonetics and Phonology Phonetics and Phonology Voicing: In voiced sounds, the vocal cords (=vocal folds, Stimmbände) are pulled together Seminar Introduction to Linguistics, Andrew McIntyre and vibrate, unlike in voiceless sounds. Compare zoo/sue, ban/pan. Tests for voicing: 1 Phonetics vs. phonology Put hand on larynx. You feel more vibrations with voiced consonants. Phonetics deals with three main areas: Say [fvfvfv] continuously with ears blocked. [v] echoes inside your head, unlike [f]. Articulatory phonetics: speech organs & how they move to produce particular sounds. Acoustic phonetics: what happens in the air between speaker & hearer; measurable 4.2 Description of English consonants (organised by manners of articulation) using devices such as a sonograph, which analyses frequencies. The accompanying handout gives indications of the positions of the speech organs Auditory phonetics: how sounds are perceived by the ear, how the brain interprets the referred to below, and the IPA description of all sounds in English and other languages. information coming from the ear. Phonology: study of how particular sounds are used (in particular languages, in languages 4.2.1 Plosives generally) to distinguish between words. Study of how sounds form systems in (particular) Plosive (Verschlusslaut): complete closure somewhere in vocal tract, then air released. languages. Examples of phonological observations: (2) Bilabial (both lips are the active articulators): [p,b] in pie, bye The underlined sound sequence in German Strumpf can occur in the middle of words (3) Alveolar (passive articulator is the alveolar ridge (=gum ridge)): [t,d] in to, do in English (ashtray) but not at the beginning or end. (4) Velar (back of tongue approaches soft palate (velum)): [k,g] in cat, go In pan and span the p-sound is pronounced slightly differently.
    [Show full text]