Principles and Parameters Set out from Europe
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Principles and Parameters Set Out from Europe Mark Baker MIT Linguistics 50th Anniversary, 9 December 2011 1. The Opportunity Afforded (1980-1995) The conception of universal principles plus finite discrete parameters of variation offered: The hope and challenge of simultaneously doing justice to both the similarities and the differences among languages. The discovery and expectation of patterns in crosslinguistic variation. These were first presented with respect to “medium- sized” differences in European languages: The subjacency parameter (Rizzi, 1982) The pro-drop parameter (Chomsky, 1981; Kayne, 1984; Rizzi, 1982) They were then perhaps extended to the largest differences among languages around the world: The configurationality parameter(s) (Hale, 1983) “The more languages differ, the more they are the same” Example 1: Mohawk (Baker, 1988, 1991, 1996) Mohawk seems nonconfigurational, with no “syntactic” evidence of a VP containing the object and not the subject: (1) a. Sak wa-ha-hninu-’ ne ka-nakt-a’. Sak FACT-3mS-buy-PUNC NE 3n-bed-NSF b. Sak kanakta wahahninu’ c. Kanakta’ wahahninu’ ne Sak d. Kanakta’ Sak wahahninu’ e. Wahahninu’ ne Sak ne kanakta’ f. Wahahninu’ ne kanakta’ ne Sak g. Wahahninu’ ne kanakta’ h. Kanakta’ wahahninu’ i. Sak wahahninu’ j. Wahahninu’ ne Sak k. Wahahninu. All: ‘Sak/he bought a bed/it.’ There are also no differences between subject and object in binding (Condition C, neither c- commands the other) or wh-extraction (both are islands, no “subject condition”) (Baker 1992) Mohawk is polysynthetic (agreement, noun incorporation, applicative, causative, directionals…): (2) a. Sak wa-ha-nakt-a-hninu-’ Sak FACT-3mS-bed-Ø-buy-PUNC ‘Sak bought the bed.’ 1 b. Wa-sh-ako-t-ya’t-awi-tsher-ahetkv-ht-v-’. FACT-MsS-FsO-SRFL-body-wrap-NOM-be.ugly-CAUS-BEN-PUNC ‘He made the thing you wrap around your body (a dress, a shirt) ugly for her.’ But the polysynthesis reveals the fundamental subject-object asymmetry in a new way: (3) a. O-wir-a’a wa-hra-k-e’ ne o-’wahr-u. N-baby-NSF FACT-MsS-eat-PUNC NE N-meat-NSF ‘The baby ate the meat.’ b. O-wir-a’a wa-ha-‘wahr-a-k-e’. N-baby-NSF FACT-MsS-meat-Ø-eat-PUNC ‘The baby ate the meat.’ c. *Wa-ka-wir-a-k-e’ ne o-’wahr-u. FACT-NsS-baby-Ø-eat-PUNC NE N-meat-NSF ‘The baby ate the meat.’ Compare English phrase structure; also compounds (meat-eating, #baby-eating). Therefore there are many of Principles of Universal Grammar: The theta-criterion, projection principle The Uniformity of Theta-role Assignment Hypothesis The laws of movement: upward, structure preserving, local; compare VT and TC Plus a small number of strategically-placed parameters: Head movement can apply to NV (Baker 1988, 1996) Also whatever induces nonconfigurationality given super-rich agreement (Baker 1996) Example 2: Kayardild Evans and Levinson (2009) say that maybe no believer in UG would imagine that tense marking (etc.) would spread to elements of the clause other than the verb. A difference, a parameter. (4) Nyingka kurri-nang.ku niwan-ju balmbi-wu. 2sg.NOM see-NEG-FUT 3sg-FUT morrow-FUT ‘You will not see her tomorrow.’ But Evans (1995) observes that this marking spreads onto objects but not subjects, revealing a VP in this otherwise nonconfigurational-seeming language. A similarity, underlying principles. “The more languages differ, the more they are the same” 2 2. The Promise Continues: A Current, Quotidian Example. Chomsky (2000, 2001): Case and agreement are two sides of the same coin, reflexes of Agree Nominative case and subject agreement result from finite T in Agree with the closest NP. Accusative case and object agreement result from active v in Agree with the closest NP. (One of) My current fascinations: Is this relationship between case and agreement universal or parameterized? Is there empirical support for the close connection between accusative case and object agreement in languages where both are overt, as there is for nominative case and subject agreement in English, Icelandic, Hindi, etc.? Joining the story already in progress: Baker and Vinokurova (2010) on Sakha (Turkic) Nominative case and agreement on T are closely related o If the subject is dative, T agrees with the nominative object, or is default. o If the verb is nonfinite (T doesn’t agree), the subject must agree with D and be genitive (in relative clauses, noun complements), or it must be null (PROarb). o Only one verb agrees with the nominative subject in constructions with auxiliary verbs as well as main verbs. Like familiar languages of Greater Europe (roughly) – Principles Accusative case is not related to agreement with an active v o There is no overt object agreement anywhere in the language. o Passive verbs can have accusative themes, if an implicit agent is present. [Cup-ACC intentionally break-PASS-3sS.] o Agentive nominalizations have accusative themes [company-ACC manage-AG.NOM] ‘The manager of the company’ o “Raising to object” with unaccusative matrix verbs [Keskil [Aisen-ACC come-NEG-AOR that ] become.sad-PAST.3sS] ‘Keskil became sad that/because Aisen is not coming’ o Accusative on the objects of certain Ps iff the verb has a thematic subject. Unlike familiar languages of Greater Europe – a parameter? (5) If there are two distinct argumental NPs in the same phase such that NP1 c-commands NP2, then value the case feature of NP2 as accusative unless NP1 has already been marked for case. (Developed from Marantz 1991) Amharic: a language with both overt accusative case marking and overt object agreement (6) Ləmma wɨʃʃa -w-ɨn j-aj-əw-al. Lemma dog-DEF-ACC 3mS-see-3mO-Aux(3mS) ‘Lemma sees the dog.’ (Amberber, 2005:299; Kramer, 2010:1; Leslau, 1995:186) But the two do not pattern together closely in general. 3 NPs with accusative case but that cannot trigger object agreement: (7) a. Ləmma wɨʃʃa-w-ɨn j-aj-al. (Definite objects, as an option) Lemma dog-DEF-ACC 3mS-see-AUX(3mS) ‘Lemma sees the dog.’ b. Mann-ɨn ajj-ɨʃ? (??ajj-ɨʃ-əw) (Indefinite and quantified NPs) Who-ACC see-2fS see-2fS-3mO ‘Who did you (feminine) see?’ (also WL:69) c. Ləmma səw-u-n hullu gabbəz-ə. (*gabbəz-ə-w, *gabbəz-atʃəw) Lemma person-DEF-ACC every invite-3mS invite-3mS-3mO -3pO ‘Lemma invived everyone.’ (also WL:151, Amberber 2005:300) d. Ləmma Aster- ɨn hɪs’an-u-n asaj-at. (#asaj-ə-w) Lemma Aster-ACC child-DEF-ACC show-(3mS)-3fO (*show-3mS-3mO) ‘Lemma showed Aster the baby.’ (also WL:185, 191) (Second objects of DOCs) NPs that trigger object agreement but do not bear accusative case: (8) a. Ləmma l-almaz məʦ’əhaf-u-n sət’t’-at. Dative objects of DOCs Lemma DAT-almaz book-DEF-ACC give-(3mS)-3fO ‘Lemma gave the book to Almaz.’ b. Aster wɨʃʃa all-at Nominative experiencers/ Aster dog exist-(3mS)-3fO possessors ‘Aster has a dog.’ c. Aster bə-mət’rəgiya-w dəʤʤ t’ərrəg-əʧʧ-ɨbb-ət Object of P with P Aster with-broom-DEF doorway sweep-3fS-with-3mO doubled on V ‘Aster swept a doorway with the broom.’ (Amberber 2002:56) So object agreement and accusative case do not seem to be two sides of the same coin here. Baker’s (to appear) analysis: Object agreement F agrees with the closest NP probing downward: the goal (7d, 8a, 8b) or the theme (6).1 This is subject to a phase-(like) condition, allowing agreement with goals/applied objects and shifted themes, but not with unshifted themes (7a-c) or objects of undoubled Ps (8c). It is not subject to the activity condition: (8a, c). (In terms of Baker 2008, this is a language with the Case Dependency of Agreement Parameter set “no”.) Where does Accusative case come from, if it is not assigned by F under agreement? The same place it does in Sakha, a language without agreement. It is dependent case. 1 The bearer of object agreement is probably not active v in Amharic, because passive and unaccusative verbs can show object agreement with goal arguments (e.g. (8b)). This is notably different from Chichewa, Mohawk, Nahuatl and Mapudungun. More parameters (e.g. the locus of object agreement)! 4 (15) If there are two distinct argumental nominals X and Y in the same clause such that X c-commands Y, then value the case feature of Y as accusative unless X has already been marked for case. Some confirmation for this: ACC is available in passives of triadic verbs but not dyadic ones (16) a. Ləmma gənzəb-u-n sərrək’-ə-w. Lemma money-DEF-ACC rob-3mS-3mO ‘Lemma stole the money.’ b. Gənzəb-u-(*n) tə-sərrək’-ə. Money-DEF-(*ACC) PASS-steal-3mS ‘The money was stolen (from Aster).’ (cf. Amberber 2002:9, WL:187) c. Ləmma Aster-ɨn gənzəb-u-n sərrək’-at. Lemma Aster-ACC money-DEF-ACC rob-(3mS)-3fO ‘Lemma robbed Aster of the money.’ d. Aster ʃant’a-wa-n tə-sərrək’-ɨʧ-(*əw) Aster suitcase-3fP-ACC PASS-rob-3fS-(*3mO) ‘Aster was robbed of her suitcase.’ (also WL:187) So we have different kinds of evidence that converges on the same parameterized principle of accusative case marking in two quite different languages: Sakha, where there is no object agreement, seen in some fancy constructions. Amharic, where there is object agreement, but it doesn’t pattern with accusative case. Question arising: Is accusative case ever the result of object agreement in languages where they are both overt (a parameter)? Or is accusative case fundamentally different from nominative in how it is assigned in all relevant languages (a principle; cf.