Mobile Containers in Human Cognitive Evolution Studies: Understudied and Underrepresented

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Mobile Containers in Human Cognitive Evolution Studies: Understudied and Underrepresented Langley Michelle (Orcid ID: 0000-0002-0299-5561) Mobile Containers in Human Cognitive Evolution Studies: Understudied and Underrepresented Michelle C. Langley & Thomas Suddendorf Issues Essay for Evolutionary Anthropology Michelle C. LANGLEY Australian Research Centre for Human Evolution Environmental Futures Research Institute Griffith University, Brisbane, AUSTRALIA Email: [email protected] Thomas SUDDENDORF Centre for Psychology & Evolution Early Cognitive Development Centre School of Psychology University of Queensland, Brisbane, AUSTRALIA Email: [email protected] Abstract Mobile carrying devices — slings, bags, boxes, containers, etc. — are a ubiquitous tool form amongst recent human communities. So ingrained are they to our present lifeways that the fundamental relationship between mobile containers and foresight is easily overlooked, resulting in their significance in the study of human cognitive development being largely unrecognised. Exactly when this game-changing innovation appeared and became an essential component of the human toolkit is currently unknown. Taphonomic processes are obviously a significant factor in this situation, however, we argue that these devices have also not received the attention that they deserve from human evolution researchers. Here we discuss what the current archaeological evidence is for Pleistocene-aged mobile containers and outline the various lines of evidence that they provide for the origins and development of human cognitive and cultural behaviour. Keywords Foresight; Planning; Bags; Slings; Organic Material Culture; Long-Distance Transport This is the author manuscript accepted for publication and has undergone full peer review but has not been through the copyediting, typesetting, pagination and proofreading process, which may lead to differences between this version and the Version of Record. Please cite this article as doi: 10.1002/evan.21857 This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. Running Title: Containers in Human Cognitive Evolution Author Biographies Michelle C. Langley is a ARC DECRA Senior Research Fellow in the Australian Research Centre for Human Evolution at Griffith University, Brisbane, Australia. She specialises in traceological studies of osseous and shell technologies with her research revolving around the evolution of modern behavioural patterns. Thomas Suddendorf is Director of the Early Cognitive Development Centre and Professor of Psychology at the University of Queensland, Australia. He investigates the cognitive capacities of nonhuman animals and of young children, to answer fundamental questions about the nature and evolution of the human mind. 2 This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Foresight — the ability to think about future situations, what will happen or be needed — is an essential human adaptive strategy1 and consequently, the subject of intense interest in evolutionary human sciences. Throughout all this debate and discussion, the collection and carrying of items by an individual for future use is most frequently cited in connection to archaeologically identifying the origin and development of foresight.2-9 Rarely though, is it considered exactly how one would carry such items over (often significant) distances while going about one’s usual subsistence and social necessities. Obviously, people can only carry so much in their hands and be actively using them at any one time. Mobile ‘containers’ — objects for holding and/or transporting something — are devices presumed to have an extended antiquity, though no substantial research has yet been undertaken to specifically pinpoint when and how this technology developed amongst hominins. Instead, publications have been largely restricted to reporting ‘earliest’ instances (usually in the context of foresight development) or brief speculations regarding their origins.10-12 Given the importance of bags, baskets, pockets, slings, boxes, tubs, bins, sacks (etc., etc.) for human existence, this oversight by scientists appears incongruous. Here we unpack the importance of including a targeted investigation of the origins and development of mobile containers for advancing our understanding of foresight in hominins. We also outline the currently available evidence for mobile containers in the global Pleistocene archaeological record and highlight possible future research avenues to be explored. 3 This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. 2. The Importance of Foresight Increasingly, foresight is being recognised as a quintessential human adaptive strategy. The modern human capacity to prudently consider future events, to be driven to prepare, to make contingency plans, and embed distant events into larger narratives has recently become a topic of intense research interest in the behavioural sciences.13-14 A surge of research has explored the neurological bases of foresight,13 its late and multifaceted development,15 its role in clinical conditions,16 in social interaction,17 decision making,18 and in economics.19 The very capacity to innovate may be regarded as a sign of foresight. Indeed, it has been argued that what turns a problem-solution into an innovation is the recognition of future utility.20 Because we can recognise future use of solutions, we are motivated to retain, refine, and potentially share them, and this ability, in turn, is critical for the emergence of the cumulative culture that characterises the modern human condition. We know that actions which increase the likelihood of future survival and reproductive success are under intense selection pressure, resulting in most species being able to act in tune with significant long-term regularities such as day/night and seasonal changes. Through associative learning, animals can further adapt to local regularities and adjust behaviour so as to increase chances of rewards and to avoid negative outcomes. Humans have evolved even more flexible prospective capacities, enabling us to foresee not only what is likely to happen next, but to consider multiple versions of potential future situations, of motivations we do not currently experience, and even of remote events that we have never experienced personally.21 There continues to be debate surrounding precisely what makes human foresight unique,22-24 especially given that various behaviours other than foresight can lead to future benefits, such as associative learning, instincts, and chance. Experiments with non-human animals and young children have thus gone to great lengths to rule out such alternative interpretations.22 3. Foresight and Containers While stone tools have long dominated debates surrounding the development of foresight in hominins,25 mobile containers could be considered more direct evidence for this capacity as carrying 4 This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. a range of items — which one’s hands cannot contain alone — cannot only solve an immediate problem, but also multiple future problems, while simultaneously and dramatically facilitating subsequent innovation by enabling the accumulation and transport of raw materials and tools to one place or the keeping on your person. Mobile containers allow us to retain a local environment that keeps tools and resources handy wherever we go. They hence help us be prepared for the future. Mobile containers can be considered meta-tools, that is tools that serve as a tool for other tools,26 and their emergence may have been a catalyst for the growth of material culture. Of course, many animals use tools, cache food, build nests, and transport materials. Functional analogs of carrying devices are also relatively widespread. Marsupials have natural baby slings in the form of pouches to carry offspring, birds carry food in their beaks and throat sacks to feed their brood, and cercopithecines stuff their cheeks with food for later consumption. But do they make or use mobile containers with foresight like humans do? McGrew noted that containers are markedly absent even in ape material culture.27 Although apes transport food in hands, feet, or mouth and by tucking it into a groin “pocket”, they do not make containers to carry more items over longer distances. This is not to say that chimpanzee and other animals cannot use containers per se. Shumaker et al.’s comprehensive survey of animal tool-use lists various instances of container use, especially in the context of obtaining and consuming water,28 however, significant differences remain in humans in terms of the manufacture and sustained as well as versatile use of mobile containers. There are very few claims for manufacture of anything akin to a container among non-human animals. Rogers and Kaplan describe rehabilitated orangutans arranging half a dozen large oblong leaves in a fan shape to eat semi-solid foods away from a feeding station.29 Captive chimpanzees also sometimes shuffle leaves into a ‘plate’ to carry food.30 However, captive and rehabilitated animals may have learned this behaviour from humans, with great apes in particular known to copy human behaviour. In language-acquisition projects, for instance, bonobos have used human-made bowls, boxes, cups, and even backpacks to carry liquids, foods, and objects.31 There is no doubt animals can be trained to act in ways which resemble human foresightful behavior. In the wild, there is little to suggest that animals retain tools for remote future events, let alone that they invented tools to keep (more) tools. Tools may be
Recommended publications
  • Tumbaga-Metal Figures from Panama: a Conservation Initiative to Save a Fragile Coll
    SPRING 2006 IssuE A PUBLICATION OF THE PEABODY MUSEUM AND THE DEPARTMENT OF ANTHROPOLOGY, HARVARD UNIVERSITY • 11 DIVINITY AVENUE CAMBRIDGE, MA 02138 DIGGING HARVARD YARD No smoking, drinking, glass-break­ the course, which was ing-what? Far from being a puritani­ taught by William Fash, cal haven, early Harvard College was a Howells director of the colorful and lively place. The first Peabody Museum, and Harvard students did more than wor­ Patricia Capone and Diana ship and study: they smoked, drank, Loren, Peabody Museum and broke a lot of windows. None of associate curators. They which was allowed, except on rare were assisted by Molly occasion. In the early days, Native Fierer-Donaldson, graduate American and English youth also stud­ student in Archaeology, ied, side by side. Archaeological and and Christina Hodge, historical records of early Harvard Peabody Museum senior Students excavating outside Matthews Hall. Photo by Patricia bring to life the experiences of our curatorial assistant and Capor1e. predecessors, teaching us that not only graduate student in is there an untold story to be Archaeology at Boston unearthed, but also, that we may learn University. ALEXANDER MARSHACK new perspectives by reflecting on our Archaeological data recovered from ARCHIVE DONATED TO THE shared history: a history that reaches Harvard Yard enriches our view of the PEABODY MUSEUM beyond the university walls to local seventeenth- through nineteenth-cen­ Native American communities. tury lives of students and faculty Jiving The Peabody Museum has received the The Fall 2005 course Anthropology in Harvard Yard. In the seventeenth generous donation of the photographs 1130: The Archaeology of Harvard Yard century, Harvard Yard included among and papers of Alexander Marshack brought today's Harvard students liter­ its four buildings the Old College, the from his widow, Elaine.
    [Show full text]
  • AP V49no1 Lu.Pdf
    Early Pottery in South China TRACEY L-D LU introduction In this paper, ‘‘early pottery’’ is defined as ceramics dated to approximately 10,000 years ago or earlier, which have been discovered from the Japanese Archi- pelago, the Russian Far East, the Yellow and the Yangzi River Valleys, to South China (Tables 1 and 2). Pottery discovered in the Japanese Archipelago are dated to between 15,000 and 12,000 years ago (Tsutsumi 2000), or even up to 17,200 b.p. (Kuzmin 2006); those found in the Russian Far East are dated between 13,300 and 12,300 years ago, or 16,500–14,100 b.p. (Kuzmin 2006; Zhushchi- khovskaya 1997). Potsherds found in North China are dated to between 12,000 and 10,000 years ago (Guo and Li 2000; Zhao et al. 2003), those found in the Yangzi River Valley are dated probably up to 18,000 years ago (Boaretto et al. 2009), and pottery found in South China is dated to approximately 12,000 years ago (Institute of Archaeology CASS et al. 2003). It seems that pottery was manu- factured by di¤erent groups in di¤erent natural and cultural contexts at the end of the Pleistocene or the beginning of the Holocene1 in various places of East Asia, although it is not clear whether pottery was invented in one center or in multi- centers. There are many hypotheses on the origin of pottery, including the ‘‘architec- tural hypothesis,’’ the ‘‘culinary hypothesis,’’ which proposes that pottery was invented for cooking cereals and/or shells, the ‘‘resources intensification’’ hypoth- esis, and the ‘‘social/symbolic elaboration’’ theory (Rice 1999:5–14).
    [Show full text]
  • Ceramic Technology
    Ceramic Technology Oxford Handbooks Online Ceramic Technology Peter Hommel The Oxford Handbook of the Archaeology and Anthropology of Hunter-Gatherers Edited by Vicki Cummings, Peter Jordan, and Marek Zvelebil Print Publication Date: Apr Subject: Archaeology, Production, Trade, and Exchange 2014 Online Publication Date: Oct DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199551224.013.008 2013 Abstract and Keywords Although the use of unfired clay and even the production of non-utilitarian ceramic artefacts by Palaeolithic hunter- gatherers has long been accepted, their role in the emergence and spread of ceramic vessel technology has, until recently, received little scholarly attention. Pottery was seen as a technology of sedentary agriculturalists, for which mobile hunters and gatherers would have had little use. This article reviews some of the archaeological evidence that has been used to support a reassessment of this traditional paradigm, showing that the agricultural connection can often be broken decisively and that themes of practicality and prestige, which characterize traditional discussions of the emergence of pottery, can be equally successful in exploring its origins as a hunter- gatherer technology. Keywords: hunter-gatherer, pottery, ceramic vessel technology, origins AT the outset, it is necessary to make the distinction between clay (a natural argillaceous material that, when suitably processed, becomes plastic and can be shaped into almost any form), ceramic (which in this context is used to refer generally to clay forms, fashioned, dried, and deliberately heated at high temperature with the intention of producing a durable product), and pottery (which refers specifically to portable ceramic vessels). Though apparently minor, these distinctions are important and not merely for reasons of greater clarity in the following discussion.
    [Show full text]
  • The Neolithic Ofsouthern China-Origin, Development, and Dispersal
    The Neolithic ofSouthern China-Origin, Development, and Dispersal ZHANG CHI AND HSIAO-CHUN HUNG INTRODUCTION SANDWICHED BETWEEN THE YELLOW RIVER and Mainland Southeast Asia, southern China1 lies centrally within eastern Asia. This geographical area can be divided into three geomorphological terrains: the middle and lower Yangtze allu­ vial plain, the Lingnan (southern Nanling Mountains)-Fujian region,2 and the Yungui Plateau3 (Fig. 1). During the past 30 years, abundant archaeological dis­ coveries have stimulated a rethinking of the role ofsouthern China in the prehis­ tory of China and Southeast Asia. This article aims to outline briefly the Neolithic cultural developments in the middle and lower Yangtze alluvial plain, to discuss cultural influences over adjacent regions and, most importantly, to examine the issue of southward population dispersal during this time period. First, we give an overview of some significant prehistoric discoveries in south­ ern China. With the discovery of Hemudu in the mid-1970s as the divide, the history of archaeology in this region can be divided into two phases. The first phase (c. 1920s-1970s) involved extensive discovery, when archaeologists un­ earthed Pleistocene human remains at Yuanmou, Ziyang, Liujiang, Maba, and Changyang, and Palaeolithic industries in many caves. The major Neolithic cul­ tures, including Daxi, Qujialing, Shijiahe, Majiabang, Songze, Liangzhu, and Beiyinyangying in the middle and lower Yangtze, and several shell midden sites in Lingnan, were also discovered in this phase. During the systematic research phase (1970s to the present), ongoing major ex­ cavation at many sites contributed significantly to our understanding of prehis­ toric southern China. Additional early human remains at Wushan, Jianshi, Yun­ xian, Nanjing, and Hexian were recovered together with Palaeolithic assemblages from Yuanmou, the Baise basin, Jianshi Longgu cave, Hanzhong, the Li and Yuan valleys, Dadong and Jigongshan.
    [Show full text]
  • Intensified Foraging and the Roots of Farming in China
    Boise State University ScholarWorks Anthropology Faculty Publications and Presentations Department of Anthropology Fall 2017 Intensified orF aging and the Roots of Farming in China Shengqian Chen Renmin University of China Pei-Lin Yu Boise State University This document was originally published in the Journal of Anthropological Research by the University of Chicago Press. Copyright restrictions may apply. doi: 10.1086/692660 Intensified Foraging and the Roots of Farming in China SHENGQIAN CHEN, School of History, Renmin University of China, Beijing, China PEI-LIN YU, Department of Anthropology, Boise State University, Boise, ID 83725, USA. Email: [email protected] In an accompanying paper ( Journal of Anthropological Research 73(2):149–80, 2017), the au- thors assess current archaeological and paleobiological evidence for the early Neolithic of China. Emerging trends in archaeological data indicate that early agriculture developed variably: hunt- ing remained important on the Loess Plateau, and aquatic-based foraging and protodomes- tication augmented cereal agriculture in South China. In North China and the Yangtze Basin, semisedentism and seasonal foraging persisted alongside early Neolithic culture traits such as or- ganized villages, large storage structures, ceramic vessels, and polished stone tool assemblages. In this paper, we seek to explain incipient agriculture as a predictable, system-level cultural response of prehistoric foragers through an evolutionary assessment of archaeological evidence for the pre- ceding Paleolithic to Neolithic transition (PNT). We synthesize a broad range of diagnostic ar- tifacts, settlement, site structure, and biological remains to develop a working hypothesis that agriculture was differentially developed or adopted according to “initial conditions” of habitat, resource structure, and cultural organization.
    [Show full text]
  • SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT Ian Morris
    SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT Ian Morris © Ian Morris Stanford University October 2010 http://www.ianmorris.org 1 Contents List of Tables, Maps, Figures, and Graphs 4 1 Introduction 7 2 Formal Definition 9 3 Core Assumptions 10 3.1 Quantification 10 3.2 Parsimony 10 3.3 Traits 10 3.4 Criteria 11 3.5 The focus on East and West 11 3.6 Core regions 12 3.7 Measurement intervals 16 3.8 Approximation and falsification 16 4 Core Objections 17 4.1 Dehumanization 17 4.2 Inappropriate definition 17 4.3 Inappropriate traits 17 4.4 Empirical errors 21 5 Models for an Index of Social Development 22 5.1 Social development indices in neo-evolutionary anthropology 22 5.2 The United Nations Human Development Index 23 6 Trait Selection 25 7 Methods of Calculation 26 8 Energy Capture 28 8.1 Energy capture, real wages, and GDP, GNP, and NDI per capita 28 8.2 Units of measurement and abbreviations 32 8.3 The nature of the evidence 33 8.4 Estimates of Western energy capture 35 8.4.1 The recent past, 1700-2000 CE 36 8.4.2 Classical antiquity (500 BCE–200 CE) 39 8.4.3 Between ancient and modern (200–1700 CE) 50 8.4.3.1 200-700 CE 50 8.4.3.2 700-1300 CE 53 8.4.3.3 1300-1700 CE 55 8.4.4 Late Ice Age hunter-gatherers (c. 14,000 BCE) 57 8.4.5 From foragers to imperialists (14,000-500 BCE) 59 8.4.6 Western energy capture: discussion 73 2 8.5 Estimates of Eastern energy capture 75 8.5.1 The recent past, 1800-2000 CE 79 8.5.2 Song dynasty China (960-1279 CE) 83 8.5.3 Early modern China (1300-1700 CE) 85 8.5.4 Ancient China (200 BCE-200 CE) 88 8.5.5 Between ancient and medieval (200-1000 CE) 91 8.5.6 Post-Ice Age hunter-gatherers (c.
    [Show full text]
  • BIBLIOGRAPHY of ROCK ART DATING Marvin W
    118 Rock Art Research 2012 - Volume 29, Number 1. ORIENTATION BIBLIOGRAPHY OF ROCK ART DATING Marvin W. Rowe Introduction thiques. Bulletin de la Société Préhistorique Française de I have tried to make this bibliography as complete l’Ariege 91: 51–70. as possible. I have made no value judgements and Clottes, J. 1996. The Chauvet Cave dates implausible? have included all references that I could find. Some International Newsletter on Rock Art13: 27–29. Clottes, J. 1998. The ‘three Cs’: fresh avenues towards of these listed have not stood the test of time, but are European Palaeolithic art. In C. Chippindale and P. S. included for completeness. I have also tried to put the C. Taçon (eds(, The archaeology of rock-art, pp. 112–129. following references into some sort of category. The Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. two broad categories are pictograms and petroglyphs. Clottes, J. 1999. The Chauvet Cave dates. In J. M. Coles, Then categories under these two are based on pigments R. Bewley and P. Mellars (eds), World history: studies in and techniques. I have undoubtedly mis-categorised the memory of Grahame Clark, pp. 13–19. Memoires of the some of the references and apologise in advance for British Academy, London. any mistakes and omissions that I have inadvertently Clottes, J. 1999. Twenty thousand years of Palaeolithic cave made. Some references appear under multiple catego- art in southern France. Proceedings of the British Academy 99: 161–175. ries. Clottes, J., J. M. Chauvet, E. Brunel-Deschamps, C. Hillaire, J. P. Daugas, M. Arnold, H. Cachier, J.
    [Show full text]
  • Program of the 82Nd Annual Meeting
    PROGRAM OF THE 82ND ANNUAL MEETING March 29–April 2, 2017 Vancouver, BC, Canada THE ANNUAL MEETING of the Society for American Archaeology provides a forum for the dissemination of knowledge and discussion. The views expressed at the sessions are solely those of the speakers and the Society does not endorse, approve, or censor them. Descriptions of events and titles are those of the organizers, not the Society. Program of the 82nd Annual Meeting Published by the Society for American Archaeology 1111 14th Street NW, Suite 800 Washington, DC 20005-5622 USA Tel: +1 202/789-8200 Fax: +1 202/789-0284 E-mail: [email protected] WWW: http://www.saa.org Copyright © 2017 Society for American Archaeology. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reprinted in any form or by any means without prior permission from the publisher. Contents 4 ............. Awards Presentation and Annual Business Meeting Agenda 5 ............. 2017 Award Recipients 15 ........... Maps 24 ........... Meeting Organizers, SAA Board of Directors, and SAA Staff 27 ........... General Information 29 ........... Featured Sessions 31 ........... Summary Schedule 36 ........... A Word about the Sessions 37 ........... Sessions at a Glance 45 ........... Program 251 ......... SAA Awards, Scholarships, and Fellowships 260 ......... Presidents of SAA 261 ......... Annual Meeting Sites 262 ......... Exhibit Map 263 ......... Exhibitor Directory 274 ......... SAA Committees and Task Forces 281 ......... Index of Participants Awards Presentation and Annual Business Meeting MARCH 31, 2017 5:00 PM Call to Order Call for Approval of Minutes of the 2016 Annual Business Meeting Remarks President Diane Gifford-Gonzalez Reports Treasurer Deborah Nichols Secretary Patricia Gilman Executive Director Tobi A.
    [Show full text]
  • Chinese Pottery May Be Earliest Discovered 2 June 2009, by RANDOLPH E
    Chinese pottery may be earliest discovered 2 June 2009, By RANDOLPH E. SCHMID , AP Science Writer (AP) -- Bits of pottery discovered in a cave in and the very detailed dating of the strata around the southern China may be evidence of the earliest new pottery." development of ceramics by ancient people. "This sets Yuchanyan as the earliest site where The find in Yuchanyan Cave dates to as much as pottery has been made," she said. "We do not 18,000 years ago, researchers report in Tuesday's know if the technology moved from China to the edition of Proceedings of the National Academy of other sites, but this hypothesis is stronger now than Sciences. before." The find "supports the proposal made in the past Patrick E. McGovern, an anthropologist at the that pottery making by foragers began in south University of Pennsylvania, noted that figurines China," according to the researchers, led by have been found in what is now the Czech Elisabetta Boaretto of Bar Ilan University in Israel. Republic that go back as far as 35,000 years. But those were not actual pottery vessels, he said. The pottery found at Yuchanyan "is the earliest so far," Boaretto said. "I had long thought that Japan would be the earliest," McGovern said, but in researching his Pottery was one of the first human-made materials forthcoming book on the history of alcoholic drinks, and tracing its origins and development opens a "Uncorking the Past," he found evidence of window on the development of culture, said Tracey development of ancient drinks in China.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Dental Tissues: Advancement in Virtual Dental Analysis
    DOTTORATO DI RICERCA IN BIOLOGIA (XXX CICLO) Human dental tissues: Advancement in virtual dental analysis Tesi di Gregorio Oxilia Coordinatore Prof. Alberto Ugolini Tutor Prof. Jacopo Moggi Cecchi Co-tutor Prof. Stefano Benazzi (2018) DOTTORATO DI RICERCA IN BIOLOGIA (Antropologia e Primatologia) CICLO XXX Human dental tissues: Advancement in virtual dental analysis. Settore Scientifico Disciplinare BIO/08 Dottorando: Dott. Oxilia Gregorio Tutore: Prof. Jacopo MoggiCecchi Co-tutore: Prof. Stefano Benazzi Coordinatore: Prof. Alberto Ugolini Anni 2014/2017 TABLE OF CONTENTS Abstract ...............................................................................................................3 List of Publications .............................................................................................7 Introduction ......................................................................................................13 1. OCCLUSAL SURFACE ALTERATIONS ........................................15 1.1 Masticatory activity ..............................................................................15 1.2 Pathological alteration .........................................................................15 1.3 Anthropic alteration .............................................................................17 1.4 Cultural expression ..............................................................................18 2 TAXONOMICAL IDENTIFICATION .............................................21 2.1 Crown ....................................................................................................21
    [Show full text]
  • Homo Sapiens) and Baboon (Papio Hamadryas) Craniofacial Complex
    A01 24 April 2006 Face Value: Comparative Quantitative Genetics of the Human (Homo sapiens) and Baboon (Papio hamadryas) Craniofacial Complex Richard J. Sherwood, Lifespan Health Research Center, Wright State University School of Medicine and Department of Neuroscience, Cell Biology, and Physiology, Wright State University Dana L. Duren, Lifespan Health Research Center, Wright State University School of Medicine and Department of Neuroscience, Cell Biology, and Physiology, Wright State University John Blangero, Department of Genetics, Southwest Foundation for Biomedical Research and Southwest National Primate Research Center Michael C. Mahaney, Department of Genetics, Southwest Foundation for Biomedical Research and Southwest National Primate Research Center Bradford Towne, Lifespan Health Research Center, Wright State University School of Medicine Recent research has explored the application of quantitative genetic analyses to interpretations of traits frequently used in phylogenetic reconstructions of fossil hominins. Parameters such as heritability (h2) and genetic correlation (ρg), for example, can provide critical information regarding the potential response rate of traits to evolutionary forces and the integration of trait response. Application of these parameters to the fossil record, however, may be problematic given the caveat that these parameters are population-specific statistics, and do not describe variation at the level of genus or species. The utility of heritability and genetic correlations in interpretations of the fossil record may be justified if it can be demonstrated that estimates are robust across populations and/or taxa. To examine the genetic underpinnings of the structure and development of the primate craniofacial complex, nine craniofacial measures were taken from lateral cephalograms of 304 baboons (aged 2-8 years) at the Southwest National Primate Research Center, San Antonio, TX, and 389 humans (aged 6-24 years) from the Fels Longitudinal Study, Dayton, OH.
    [Show full text]
  • Inland Post-Glacial Dispersal in East Asia Revealed by Mitochondrial
    Peng et al. BMC Biology 2011, 9:2 http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7007/9/2 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Inland post-glacial dispersal in East Asia revealed by mitochondrial haplogroup M9a’b Min-Sheng Peng1,2,6, Malliya Gounder Palanichamy3, Yong-Gang Yao4, Bikash Mitra3,5, Yao-Ting Cheng1,2,6, Mian Zhao1,2,6, Jia Liu3, Hua-Wei Wang3, Hui Pan1,2,6, Wen-Zhi Wang1,2,6, A-Mei Zhang4,6, Wen Zhang4,6, Dong Wang4,6, Yang Zou4,6, Yang Yang3, Tapas Kumar Chaudhuri5, Qing-Peng Kong1,2*, Ya-Ping Zhang1,2,3* Abstract Background: Archaeological studies have revealed a series of cultural changes around the Last Glacial Maximum in East Asia; whether these changes left any signatures in the gene pool of East Asians remains poorly indicated. To achieve deeper insights into the demographic history of modern humans in East Asia around the Last Glacial Maximum, we extensively analyzed mitochondrial DNA haplogroup M9a’b, a specific haplogroup that was suggested to have some potential for tracing the migration around the Last Glacial Maximum in East Eurasia. Results: A total of 837 M9a’b mitochondrial DNAs (583 from the literature, while the remaining 254 were newly collected in this study) pinpointed from over 28,000 subjects residing across East Eurasia were studied here. Fifty- nine representative samples were further selected for total mitochondrial DNA sequencing so we could better understand the phylogeny within M9a’b. Based on the updated phylogeny, an extensive phylogeographic analysis was carried out to reveal the differentiation of haplogroup M9a’b and to reconstruct the dispersal histories.
    [Show full text]