MOTIVATED REASONING 1 Cognitive Dissonance, Ego-Involvement, And

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

MOTIVATED REASONING 1 Cognitive Dissonance, Ego-Involvement, And MOTIVATED REASONING 1 Cognitive Dissonance, Ego-Involvement, and Motivated Reasoning Christopher J. Carpenter Department of Communication Western Illinois University [email protected] In press at the Annals of the International Communication Association doi: 10.1080/23808985.2018.1564881 Author Acknowledgement: I would like to thank the Editor and the anonymous reviewers for providing thoughtful feedback that I believe substantially strengthened this article. MOTIVATED REASONING 2 Abstract One of the enduring topics for persuasion research is motivated reasoning, when people respond to persuasive messages in ways other than seeking to form an accurate attitude. This essay advances the position that the existing research can be synthesized using the self-concept approach to cognitive dissonance with ego-involvement added as the key explanatory variable to indicate when an issue is likely tied to the individual’s self-concept and thus potentially produce cognitive dissonance. The research on motivated reasoning is reviewed with this theoretical lens with recommendations on how to use this theory to advance research on understanding and reducing maladaptive motivated reasoning. Keywords: cognitive dissonance, ego-involvement, motivated reasoning, persuasion, science communication, political communication, health communication MOTIVATED REASONING 3 Cognitive Dissonance, Ego-Involvement, and Motivated Reasoning “Once a man’s understanding has settled on something (either because it is an accepted belief or because it pleases him), it draws everything else also to support and agree with it. And if it encounters a larger number of more powerful countervailing examples, it either fails to notice them, or disregards them, or makes fine distinctions to dismiss and reject them, and all this with much dangerous prejudice, to preserve the authority of its first conceptions” p. 43 -Francis Bacon (1620/2000) “I took care not to dispute anything he said, for there’s no arguing with an Enthusiast. Better not to take it into one’s head to tell a lover the faults of his mistress, or a litigant the weakness of his cause- or to talk sense to a fanatic.” p. 5 -Voltaire “Philosophical Letters” (1732/1961) The above quotations reveal that thinkers have long known that humans are biased towards their cherished beliefs. For some of our opinions, we are willing to ignore compelling evidence, credible sources, and even basic logic in order to go on maintaining our original opinion. If the arguments presented to us differ more substantially from our established opinions, we cling even more strongly to our own opinions. For example, if we strongly identify with a position on restrictions on firearm ownership, we may reject any argument in favor of substantially greater or weaker restrictions than those we already support. For some other opinions, we are readily willing to shift our attitudes towards a different position if compelling evidence is presented. The greater the gap between the position presented MOTIVATED REASONING 4 and our existing opinion, the greater our shift towards the position presented. If a doctor told us that credible research suggests that eating kiwi greatly increased our cancer risk, most of us would give up eating kiwi (no such research exists, to my knowledge). What these situations illustrate is the differential ways that we tend to react to attitude discrepant messages. A greater gap between what one already believes and what is advocated in a well-evidenced, logical, credible persuasive message can either greatly move us to change our own position or strengthen our resolve to maintain it. A number of variables have been studied to determine the outcomes associated with greater or lesser discrepancy between the message and the audience’s existing attitude (Fink & Cai, 2013). Greater resistance to an attitude discrepant message can be considered a type of motivated reasoning. Kahan defined motivated reasoning as “the tendency of people to conform assessments of information to some goal or end extrinsic to accuracy” (2013, p. 408). That definition will be used for this review. Discovering the moderating variable(s) that predict motivated reasoning is a key problem for persuasion researchers as well as any applied researchers seeking to influence public behavior in areas such as health communication, science communication, risk communication, and advertising. The position I articulate in the following review is that cognitive dissonance theory can be used to explain and predict motivated reasoning outcomes. In particular this review will expand the modification of cognitive dissonance proposed by Aronson (1968; 1992) and Rokeach (1968; 1973) labeled the self-concept approach to cognitive dissonance. I will argue this approach to cognitive dissonance can be profitably expanded with the postulate that ego- involvement, as originally described by Sherif and Cantril (1947), can be used to predict the ego- defensive motivations associated with the self-concept approach to cognitive dissonance and thereby motivated reasoning more generally. MOTIVATED REASONING 5 This review will begin by describing cognitive dissonance theory. Then I will explain how ego-involvement can serve as an indicator of the key motives in the theory. This section will be followed by an exposition of the various ways it and similar variables have been studied. Then I will explore how cognitive dissonance can be used to understand various types of motivated reasoning. Finally, I will review research on attempts to reduce people’s likelihood of using motivated reasoning before offering closing remarks. Aronson (1992) argued that the social sciences have become saturated with many, smaller theories, when the self-concept approach to cognitive dissonance could serve as a stronger tool for theoretical integration. This review attempts to continue that integration. Cognitive Dissonance Cognitive dissonance, according to Festinger (1957), is produced when one person has two cognitive elements that are inconsistent with each other. One of the key parts of cognitive dissonance theory is that such inconsistency produces a feeling of negative arousal. People experiencing dissonance are motivated to reduce the inconsistency because they wish to avoid or reduce that negative arousal. They then attempt to reduce the dissonance by altering one of the dissonant cognitive elements. For example recalling that one wasted time by going for a walk is inconsistent with a belief that one should use one’s time wisely. Dissonance might be reduced by considering that breaks make one more productive after the walk. Festinger also argued people can foresee when dissonance might occur and become motivated to avoid dissonance. The inconsistency between two elements is heightened when one or more of the dissonant elements are more important to the individual. In particular, he claimed that, “The maximum dissonance which could exist would, in such circumstances, be determined by the resistance to admitting that he had been wrong or foolish” (p. 29). MOTIVATED REASONING 6 One of the misunderstandings concerning cognitive dissonance theory is that it is only concerned with the effects of behavior on attitudes. This misperception is understandable given that the seminal demonstration of the theory focused on that aspect (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959) and that much of the cognitive dissonance-inspired communication research has focused on the effects of counterattitudinal advocacy (see S. Y. Kim, Allen, Preiss, & Peterson, 2014, for a meta-analysis of this research). Yet, in the original formulation of the theory, Festinger (1957) argued that exposure to messages inconsistent with one’s own beliefs could produce cognitive dissonance. The belief that an otherwise reasonable individual believes something different than oneself is theoretically predicted to produce cognitive dissonance. He explained further that such dissonance “may be reduced if he can believe that the latter is a stupid, ignorant, unfriendly, and bigoted individual” (p. 183). Expanding on this aspect of cognitive dissonance theory, Aronson, Turner, and Carlsmith (1963) argued that hearing a message that is highly discrepant from one’s own attitudes from a highly credible source would be likely to produce a substantial amount of cognitive dissonance. They noted that the audience can reduce the dissonance created by such a message through many means including changing their own attitudes, trying to change the source’s opinion, finding support for their own opinion from like-minded people, or rejecting the source. The idea of general cognitive dissonance was narrowed to cognitive dissonance centered on the self-concept by one of Festinger’s students, Elliot Aronson (1968). He argued that dissonance is substantially more likely to be aroused when at least one of the dissonant elements is closely tied to one’s self-concept. Thus, people who feel that their affection for kiwifruit is a key part of their self-identity would experience cognitive dissonance when encountering a message about the long-term dangers of consuming this fruit. On the other hand, someone who MOTIVATED REASONING 7 has generally enjoyed eating kiwi but does not strongly identify with that habit would not feel cognitive dissonance when encountering a message about its dangers. This approach is consistent with the synthesis of cognitive dissonance and self-perception theory proposed by Fazio, Zanna, and Cooper (1977) that suggested that cognitive dissonance
Recommended publications
  • Back to the Future of Dissonance Theory: Cognitive Consistency As a Core Motive
    Social Cognition, Vol. 30, No. 6, 2012, pp. 652–668 GAWRONSKI COGNITIVE CONSISTENCY AS A CORE MOTIVE BACK TO THE FUTURE OF DISSONANCE THEORY: COGNITIVE CONSISTENCY AS A CORE MOTIVE Bertram Gawronski The University of Western Ontario In his theory of cognitive dissonance, Festinger (1957) described cogni- tive consistency as a psychological need that is as basic as hunger and thirst. Over the past decades, however, the idea of cognitive consistency as a core motive has been replaced by an increasingly narrow focus on dissonance-related changes in attitudes and alternative accounts that at- tribute such changes to mechanisms of ego-defense. The current article aims at reviving the idea of cognitive consistency as a core motive, arguing that inconsistency serves as an epistemic cue for errors in one’s system of beliefs. Because inconsistency can often be resolved in multiple ways, motivated reasoning can bias processes of inconsistency resolution toward desired conclusions, although motivated distortions are constrained by the need for cognitive consistency. The ubiquity of consistency processes is il- lustrated through its role in various instances of threat-compensation (e.g., victim derogation, self-verification, system justification) and the insights that can be gained from reconceptualizing various social psychological phenomena in terms of cognitive consistency (e.g., prejudice-related belief systems, dispositional inference, stability of first impressions). Festinger’s (1957) theory of cognitive dissonance is arguably one of the most in- fluential theories in the history of social psychology. The theory postulates that inconsistent cognitions elicit an aversive state of arousal (i.e., dissonance), which in turn produces a desire to reduce the underlying inconsistency and to maintain a state of consonance.1 Although Festinger was convinced that the psychological need for cognitive consistency is as basic as hunger and thirst, several revisions 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Psychological Approaches to Consciousness
    P1: KAE 0521857430c20 CUFX049/Zelazo 0 521 85743 0 printer: cupusbw November 6, 2006 15:55 F. Anthropology/Social Psychology of Consciousness 551 P1: KAE 0521857430c20 CUFX049/Zelazo 0 521 85743 0 printer: cupusbw November 6, 2006 15:55 552 P1: KAE 0521857430c20 CUFX049/Zelazo 0 521 85743 0 printer: cupusbw November 6, 2006 15:55 CHAPTER 20 Social Psychological Approaches to Consciousness John A. Bargh Abstract any given phenomenon. However, because these studies focus on the relative influence A central focus of contemporary social psy- of both conscious and automatic processes, chology has been the relative influence there has been a strong influence within of external (i.e., environmental, situational) social psychology of dual-process models versus internal (i.e., personality, attitudes) that capture these distinctions (e.g., inten- forces in determining social judgment and tional versus unintentional, effortful versus social behavior. But many of the classic find- efficient, aware versus unaware). Another ings in the field – such as Milgram’s obe- reason that dual-process models became dience research, Asch’s conformity studies, popular in social psychology is that the dis- and Zimbardo’s mock-prison experiment – tinction nicely captured an important truth seemed to indicate that the external forces about social cognition and behavior: that swamped the internal ones when the chips people seem to process the identical social were down. Where in the social psycholog- information differently depending on its rel- ical canon was the evidence showing the evance or centrality to their important goals internal, intentional, rational control of one’s and purposes. own behavior? Interestingly, most models of a given phenomenon in social psychol- ogy have started with the assumption of a Introduction major mediational role played by conscious choice and intentional guidance of judg- Historically, social psychology has been con- ment and behavior processes.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Accuracy Dominates Bias and Self-Fulfilling Prophecy
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2017), Page 1 of 65 doi:10.1017/S0140525X1500062X,e1 Précis of Social Perception and Social Reality: Why accuracy dominates bias and self-fulfilling prophecy Lee Jussim Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ 08544. [email protected] http://www.rci.rutgers.edu/∼jussim Abstract: Social Perception and Social Reality (Jussim 2012) reviews the evidence in social psychology and related fields and reaches three conclusions: (1) Although errors, biases, and self-fulfilling prophecies in person perception are real, reliable, and occasionally quite powerful, on average, they tend to be weak, fragile, and fleeting. (2) Perceptions of individuals and groups tend to be at least moderately, and often highly accurate. (3) Conclusions based on the research on error, bias, and self-fulfilling prophecies routinely greatly overstate their power and pervasiveness, and consistently ignore evidence of accuracy, agreement, and rationality in social perception. The weight of the evidence – including some of the most classic research widely interpreted as testifying to the power of biased and self-fulfilling processes – is that interpersonal expectations relate to social reality primarily because they reflect rather than cause social reality. This is the case not only for teacher expectations, but also for social stereotypes, both as perceptions of groups, and as the bases of expectations regarding individuals. The time is long overdue to replace cherry-picked and unjustified stories emphasizing error, bias, the power of self-fulfilling prophecies, and the inaccuracy of stereotypes, with conclusions that more closely correspond to the full range of empirical findings, which includes multiple failed replications of classic expectancy studies, meta- analyses consistently demonstrating small or at best moderate expectancy effects, and high accuracy in social perception.
    [Show full text]
  • Illusion and Well-Being: a Social Psychological Perspective on Mental Health
    Psyehologlcal Bulletin Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1988, Vol. 103, No. 2, 193-210 0033-2909/88/$00.75 Illusion and Well-Being: A Social Psychological Perspective on Mental Health Shelley E. Taylor Jonathon D. Brown University of California, Los Angeles Southern Methodist University Many prominenttheorists have argued that accurate perceptions of the self, the world, and the future are essential for mental health. Yet considerable research evidence suggests that overly positive self- evaluations, exaggerated perceptions of control or mastery, and unrealistic optimism are characteris- tic of normal human thought. Moreover, these illusions appear to promote other criteria of mental health, including the ability to care about others, the ability to be happy or contented, and the ability to engage in productive and creative work. These strategies may succeed, in large part, because both the social world and cognitive-processingmechanisms impose filters on incoming information that distort it in a positive direction; negativeinformation may be isolated and represented in as unthreat- ening a manner as possible. These positive illusions may be especially useful when an individual receives negative feedback or is otherwise threatened and may be especially adaptive under these circumstances. Decades of psychological wisdom have established contact dox: How can positive misperceptions of one's self and the envi- with reality as a hallmark of mental health. In this view, the ronment be adaptive when accurate information processing wcU-adjusted person is thought to engage in accurate reality seems to be essential for learning and successful functioning in testing,whereas the individual whose vision is clouded by illu- the world? Our primary goal is to weave a theoretical context sion is regarded as vulnerable to, ifnot already a victim of, men- for thinking about mental health.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory
    IFM LAB TUTORIAL SERIES # 6, COPYRIGHT c IFM LAB Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory Jiawei Zhang [email protected] Founder and Director Information Fusion and Mining Laboratory (First Version: May 2019; Revision: May 2019.) Abstract This is a follow-up tutorial article of [17] and [16], in this paper, we will introduce several important cognitive functions of the brain. Brain cognitive functions are the mental processes that allow us to receive, select, store, transform, develop, and recover information that we've received from external stimuli. This process allows us to understand and to relate to the world more effectively. Cognitive functions are brain-based skills we need to carry out any task from the simplest to the most complex. They are related with the mechanisms of how we learn, remember, problem-solve, and pay attention, etc. To be more specific, in this paper, we will talk about the perception, attention and memory functions of the human brain. Several other brain cognitive functions, e.g., arousal, decision making, natural language, motor coordination, planning, problem solving and thinking, will be added to this paper in the later versions, respectively. Many of the materials used in this paper are from wikipedia and several other neuroscience introductory articles, which will be properly cited in this paper. This is the last of the three tutorial articles about the brain. The readers are suggested to read this paper after the previous two tutorial articles on brain structure and functions [17] as well as the brain basic neural units [16]. Keywords: The Brain; Cognitive Function; Consciousness; Attention; Learning; Memory Contents 1 Introduction 2 2 Perception 3 2.1 Detailed Process of Perception .
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion: Social and Neuroscience Perspectives Mahzarin Banaji and Nalini Ambady Spring 2002
    Emotion: Social and Neuroscience Perspectives Mahzarin Banaji and Nalini Ambady Spring 2002 When two disciplines, one interested in understanding social cognition, the other in understanding the brain meet, a topic of obvious mutual interest is the study of emotion. Both neuroscientists and psychologists have learned on their own about the neural basis of emotion and its expressions in social life. In this course, two instructors whose primary expertise is in social cognition, will teach about social and neuroscience perspectives on emotion, with a focus on recent advances. The first part of the course will focus on theoretical perspectives that address the neurobiology and neuropsychology of emotion. The second part will require attention to specific emotions such as fear, anger, and disgust – emotions whose neural basis is best understood. The third section will explore the interaction of emotion with memory and the fourth will delve into the newly growing area of emotion and social relations. Finally, in section 5, we take up issues concerning attitude and beliefs as examined in the minds and brains of individuals as they construe themselves and others as members of social groups. Requirements. 1. Students will be expected to lead at least one of the class sessions, most likely with other students, depending on the size of the class. This will count for 20% of the final grade. 2. The day before each class session, students will be expected to turn in a thoughtful discussion question that draws on the readings. Of course, the questions should be relevant to the topic. Attempts to integrate social and neuroscience perspectives on the issue are particularly important.
    [Show full text]
  • Facial Image Manipulation : a Tool for Investigating Social Perception
    This is a repository copy of Facial image manipulation : a tool for investigating social perception. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/118275/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Sutherland, Clare, Rhodes, Gillian and Young, Andrew William orcid.org/0000-0002-1202- 6297 (2017) Facial image manipulation : a tool for investigating social perception. Social Psychological and Personality Science. pp. 538-551. ISSN 1948-5506 https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617697176 Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ FACIAL IMAGE MANIPULATION 1 Authors' version of manuscript accepted for publication in Social Psychological and Personality Science. Date accepted: 30th January 2017. Facial Image Manipulation: A Tool for Investigating Social Perception Clare AM Sutherlanda, Gillian Rhodesa & Andrew W Younga,b aARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia. bDepartment of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York, UK.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Perception and Attribution 3 Brian Parkinson
    9781405124003_4_003.qxd 10/31/07 2:55 PM Page 42 Social Perception and Attribution 3 Brian Parkinson KEY CONCEPTS actor–observer difference analysis of non-common effects attributional bias augmenting principle causal power causal schema central trait cognitive algebra configural model consensus information consistency information correspondence bias correspondent inference theory covariation theory depressive realism discounting principle distinctiveness information false consensus bias implicit personality theory learned helplessness theory naïve scientist model peripheral trait primacy effect salience self-fulfilling prophecy self-serving biases 9781405124003_4_003.qxd 10/31/07 2:55 PM Page 43 CHAPTER OUTLINE How can we tell what other people are like? How do we explain their actions and experiences (and our own)? This chapter introduces research intended to answer these questions. Studies of social perception show that impressions of others depend on what information is presented, how it is pre- sented, and on prior assumptions about how it fits together. Research into attribution demonstrates that perceivers consistently favour certain kinds of explanation over others. Our impressions and ex- planations are also shaped by our specific reasons for constructing them. In particular, we present social events in different ways to different people under different circumstances. Both social percep- tion and attribution therefore involve communication in combination with private interpretation. Introduction Can you remember when you first met your closest friend? How quickly did you get a sense of what he or she was like, and of how well you would get on together? Did your impression turn out to be correct, and if not, where and why did you go wrong? Now imagine that instead of meeting another person face to face, you are told about them by someone else.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Psychology Could Legitimately Be Regarded As &Dquo
    Cognitive Social Psychology Melvin Manis1 University of Michigan and Ann Arbor VA Hospital Abstract. Social psychology is presently dominated by cognitive theories that emphasize the importance of personal beliefs and in- tellective processes as the immediate determinants of behavior. The present paper explores two areas of.research within this tra- dition : (1) beliefs about the external world, and (2) beliefs about the self. The paper concludes with a brief critique of the cognitive approach to social psychology. Social psychology, along with many other areas of behavioral research, is presently dominated by the cognitive point of view. In preparing to write this paper it was therefore difficult to determine what aspects of social psychology could legitimately be regarded as &dquo;noncognitive,&dquo; and hence irrelevant to the topic under consideration. I have therefore taken the liberty of covering the field somewhat selectively, so that I might have the opportunity to discuss some continuing themes that strike me as particularly interesting and noteworthy. For the social psychologist, the cognitive approach normally implies an emphasis on personal beliefs and hypotheses as the immediate determin- ants of behavior. Cognitive social psychologists thus focus on what Lewin called the perceived world, including the inferences and illusions that derive from cognitive activity, as opposed to the unembellished objective reality of the physical and social environment. The dominant tone empha- sizes man’s rational, intellective processes and downgrades the importance of irrational and self-serving motives, mindless conformity, and mechan- istic learning principles. Thus, a number of investigators have recently focussed attention on the number and the quality of the arguments that support a given position, as important determinants of attitude change (Burnstein & Vinokur, 1977; Eagly, 1974; Greenwald, 1968).
    [Show full text]
  • STEVEN J. STROESSNER Curriculum Vitae January, 2019 PERSONAL
    STEVEN J. STROESSNER Curriculum Vitae January, 2019 PERSONAL DATA Current Rank: Professor of Psychology, Barnard College Office Address: Department of Psychology Barnard College, Columbia University 3009 Broadway New York, NY 10027-6598 Contact: [email protected] 718.637.7696 DEGREES IN HIGHER EDUCATION Ph.D. 1992, University of California, Santa Barbara, Social/Personality Psychology B.A. 1987, Hope College, Psychology and Psychology/Sociology, Magna Cum Laude PROFESSIONAL EXPERIENCE IN HIGHER EDUCATION Professor, Department of Communication, UCLA, 2020- Professor, Department of Psychology, Barnard College, 2007-2020 Senior Research Scientist, Disney Research, 2015-2017 Ann Whitney Olin Professor of Psychology, Barnard College, 2012-2017 Chair, Department of Psychology, Barnard College, 2004-2007 Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, Barnard College, 2001-2007 Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, Barnard College, 1992-2001 ACADEMIC AND PROFESSIONAL HONORS Elected Fellow, Society of Experimental Social Psychology, 2009 Elected Fellow, Association for Psychological Science, 2007 Elected member of Society for Experimental Social Psychology, 1997 Recipient, Gladys Brooks Faculty Excellence in Teaching Award, 1996 Jacob K. Javits National Graduate Student Fellowship, 1987-91 Social Sciences Dissertation Award, University of California, Santa Barbara, 1991 Elected member of Phi Beta Kappa, 1987 Sigma Xi Award for Excellence in Research, 1987 AFS International/Intercultural Scholar to the Republic of South Africa, 1982
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Familiarity and Time on Social Perception Clarissa Arms Chavez University of Texas at El Paso, [email protected]
    University of Texas at El Paso DigitalCommons@UTEP Open Access Theses & Dissertations 2009-01-01 Getting to know you: The effects of familiarity and time on social perception Clarissa Arms Chavez University of Texas at El Paso, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.utep.edu/open_etd Part of the Cognitive Psychology Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Chavez, Clarissa Arms, "Getting to know you: The effects of familiarity and time on social perception" (2009). Open Access Theses & Dissertations. 227. https://digitalcommons.utep.edu/open_etd/227 This is brought to you for free and open access by DigitalCommons@UTEP. It has been accepted for inclusion in Open Access Theses & Dissertations by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@UTEP. For more information, please contact [email protected]. GETTING TO KNOW YOU: THE EFFECTS OF FAMILIARITY AND TIME ON SOCIAL PERCEPTION CLARISSA JAYNE CHAVEZ Department of Psychology APPROVED: ______________________________ Michael Zárate, Ph.D., Chair ______________________________ Harmon Hosch, Ph.D. ______________________________ Christian Meissner, Ph.D. ______________________________ Ana Schwartz, Ph.D. ______________________________ Penelope Espinoza, Ph.D. ____________________________ Patricia D. Witherspoon, Ph.D. Dean of the Graduate School Copyright © By Clarissa Jayne Chavez 2009 Dedicated to my son, Parker Thomas Chavez. This has always been for you. GETTING TO KNOW YOU: THE EFFECTS OF FAMILIARITY AND TIME ON SOCIAL PERCEPTION by CLARISSA JAYNE CHAVEZ, M.A. DISSERTATION Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of The University of Texas at El Paso in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology THE UNIVERSITY OF TEXAS AT EL PASO August 2009 ABSTRACT A new direction in social perception research is developed.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception: a Selective Review Focused on the Event-Related Brain Potential
    W&M ScholarWorks Arts & Sciences Book Chapters Arts and Sciences 2007 Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception: A Selective Review Focused on the Event-Related Brain Potential. Bruce D. Bartholow Cheryl L. Dickter College of William and Mary, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wm.edu/asbookchapters Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons Recommended Citation Bartholow, B. D., & Dickter, C. L. (2007). Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception: A Selective Review Focused on the Event-Related Brain Potential.. E. Harmon-Jones & P. Winkielman (Ed.), Social neuroscience: Integrating biological and psychological explanations of social behavior (pp. 376-400). New York, NY: Guilford Press. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/asbookchapters/46 This Book Chapter is brought to you for free and open access by the Arts and Sciences at W&M ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Arts & Sciences Book Chapters by an authorized administrator of W&M ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. V PERSON PERCEPTION, STEREOTYPING, AND PREJUDICE Copyright © ${Date}. ${Publisher}. All rights reserved. © ${Date}. ${Publisher}. Copyright 17 Social Cognitive Neuroscience of Person Perception A SELECTIVE REVIEW FOCUSED ON THE EVENT-RELATED BRAIN POTENTIAL Bruce D. Bartholow and Cheryl L. Dickter Although social psychologists have long been interested in understanding the cognitive processes underlying social phenomena (e.g., Markus & Zajonc, 1985), their methods for studying them traditionally have been rather limited. Early research in person perception, like most other areas of social-psychological inquiry, relied primarily on verbal reports. Researchers using this approach have devised clever experimental designs to ensure the validity of their conclusions concerning social behavior (see Reis & Judd, 2000).
    [Show full text]