The Introspection Illusion

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Introspection Illusion CHAPTER ONE The Introspection Illusion Emily Pronin Contents 1. Introduction 2 1.1. Self and other 3 1.2. Components of the illusion 4 2. Identifying the Illusion: The Case of Bias 6 2.1. Introspective weighting 7 2.2. Self–other asymmetry 9 2.3. Behavioral disregard 10 2.4. Differential valuation 12 3. The Introspection Illusion in Social Psychology 15 3.1. Judgment and decision making 15 3.2. The self 16 3.3. Attitudes and attitude change 18 3.4. Social influence 20 3.5. Interpersonal interaction 21 3.6. Personal relationships 23 3.7. Stereotyping and prejudice 25 4. Implications for Major Theoretical Concerns 26 4.1. The perspectives of actors and observers 26 4.2. Self enhancement 31 4.3. Psychological distance 36 4.4. Free will 41 5. Roots of the Illusion 44 5.1. Development 44 5.2. Culture 45 5.3. The brain 47 5.4. Further thoughts: Projection and perspective taking 48 6. Applications 49 6.1. Conflict 49 6.2. Persistence of racism, sexism, and inequality 50 6.3. Ethical lapses 51 Department of Psychology and Woodrow Wilson School of Public & International Affairs, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey, USA Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Volume 41 # 2009 Elsevier Inc. ISSN 0065-2601, DOI: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00401-2 All rights reserved. 1 2 Emily Pronin 6.4. Introspective education 52 6.5. Pursuits of self-knowledge and social connection 53 7. Conclusion 55 Acknowledgments 55 References 55 Abstract Introspection involves looking inward into conscious thoughts, feelings, motives, and intentions. Modern social psychological research has raised ques- tions about the value and reliability of information gained via introspection. This chapter concerns people’s heavy weighting of introspective information for making self-assessments. It also concerns a few principles associated with that weighting—that is, that it does not extend to how people treat others’ introspections, that it can lead people to disregard information conveyed by their own (but not others’) behavior, and that it is rooted not only in people’s unique access to their introspections but also in the unique value they place on them. Over-valuing of personal introspections occurs in a variety of domains, including judgment and decision making, personal relationships, and stereo- typing and prejudice. An understanding of it sheds light on theoretical concerns involving the actor–observer bias, self-enhancement, temporal distance effects, and the perception of free will. People’s unique valuing of their introspections likely has deep roots, but this ‘‘introspection illusion’’ also causes problems. It can foster conflict, discrimination, lapses in ethics, and barriers to self- knowledge and social intimacy. Understanding its sources and effects may help alleviate some of those problems. 1. Introduction ‘‘I think therefore I am.’’ In 1637, the most well-known line in the history of modern philosophy was written. Descartes’ claim derived from a basic intuition: If there was one thing in which he could be confident, it was the reality of his own thoughts. Indeed, the capacity for conscious intro- spection is fundamental to human experience and is commonly thought to differentiate humans from other animals. Through introspection, people constantly are aware of the various thoughts, feelings, and motives that reside in their conscious minds. However, important advances in cognitive and social psychology have questioned the degree to which introspection can uncover the sources of our judgments and actions. People, it has been shown, can form impressions of others, pursue goals, adopt attitudes, and regulate their emotions—all without awareness, effort, or intent (e.g., Hassin et al., 2005; Wegner & Bargh, 1998). People’s introspective access to their conscious intentions, emotions, prescient thoughts, and salient attitudes all can mislead them in The Introspection Illusion 3 their efforts at self-understanding (respectively, Epley & Dunning, 2000; Gilbert et al., 1998; Pronin et al., 2006b; Wilson et al., 1993). In short, introspection often is not a valid and reliable method for assessing the self. Nevertheless, people show a persistent and widespread tendency to place heavy weight on introspection when seeking self-understanding. I refer to this tendency as an introspection illusion. The ‘‘introspection’’ part refers to information gained by looking inward, however briefly, to thoughts, feel- ings, motives, intentions, and other mental contents. The ‘‘illusion’’ part refers to some mistaken notions that people have about introspective information and its value. Given the sometimes weak value of introspection, why do people place so much value on the information they obtain from it? In a classic study, college students watched a video interview with a university instructor who had an unfamiliar foreign accent. In one version of the video, his responses were warm and likable; in the other, they were cold and unlikable. Students who saw the warm and likable version later rated the instructor’s accent as more pleasant than did those who saw the other version. However, they were unaware that his likability influenced how they perceived his accent and even confidently claimed that the reverse had occurred—that his accent made them like (or dislike) him. This experiment by Nisbett and Wilson (1977a) demonstrates something more than the unreliability of introspection. It also suggests people’s false confidence in its reliability. One reason for that confidence likely involves the wealth of introspec- tive information available to people. When individuals are inundated with information about their thoughts, feelings, and intentions, they may naturally come to view those introspections as an authentic source of self-knowledge (Wilson, 2002). Another important reason for people’s confidence in their introspections likely involves the felt directness of introspective information. William James described people’s experience of their thoughts as having ‘‘a warmth and intimacy about them of which [others’] are completely devoid’’ ( James, 1890, p. 314). This fact may lead people to value their own introspections more than those of other people because, of course, people cannot experience others’ thoughts and feelings with the same directness, warmth, and intimacy. Even when one is privileged enough to have access to others’ introspections, such as when those others share their thoughts about a particular judgment, that access is of an indirect sort. Consequently, one may value it less (e.g., ‘‘I know you think his cold personality didn’t affect your perception of him, but I have to weigh that against how harsh you were about his accent.’’). 1.1. Self and other In the case of others, what individuals seem to experience more directly than introspective information is extrospective information—that is, informa- tion gained by looking outward to behavior rather than inward to thoughts 4 Emily Pronin and feelings (Pronin, 2008a). People generally cannot directly perceive their own appearance and actions, and this distinction in visual attention has been shown to influence the attributions people make (Storms, 1973; Taylor & Fiske, 1975). Given the confidence people place in information that arrives at them ‘‘directly’’ (Ross & Ward, 1996), it seems that people may not only differentially attend to introspective versus extrospective information about themselves versus others, but they also may differentially value those sources of information when considering themselves versus others. In proposing such an asymmetry, this theorizing offers a new chapter to social psychology’s unfinished story about the divergent perspectives of actors and observers. Even unfinished, that story has earned the status of a classic—owing in large part to a set of theoretical accounts proposed nearly 40 years ago by Jones and Nisbett (1972) and Bem (1972), with further elaboration by Nisbett and Wilson (1977b). These sometimes seemingly contradictory accounts differ both in the emphasis they place on introspec- tive awareness and in the introspective material they consider. Jones and Nisbett generally were interested in circumstances in which actors have ‘‘more, and more precise’’ introspective information about their internal states than do observers (p. 85). In that context, they theorized about actors’ inclination to form different causal explanations from observers. Bem and also Nisbett and Wilson were interested in circumstances in which actors lack privileged introspective access to mental process. In that context, they emphasized actors’ tendency to rely on the same information and to reach the same conclusions as observers. The current theorizing speaks to circumstances in which actors have rich access to introspective information, such as when introspection provides actors with convincing evidence of their good intentions. Importantly, though, it also speaks to circumstances in which actors lack introspective access, such as when introspection fails to reveal to actors the influence of bias on their own judgments because that bias operates automatically and does not leave conscious traces. In both cases, the introspection illusion involves actors’ placing too much weight on introspection. In the former case, actors place that weight on whatever information is present in intro- spection (e.g., on their good intentions); in the latter case, they place it on the absence of information in introspection (e.g., on their absence of feeling a motive to be biased). In both cases, people
Recommended publications
  • 'Introspectionism' and the Mythical Origins of Scientific Psychology
    Consciousness and Cognition Consciousness and Cognition 15 (2006) 634–654 www.elsevier.com/locate/concog ‘Introspectionism’ and the mythical origins of scientific psychology Alan Costall Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, Hampshire PO1 2DY, UK Received 1 May 2006 Abstract According to the majority of the textbooks, the history of modern, scientific psychology can be tidily encapsulated in the following three stages. Scientific psychology began with a commitment to the study of mind, but based on the method of introspection. Watson rejected introspectionism as both unreliable and effete, and redefined psychology, instead, as the science of behaviour. The cognitive revolution, in turn, replaced the mind as the subject of study, and rejected both behaviourism and a reliance on introspection. This paper argues that all three stages of this history are largely mythical. Introspectionism was never a dominant movement within modern psychology, and the method of introspection never went away. Furthermore, this version of psychology’s history obscures some deep conceptual problems, not least surrounding the modern conception of ‘‘behaviour,’’ that continues to make the scientific study of consciousness seem so weird. Ó 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Introspection; Introspectionism; Behaviourism; Dualism; Watson; Wundt 1. Introduction Probably the most immediate result of the acceptance of the behaviorist’s view will be the elimination of self-observation and of the introspective reports resulting from such a method. (Watson, 1913b, p. 428). The problem of consciousness occupies an analogous position for cognitive psychology as the prob- lem of language behavior does for behaviorism, namely, an unsolved anomaly within the domain of an approach.
    [Show full text]
  • Pet Shop Boys Introspective (Introspectivo) Mp3, Flac, Wma
    Pet Shop Boys Introspective (Introspectivo) mp3, flac, wma DOWNLOAD LINKS (Clickable) Genre: Electronic Album: Introspective (Introspectivo) Country: Ecuador Released: 1988 Style: Synth-pop MP3 version RAR size: 1773 mb FLAC version RAR size: 1773 mb WMA version RAR size: 1819 mb Rating: 4.9 Votes: 443 Other Formats: MP3 MMF ASF ADX AHX AC3 VQF Tracklist Hide Credits Hago Lo Que Se Me Antoja (Left To My Own Devices) A1 Producer – Stephen Lipson, Trevor HornVocals – Sally Bradshaw Quiero Un Perro (I Want A Dog) A2 Mixed By – Frankie Knuckles La Danza Del Dominó (Domino Dancing) A3 Mixed By – Lewis A. Martineé, Mike Couzzi, Rique AlonsoProducer – Lewis A. Martineé No Tengo Miedo (I'm Not Scared) B1 Producer – David Jacob (Siempre En Mi Mente / En Mi Casa) (Always On My Mind / In My House) B2 Producer – Julian MendelsohnWritten-By – Christopher*, James*, Thompson* Está Bien (It's Alright) B3 Producer – Stephen Lipson, Trevor HornWritten-By – Sterling Void Companies, etc. Manufactured By – Ifesa Distributed By – Fonorama Other versions Category Artist Title (Format) Label Category Country Year PCS 7325, 79 Pet Shop Introspective (LP, Parlophone, PCS 7325, 79 UK 1988 0868 1 Boys Album) Parlophone 0868 1 Pet Shop Introspectivo (Cass, 66312 EMI 66312 Argentina 1988 Boys Album, RE) Pet Shop Introspectivo (Cass, 20059 EMI 20059 Argentina 1988 Boys Album) 080 79 0868 Parlophone, 080 79 0868 Pet Shop Introspective (LP, 1, 080 7 EMI, 1, 080 7 Spain Unknown Boys Album) 90868 1 Parlophone, EMI 90868 1 Introspective (CD, Pet Shop Not On Label 13421 Album,
    [Show full text]
  • III. Embodied Cognition A
    +(,121/,1( Citation: 35 Queen's L.J. 117 2009-2010 Content downloaded/printed from HeinOnline (http://heinonline.org) Wed Nov 17 15:51:32 2010 -- Your use of this HeinOnline PDF indicates your acceptance of HeinOnline's Terms and Conditions of the license agreement available at http://heinonline.org/HOL/License -- The search text of this PDF is generated from uncorrected OCR text. -- To obtain permission to use this article beyond the scope of your HeinOnline license, please use: https://www.copyright.com/ccc/basicSearch.do? &operation=go&searchType=0 &lastSearch=simple&all=on&titleOrStdNo=0316-778X Embodied Rationality BarbaraA. Spellman and Simone Schnall* Standard law and economics theory relies on the assumption that human beings act as ideal rationaldecision makers. However, significantpsychological researchhas undermined the view that individuals act completely rationally. The authors detail a recent approach to the human mind known as "embodied cognition", which maintains that mental processes are grounded in actual bodily states. This link between the mind and body is not captured in the standard view of the rationalhuman. Studying the mind in relation to the body can help us better understandand predict seemingly irrationalactions. The authors describe the precursors to the embodied cognition movement, and note that although embodied cognition is similar to earlierapproaches that consideredheuristics, it offers a more complete theory of human behaviour. They use embodied cognition as the basisfor an expanded notion of embodied rationality that goes beyond the domain of affect and actions into the domain ofjudgments. The concept of embodied rationalitycan be applied to reasoning and decision-makingprocesses central to Behavioural Law and Economics.
    [Show full text]
  • Casual and Hardcore Players in HBO's Westworld (2016): the Immoral and Violent Player
    Casual and Hardcore Players in HBO’s Westworld (2016): The Immoral and Violent Player MA Thesis Ellen Menger – 5689295 [email protected] Supervisor: Dr. René Glas Second reader: Dr. Jasper van Vught Utrecht University MA New Media & Digital Culture MCMV10009 - THE-Masterthesis/ MA NMDC May 8, 2017 Abstract This thesis approaches the HBO series Westworld (2016) through the lens of game studies and interprets the series as a commentary on the stereotype of casual and hardcore players and immoral violence in video games. By performing a textual analysis, this thesis explores how Westworld as a series makes use of the casual and hardcore player stereotype, looking at the way the series explores the construction of player categories and how it ties these to the dialogue about violence and immoral behaviour in video games. Keywords: Westworld, games, player types, Western, casual gamer, hardcore gamer, violence, ethics, morals SPOILER WARNING This thesis reveals certain plot twists that may spoil part of the storyline of the first season of HBO’s Westworld. If you have not watched the series, read at your own risk. Table of Contents 1. Introduction ........................................................................................................................ 1 2. Theoretical framework ....................................................................................................... 7 2.1. (Im)moral Behaviour in Game Worlds ....................................................................... 7 2.2. The Player as a Moral
    [Show full text]
  • Flowers for Algernon.Pdf
    SHORT STORY FFlowerslowers fforor AAlgernonlgernon by Daniel Keyes When is knowledge power? When is ignorance bliss? QuickWrite Why might a person hesitate to tell a friend something upsetting? Write down your thoughts. 52 Unit 1 • Collection 1 SKILLS FOCUS Literary Skills Understand subplots and Reader/Writer parallel episodes. Reading Skills Track story events. Notebook Use your RWN to complete the activities for this selection. Vocabulary Subplots and Parallel Episodes A long short story, like the misled (mihs LEHD) v.: fooled; led to believe one that follows, sometimes has a complex plot, a plot that con- something wrong. Joe and Frank misled sists of intertwined stories. A complex plot may include Charlie into believing they were his friends. • subplots—less important plots that are part of the larger story regression (rih GREHSH uhn) n.: return to an earlier or less advanced condition. • parallel episodes—deliberately repeated plot events After its regression, the mouse could no As you read “Flowers for Algernon,” watch for new settings, charac- longer fi nd its way through a maze. ters, or confl icts that are introduced into the story. These may sig- obscure (uhb SKYOOR) v.: hide. He wanted nal that a subplot is beginning. To identify parallel episodes, take to obscure the fact that he was losing his note of similar situations or events that occur in the story. intelligence. Literary Perspectives Apply the literary perspective described deterioration (dih tihr ee uh RAY shuhn) on page 55 as you read this story. n. used as an adj: worsening; declining. Charlie could predict mental deterioration syndromes by using his formula.
    [Show full text]
  • EDUCATION in CHINA a Snapshot This Work Is Published Under the Responsibility of the Secretary-General of the OECD
    EDUCATION IN CHINA A Snapshot This work is published under the responsibility of the Secretary-General of the OECD. The opinions expressed and arguments employed herein do not necessarily reflect the official views of OECD member countries. This document and any map included herein are without prejudice to the status of or sovereignty over any territory, to the delimitation of international frontiers and boundaries and to the name of any territory, city or area. Photo credits: Cover: © EQRoy / Shutterstock.com; © iStock.com/iPandastudio; © astudio / Shutterstock.com Inside: © iStock.com/iPandastudio; © li jianbing / Shutterstock.com; © tangxn / Shutterstock.com; © chuyuss / Shutterstock.com; © astudio / Shutterstock.com; © Frame China / Shutterstock.com © OECD 2016 You can copy, download or print OECD content for your own use, and you can include excerpts from OECD publications, databases and multimedia products in your own documents, presentations, blogs, websites and teaching materials, provided that suitable acknowledgement of OECD as source and copyright owner is given. All requests for public or commercial use and translation rights should be submitted to [email protected]. Requests for permission to photocopy portions of this material for public or commercial use shall be addressed directly to the Copyright Clearance Center (CCC) at [email protected] or the Centre français d’exploitation du droit de copie (CFC) at [email protected]. Education in China A SNAPSHOT Foreword In 2015, three economies in China participated in the OECD Programme for International Student Assessment, or PISA, for the first time: Beijing, a municipality, Jiangsu, a province on the eastern coast of the country, and Guangdong, a southern coastal province.
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: a REVIEW and EVALUATION of an OPEN 1
    Running Head: A REVIEW AND EVALUATION OF AN OPEN 1 A Review and Evaluation of an Open-Source Mood Assessment, the Brief Mood Introspection Scale Rachael M. Cavallaro University of New Hampshire Victoria Bryan University of New Hampshire John D. Mayer University of New Hampshire A REVIEW AND EVALUATION OF AN OPEN 2 Abstract The Brief Mood Introspection Scale (BMIS) is an open-source, 16-item assessment of current mood regularly used in psychological research. The scale has two sets of scales: one pair of scales assesses mood using Pleasant-Unpleasant and Arousal-Calm dimensions; the other (less used alternative) assesses mood using Positive and Negative Affect. However, since its publication in 1988, there have been no systematic reports of its usage pattern, norms or of evidence for its validity. The present meta-analysis aims to (1) identify the nature of the studies that employed the BMIS (i.e. research areas and design), (2) establish the norms for the BMIS the scale means, standard deviations, and reliabilities, and (3) evaluate for the BMIS’s validity. One hundred and fifty studies that utilized the BMIS over a five-year timespan (2016-2011) were identified, including 27 studies that reported information pertinent to the scale’s norms. Our findings indicated that BMIS was primarily used in experimental research, and that its norms were similar to those from the original report. We make recommendations for use of Likert response scales and scoring conventions. A more qualitative review suggested that the Pleasant- Unpleasant mood scale had considerable evidence for its validity from its relation to criteria.
    [Show full text]
  • Straight from the Source — What Works
    Straight from the Source What Works for First-Generation College Students THE PELL INSTITUTE FOR THE STUDY OF OPPORTUNITY IN HIGHER EDUCATION The Pell Institute for the Study of Opportunity in Higher Education The Pell Institute, sponsored by Senior Scholars the Council for Opportunity Adolfo Bermeo in Education, conducts and Marshall Grigsby disseminates research and Thomas Mortenson policy analysis to encourage Lana Muraskin policymakers, educators, and the Congressman Louis Stokes public to improve educational Vincent Tinto opportunities and outcomes of Wayne Upshaw low-income, first-generation, and disabled college students. Advisory Committee The Pell Institute is the first Sonya Anderson, research institute to specifically The Oprah Winfrey Foundation examine the issues affecting educational opportunity for Estela Bensimon, University of Southern this growing population. California For further information contact: Betsy Brand, American Youth Colleen O’Brien, Director Policy Forum THE PELL INSTITUTE Alberto Cabrera, for the Study of Opportunity University of Maryland, in Higher Education College Park 1025 Vermont Avenue, NW Heather Eggins, Suite 1020 Staffordshire University (UK) Washington, DC 20005 David Evans, Tel: 202-638-2887 Educational Policy Consultant Fax: 202-638-3808 Donald Heller, www.pellinstitute.org Pennsylvania State University Scott Miller, Pennsylvania Higher Education Assistance Agency Barmak Nassirian, American Association of Collegiate Registrars and Admissions Officers Raymund Paredes, Texas Higher Education Coordinating Board Thomas Wolanin, Institute for Higher Education Policy Straight from the Source What Works for First-Generation College Students DECEMBER 2006 THE PELL INSTITUTE FOR THE STUDY OF OPPORTUNITY IN HIGHER EDUCATION by Jennifer Engle, Adolfo Bermeo, and Colleen O’Brien Acknowledgements This report was written by Jennifer Engle, Research Analyst; Adolfo Bermeo, Senior Scholar; and Colleen O’Brien, Director, at the Pell Institute.
    [Show full text]
  • Power and Illusory Control
    Research Paper No. 2009 Illusory Control: A Generative Force Behind Power’s Far-Reaching Effects Nathanael J. Fast Deborah H. Gruenfeld Niro Sivanathan Adam D. Galinsky December 2008 R ESEARCH P APER S ERIES Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1314952 Running Head: POWER AND ILLUSORY CONTROL Illusory Control: A Generative Force Behind Power’s Far-Reaching Effects Nathanael J. Fast and Deborah H Gruenfeld Stanford University Niro Sivanathan London Business School Adam D. Galinsky Northwestern University *In press at Psychological Science Please address correspondence concerning this article to Nathanael J. Fast, Department of Organizational Behavior, Stanford University, 518 Memorial Way, Stanford, CA 94305; email: [email protected]. Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1314952 Power and Illusory Control 2 Abstract Three experiments demonstrated that the experience of power leads to an illusion of personal control. Regardless of whether power was experientially primed (Experiments 1 and 3) or manipulated through manager-subordinate roles (Experiment 2), it led to perceived control over outcomes that were beyond the reach of the powerholder. Furthermore, this illusory control mediated the influence of power on several self-enhancement and approach-related effects found in the power literature, including optimism (Experiment 2), self-esteem (Experiment 3), and action-orientation (Experiment 3), demonstrating its theoretical importance as a generative cause and driving force behind many of power’s far-reaching effects. A fourth experiment ruled out an alternative explanation: that positive mood, rather than illusory control, is at the root of power’s effects. The discussion considers implications for existing and future research on the psychology of power, perceived control, and positive illusions.
    [Show full text]
  • The Art of Thinking Clearly
    For Sabine The Art of Thinking Clearly Rolf Dobelli www.sceptrebooks.co.uk First published in Great Britain in 2013 by Sceptre An imprint of Hodder & Stoughton An Hachette UK company 1 Copyright © Rolf Dobelli 2013 The right of Rolf Dobelli to be identified as the Author of the Work has been asserted by him in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means without the prior written permission of the publisher, nor be otherwise circulated in any form of binding or cover other than that in which it is published and without a similar condition being imposed on the subsequent purchaser. A CIP catalogue record for this title is available from the British Library. eBook ISBN 978 1 444 75955 6 Hardback ISBN 978 1 444 75954 9 Hodder & Stoughton Ltd 338 Euston Road London NW1 3BH www.sceptrebooks.co.uk CONTENTS Introduction 1 WHY YOU SHOULD VISIT CEMETERIES: Survivorship Bias 2 DOES HARVARD MAKE YOU SMARTER?: Swimmer’s Body Illusion 3 WHY YOU SEE SHAPES IN THE CLOUDS: Clustering Illusion 4 IF 50 MILLION PEOPLE SAY SOMETHING FOOLISH, IT IS STILL FOOLISH: Social Proof 5 WHY YOU SHOULD FORGET THE PAST: Sunk Cost Fallacy 6 DON’T ACCEPT FREE DRINKS: Reciprocity 7 BEWARE THE ‘SPECIAL CASE’: Confirmation Bias (Part 1) 8 MURDER YOUR DARLINGS: Confirmation Bias (Part 2) 9 DON’T BOW TO AUTHORITY: Authority Bias 10 LEAVE YOUR SUPERMODEL FRIENDS AT HOME: Contrast Effect 11 WHY WE PREFER A WRONG MAP TO NO
    [Show full text]
  • Valuing Thoughts, Ignoring Behavior: the Introspection Illusion As a Source of the Bias Blind Spot ଝ
    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007) 565–578 www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Valuing thoughts, ignoring behavior: The introspection illusion as a source of the bias blind spot ଝ Emily Pronin ¤, Matthew B. Kugler Department of Psychology, Green Hall, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA Received 3 September 2005; revised 16 May 2006 Available online 20 July 2006 Communicated by Fiedler Abstract People see themselves as less susceptible to bias than others. We show that a source of this bias blind spot involves the value that people place, and believe they should place, on introspective information (relative to behavioral information) when assessing bias in themselves versus others. Participants considered introspective information more than behavioral information for assessing bias in themselves, but not others. This divergence did not arise simply from diVerences in introspective access. The blind spot persisted when observers had access to the introspections of the actor whose bias they judged. And, participants claimed that they, but not their peers, should rely on introspections when making self-assessments of bias. Only after being educated about the importance of nonconscious processes in guiding judgment and action—and thereby about the fallibility of introspection—did participants cease denying their relative susceptibility to bias. © 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Bias blind spot; Introspection illusion; Self—other; Nonconscious inXuences; Self-perception In a recent news story, a federal judge defended his ability The tendency to see bias in others, while being blind to it to be objective in handling a case involving a long-time friend. in ourselves, has been shown across a range of cognitive In another story, government scientists defended their impar- and motivational biases (for a review, see Pronin, Gilovich, tiality in evaluating drug companies from whom they received & Ross, 2004).
    [Show full text]
  • “Being an Asian American Male Is Really Hard Actually”: Cultural Psychology of Asian American Masculinities and Psychological Well-Being
    “BEING AN ASIAN AMERICAN MALE IS REALLY HARD ACTUALLY”: CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY OF ASIAN AMERICAN MASCULINITIES AND PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING by Jennifer Young Yim A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (Psychology and Women’s Studies) in The University of Michigan 2009 Doctoral Committee: Associate Professor Ramaswami Mahalingam, Chair Associate Professor Edward C. Chang Associate Professor Anthony S. Chen Associate Professor Elizabeth R. Cole © Jennifer Young Yim 2009 This dissertation is dedicated to my parents, my ancestors, and my teachers. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to offer gratitude and thanks to my mentor, advisor, and committee chair, Dr. Ramaswami Mahalingam, for his unconditional mentorship. I would also like to thank the members of my committee, Drs. Edward Chang, Elizabeth Cole, and Anthony Chen, for their generous support in this project. I also give thanks to the members of the Immigrant Psychology Research Lab for sharing their perspectives on the use of intersectionality and social marginality in different contexts. And I would like to thank the Program on Intergroup Relations for their unflagging support and enthusiasm. I would like to acknowledge the grants that funded this research from the University of Michigan Psychology Department, Women’s Studies Program, and Rackham School of Graduate Studies, which made this project possible. I would also like to acknowledge Apple for allowing me to use iTunes gift cards to promote this study and reach a wider population. Thank you also to Tyler Dancer and Madi Wachman for their assistance with data maintenance and proofreading. And thank you to my students who have all challenged me to think differently about what masculinity means to college students and for pursuing their dreams to be forces of positive change.
    [Show full text]