Friendship, Kinship and the Bases of Social Organisation Martine Guichard, Patrick Heady, Wolde Gossa Tadesse
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Overarching Essays 7 Overarching Essays Friendship, Kinship and the Bases of Social Organisation Martine Guichard, Patrick Heady, Wolde Gossa Tadesse Anthropological Approaches to Friendship In the last two decades social anthropologists have rediscovered friend- ship as a field of study. At the present much more attention is paid to Western societies than to non-Western ones. The latter are often con- sidered as being less friendship-oriented than the former because they correspond with settings, in which kinship continues to be the main set of relationships upon which communities are structured. The regional bias in the anthropology of friendship also reflects the view that friend- ship is a product of modernity. As Silver states, friendship is a relation- ship the expansion of which “requires the very impersonality of admini- stration, contractualism and monetarised exchange over against which it is culturally distinguished” (1989:293). This kind of friendship may be a “luxury” (Paine 1969:508) that seems to be unaffordable in non- Western societies. Such a view seems to be nurtured by the fact that many anthropolo- gists tend to seek for a ‘pure’ and ‘distilled’ essence of friendship that they associate with close friendship. They tend to forget that this kind of relationship is rare, even in the social settings where they themselves come from. At the same time they appear to ignore works such as that of Monica Wilson (1951). In her study devoted to the setting of age- villages of the Tanzanian Nyakyusa, she has stressed that the “enjoy- ment of good company” is a central value that is extended to “the mu- tual aid and sympathy which spring from personal friendship” (p. 66). Wilson also points out that such a relationship is seen as a great source of wisdom and as important element forging character. We clearly need to think more deeply, and less ethnocentrically, about the meanings that can be attached to the idea of friendship. ‘True’ or ‘real’ friendships have frequently been defined as being ex- pressive or emotional and distinguished from instrumental ones. This classical opposition, which has been revitalised by Wolf (1966), is rather one of degree than an absolute one, for all friendships are both expres- sive and instrumental. These two aspects have also been discussed in theories of reciprocity derived from economic anthropology. In this con- text friendship is usually seen as a series of interactions and exchanges involving a more generalised form of reciprocity than the reciprocity observed in a bargaining relationship which is governed by the principle of tit-for-tat. Particularly influential are two streams of exchange theo- 8 Max Planck Institute for Social Anthropology ries which are respectively known as interdependence and equity theo- ries. The advocates of interdependence theories (Thibaut and Kelley 1959; Homans 1961) predict that the rewards must outweigh the costs and that the evaluation of outcomes determines the level of satisfaction in a relationship. On the other hand, they stress that the process of assessing satisfaction involves comparisons with the outcomes in the past, those anticipated in available alternative relationships as well as comparisons with what other people are receiving from this relationship. An exten- sion of this theory is the investment model that focuses on commitment (Rusbult 1980). Here it is argued that commitment within friendship does not only depend on satisfaction, but also on how much one has invested in the relationship. Investments are defined as resources such as time and emotional energy that would be lost if the relationship is ended. Thus, investments increase commitment to a relationship by making it more costly to end it. In contrast to these approaches, equity theories (Hatfield and Traupmann 1981), postulate that satisfaction is a question of fairness. Instead of predicting that we will be satisfied with a relationship as long as its benefits outweigh its costs, equity theorists posit that satisfaction is only felt if we perceive that our outcomes are comparable to those of our partner. All of these theories have been criticised for neglecting the subjec- tively felt meaning of friendship and for failing to take into considera- tion that a friend is someone whose company one enjoys, “someone who understands one and who can explain one to oneself” (Paine 1969: 507). This depiction, idealised as it is, remains an important issue in friendship, at least in those which are labelled as close in many socie- ties. But friendship has many faces in our cultures as well as in others. Not all great friends are persons with whom one can speak about many things, sometimes (e.g. in the case of certain stereotypically benevolent relatives) they are people who one avoids meeting. Accounts of friendship in non-Western societies have been appearing since the thirties. The writings of this period as well as those published in the two following decades focused on somewhat exotic forms of friendship, namely those entered into ritually or ceremonially. These institutionalised friendships include such phenomena as best- friendship, blood-brotherhood, bond-friendship, trading-partnership and co-parenthood, especially those forms of spiritual kinship estab- lished through baptismal rites of the Roman Catholic Church. The first attempts to discuss these institutionalised friendships comparatively were made in the late fifties and in the sixties. Eisenstadt (1956) hy- pothesises that they mostly arise in kinship or caste-dominated societies and are related to “some tensions and strains inherent to these socie- ties” (p. 91). He posits that their function is one of social control and social integration. An alternative view is that the social function of in- Overarching Essays 9 stitutionalised friendships is primarily one of communication (Burridge 1959, Paine 1974). Another early attempt to approach friendships from a comparative point of view was made by Cohen (1961) who proposes a typology of both friendships and societies. By classifying the latter along a conti- nuum of decreasing solidarity and of increasing materialism, he seeks to demonstrate how degrees of solidarity coincide with degrees of com- mitment between friends. Cohen’s model is questionable in many re- spects. For example, one should wonder to what extent the kind of friendship he gives the most importance to in his typology, namely that one which is institutionalised, constitutes a category that is more rele- vant than other forms of friendships. While providing diverse typologies (Wolf 1966, Du Bois 1974), an- thropologists have sought to offer a definition of friendship having cross-cultural validity. These relationships are described in most of the literature as voluntary. But this is mainly true where friendship is weakly institutionalised. Further defining qualities and gradients of friendship are affectivity and instrumentality, intimacy, trust, durability and the dyadic-polyadic element. The Risk of Theoretical Fragmentation There is a deep question here. Are the kinds of friendship described by the various theoretical and empirical accounts really different varieties of the same thing, in which case it would make sense to try to integrate them into a single overall framework? Or are the relationships involved in, say, trading partnerships, compadrazgo, and informal companionship so different that it is not really sensible to seek a theoretical approach that could apply to all of them? Similar questions were debated two or three decades ago in the field of kinship studies, and given a very influential answer by Schneider (1984). He argued that the complex of ideas that members of western societies attach to kinship do not necessarily have anything in common with the ideas in other societies. Indeed, so he claimed, some societies may not conceptualise the relationships involved in conceiving and rearing children as a special social sphere at all. It seems to follow from this that there can be no universal theory of kinship. The consequences of Schneider’s critique were far reaching. The number of research pro- jects that were explicitly concerned with kinship plummeted, at least in America, and topics that would once have been dealt with together in the framework of kinship have often since been studied under separate headings such as personhood, gender, and network analysis. Schnei- der’s critique also reinforced anthropology’s persistent tendency to- wards ethnographic particularism: the reluctance to consider theories and explanatory principles that might apply to all societies. 10 Max Planck Institute for Social Anthropology The variety of kinds of ‘friendship’, when we compare them between and within cultures, is at least as great as the variety of ‘kinship’ sys- tems. Does that mean that friendship studies are doomed to a similar state of theoretical fragmentation, and ethnographic particularism? We believe that the answer to this question is ‘no’, but not because friend- ship as such can be successfully studied as a separate topic. Instead we think that the solution to the theoretical difficulties of both friendship and kinship studies may lie in widening the focus of study to include both. This was the idea behind a workshop on Friendship, Descent and Alliance. New perspectives on social integration and dissociation in changing African socie- ties which was held at MPI for Social Anthropology in December 2002. The conference was organised by Youssouf Diallo, Martine Guichard and Tilo Grätz