Quick viewing(Text Mode)

Deception in Facial Expressions of Pain

Deception in Facial Expressions of Pain

DECEPTION IN EXPRESSIONS OF PAIN:

STRATEGIES TO IMPROVE DETECTION

by

MARILYN LOUISE HILL

B.A. (Honours), 's University, 1989

M.Sc, Memorial University of Newfoundland, 1992

A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF

THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

in

THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES

Department of Psychology

We accept this thesis as conforming

to the required standard

THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA

AUGUST 1996

© Marilyn Louise Hill, 1996 In presenting this thesis in partial fulfilment of the requirements for an advanced degree at the University of British Columbia, I agree that the Library shall make it freely available for reference and study. I further agree that permission for extensive copying of this thesis for scholarly purposes may be granted by the head of my department or by his or her representatives. It is understood that copying or publication of this thesis for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission.

Department

The University of British Columbia Vancouver, Canada

DE-6 (2/88) Abstract

Research suggests that clinicians assign greater weight to nonverbal expression than to patients' self-report when judging the location and severity of their pain. However, it has also been found that pain patients are fairly successful at altering their facial expressions of pain, as their deceptive and genuine pain expressions show few differences in the frequency and intensity of pain-related facial actions. The general aim of the present research was to improve the detection of deceptive pain expressions using both an empirical and a clinical approach. The first study had an empirical focus to pain identification, and provided a more detailed description of genuine and deceptive pain expressions by using a more comprehensive range of facial coding procedures than previous research. A review of research on facial expressions, pain research and deception suggested that facial analyses of genuine and deceptive pain expressions could be expanded to include new variables such as temporal patterns and contiguity of facial actions, as well as an increased focus on facial cues thought to be indicative of lying. Low back patients

(n=40) were videotaped at rest and while undergoing a painful straight leg raise under three sets of instructions: 1) to genuinely express any pain they felt, 2) pretend that it didn't hurt, 3) to fake pain without moving their legs. FACS coding was used to describe and quantify their facial activity and the neutral, genuine pain, masked pain and faked pain expressions were compared on the number, type, intensity, temporal qualities , and contiguity in time of individual facial actions, as well as the incidence of facial actions thought to be indicative of deception. Findings

confirmed the difficulty of discriminating the various facial expressions, but indicated that the

faked pain expressions show a greater number of pain-related and non pain-related actions,

remain at peak intensity longer, and last for a longer period of time than do genuine pain

ii expressions. The difference between masked pain and neutral expressions were more subtle, with masked pain expressions exhibiting a greater frequency of mouth opening and residual movement of the eyebrows.

The second study was more clinical in nature and was designed to see if specific training could improve the detection skills of naive judges by increasing their knowledge and awareness of possible cues to deception. University students (60 male, 60 female) were randomly assigned to one of four experimental groups: l)control, 2) corrective feedback, 3) deception training, and

4) deception training plus feedback. Each subject was shown the videotaped data of the pain patients in all four conditions (neutral expressions, genuine, masked and faked pain expressions).

For each videoclip, the subjects rated the intensity and unpleasantness of the pain experienced by the patient, decided which category each of the four videoclips represented, and described which cues they used to make their decision. There were significant individual differences in accuracy, with accuracy being unrelated to past pain experience, empathy, or the number or type of facial cues used. There was, however, a significant sex difference in judgement accuracy, with females being more accurate than males. Results also showed that immediate corrective feedback led to significant improvements in subjects' detection accuracy, while there was no support for the use of an information-based training program.

iii Table of Contents

Pag Abstract : ii Table of Contents iv List of tables ix Acknowledgement xi

Introduction 1

Literature Review ; 4 Describing genuine and deceptive pain expressions 4 Facial expressions of pain 5 Factors contributing to the discrepancies in pain expression research 5 Type and severity of pain 5 Methodological differences 7 Situational variables 8 Validity and utility of the pain expression 8 Discriminating genuine versus deceptive pain expressions 11 Genuine pain..... 11 Faked pain 12 Masked pain 12 Individual difference variables 14 Summary J. 15 Deception theory and research '. 16 Types of deception 16 Theoretical basis for differences between genuine and deceptive expressions 17 Cues to deception 18 Concealing a genuine expression 18 Falsifying an expression 18 Temporal qualities of facial expressions 19 Temporal contiguity of pain expressions 19 Patterns of facial expression 20

iv Blinking 21 Asymmetry 22 Describing and quantifying facial expressions 25 Increasing the comprehensiveness of FACS coding 25 Detecting pain through facial expression 26 Judging genuine pain using facial expression 27 Detecting genuine versus deceptive pain 28 Detecting deception in others 30 Individual differences in detection accuracy 32 Possible individual difference variables 32 Cue utilization 32 Decision-making confidence 33 Sex differences 34 Empathy 35 Summary 36 Training to improve lie detection 36 Prospects for deception training specific to pain .....37 Summary 40 Study one: A comprehensive description of genuine and deceptive pain expressions ....41

Hypotheses 42 Study two: Effectiveness of training strategies to improve the discrimination of genuine and deceptive pain expressions 43 Hypotheses 43

Study one: Analysis of genuine, faked and masked pain expressions 44

Method 44 Subjects 44 Videotape equipment 44 Coding videotape 44 Measures 45 Personal data sheet 45 Descriptor differential scales 45

v Procedure 45 Videotaping protocol 46 Quantification of facial activity 47 Results 49 Description of patient population 49 Reliability coding 49 Preliminary analyses 50 Frequency 50 Intensity 54 Apex duration 57 Onset duration 58 Facial action duration 58 Asymmetry and pulsating apexes 63 Temporal contiguity of facial actions 63

Study two: training to detect deception 65 Method 65 Subjects 65 Materials 65 Video equipment 65 Videotape 65 Training manual 66 Measures 66 Personal information sheet 66 Interpersonal Reactivity Index 67

Procedure 67 Training conditions 67 Judgement task 68 Results 69 Judgements of pain 69 Sex 69 Training group 69

vi Pain condition 73 Perceived judgement accuracy 74 Decision-making process variables 74 Number of cues used 74 Type of cues used 77 Individual difference variables 85 Pain experience 85 Empathy 86 Comparison of successful and unsuccessful judges 87

Discussion 88 Describing genuine and deceptive pain expressions 88 Genuine pain expressions 88 Faked pain expressions 90 Masked pain expressions 92 Individual differences in facial expressions of pain 93 Temporal qualities of deceptive versus genuine pain expressions 94 Incidence of deception cues in deceptive vs genuine pain expressions 95 Summary 95

Detecting genuine and deceptive pain displays 96 Judgements of intensity and unpleasantness 97 Discrimination of genuine and deceptive pain expressions 99 The effects of training on facial expression judgements 101 Individual differences in judgement accuracy 104 Sex differences 105 Pain experience and empathy 106 Comparison of successful and unsuccessful judges 107 Summary 107 Future directions 108 Limits to generalizability 109

vii References Ill

Appendices Appendix A: FACS action units 118 Appendix B: Research identifying pain-related AU's 119 Appendix C: Study one questionnaire package 121 Appendix D: Consent form for pain patients 124 Appendix E: Physiotherapy protocol 125 Appendix F: Descriptor differential scales 126 Appendix G: Training manual 128 Appendix H: Questionnaire package 142 Appendix I: Videotape description and rating forms 147 Appendix J: Frequency of facial cue use per subject 151 Appendix K: Categorization of facial cues 156 Appendix L: Type and frequency of subjects' pain experiences 158 Appendix M: Correlation matrix 159

viii List of Tables Table Page 1 Results of MANOVA and Followup ANOVA's Examining Frequency of Facial Actions Across Pain Conditions 51

2 Mean Facial Action Frequency Across Pain Conditions 52

3 Results of MANOVA and Followup ANOVA's Examining

Intensity of Facial Actions Across Pain Conditions 55

4 Mean Facial Action Intensity Across Pain Condition 56

5 Results of MANOVA and Followup ANOVA's Examining

Apex Duration of Facial Actions Across Pain Conditions 59

6 Mean Facial Action Apex Duration Across Pain Condition 60

7 Results of MANOVA and Followup ANOVA's Examining

the Duration of Facial Actions Across Pain Conditions 61

8 Mean Facial Action Duration Across Pain Condition 62

9 Results of MANOVA on the Effects of Sex, Training and Pain Condition on Judgements of Pain 70 10 Mean Accuracy and Decision-Making Confidence Across Training Group and Pain Condition 71

11 Mean Ratings of Pain Intensity and Unpleasantness Across Training group and Pain Condition 72

ix Results of MANOVA on the Effects of Sex, Training and Pain Condition on Number of Cues Used

Mean Number of Facial Cues Detected Across Training Groups and Pain Condition

Results of MANOVA on the Effects of Training Group on Individual Cue Use

Results of MANOVA on the Effects of Pain Condition on Individual Cue Use

Mean Individual Cue Use Across Pain Condition

Results of MANOVA on the Effects of Training Group on the Use of Cue Type

Mean Cue Type Use Across Training Group

x Acknowledgement

I would like to take this opportunity to thank the many individuals who were involved with this project in some capacity over the years. First, a sincere thanks to my research supervisor, Dr. Ken Craig, for both his creative input and his impressive editorial skills. His financial contributions must also be acknowledged, as he supported my dissertation research with funds provided by his SSHRC grant,as well as providing me with RA funding in times of dire financial need. Within the research lab itself, many individuals provided research assistanceship, including Neda Dadmand, Terri Wilmon, and Debbie Sandbrand. Jyotika Reddy deserves a special mention for her capable handling of the data collection with only minimal long-distance supervision. I am also indebted to Bruce McMurtry and Christine Lilley for many things, including the support and humour they provided in response to both graduate school and real-life crises. In addition, I will never be able to thank them enough for their willingness to complete the very tedious and time consuming FACS coding required for this project. A last heartfelt thank-you to my family, for being so supportive and involved, and for not asking (at least not too often) why this degree was taking so long. Finally, I would like to dedicate the final product to my grandfather, Percy Hill, for his interest in my work and for his overwhelming pride in my ambition.

xi 1

INTRODUCTION

Pain is a private experience with complex sensory, affective and evaluative qualities. To properly assess pain and provide effective management, information is needed regarding these often subtle complexities of experience. Access to these subjective events can only be achieved through inference from verbal descriptions, nonverbal behaviours, and situational information.

Although sensitivity to such information, using contemporary measures, can lead to relatively accurate judgements about another's internal pain state (Turk & Melzack, 1993), the ability to engage in voluntary control of verbal and nonverbal pain expressions may diminish the observer's ability to make accurate inferences. It is the purposeful and intentional misrepresentation of the nature of the pain experience that constitutes the focus of these investigations.

The capacity for deception has become an important issue in clinical settings because some patients report pain and disability levels inconsistent with their degree of physical injury. It could be that the patient exaggerates or fakes pain due to perceived personal benefit, e.g., influencing pending litigation or compensation claims. Alternatively, s/he may minimize the report of pain experience due to fear of adverse consequences, e.g., prescription of addictive pain medications.

Based on these examples, and many other possibilities, it is clear that the possibility of deception needs to be considered during the assessment of clinical pain. It is equally clear that discrepancies between physical pathology and behavioural evidence of pain do not justify decisions about deception. To begin with, current diagnostic systems for identifying physical pathology can be quite limited. In addition, research has shown that there is a fairly tenuous relationship between organic damage and reports of pain (Turk & Melzack, 1993).

Where does one begin to look for cues to deception in the range of information currently available? When assessing pain in a clinical population, the clinician may have access to verbal information, including standardized questionnaires, and interviews, and nonverbal information, such as pain behaviours (e.g. limping, guarding movements) and facial expression (Keefe &

Block, 1982; Keefe, Brantley, Manuel & Crisson, 1985). Ekman and Friesen (1969,1974) have shown that observers often assume that nonverbal behaviour is less amenable to deception, which is consistent with the finding that observers assign greater importance to people's nonverbal 2 behaviour when it is discordant with their verbal self-reports of emotion (Craig & Prkachin, 1980;

1983; DePaulo, Rosenthal, Eisenstat, Rogers & Finkelstein, 1978; Jacox, 1980). Research specific to pain supports this finding; naive judges provided with both verbal report and facial activity, considered facial expressions to be the more salient source of information on which to base their judgements (Poole & Craig, 1992). Clinicians also assign greater weight to nonverbal expression than to patient self-reports when judging the location and severity of pain experienced

(Johnson, 1977). Nonverbal cues, including posture, body movements, vocalizations and facial expression, are clearly crucial sources of information about the nature of an individual's pain experience. Compared to other nonverbal cues, facial expression is a rich source of information because of its plasticity and dynamic range (Craig, 1992).

It has been suggested that another advantage of facial expression as a measure of pain is that it is less subject to conscious deception than are verbalizations (Craig & Prkachin, 1980;

1983). However, it is equally clear that facial expressions of pain cannot be considered to be simply innate or reflexive responses. Affect and cognition significantly moderate the experience of pain (Turk, Meichenbaum & Genest, 1983), and research has shown only a modest relationship between the severity of painful stimulation and the individual's behavioural response (Craig,

1986). Research has also found that individuals can inhibit their facial displays of pain when under observation (Kleck et al, 1976). It appears that complete confidence in the genuineness of facial expressions as an immediate source of information about subjective experience is premature.

Facial expressions therefore, do not represent a direct measure of pain intensity, and are amenable to manipulation.

Nevertheless, useful information in facial expressions is available to the clinician and other observers when assessing the credibility of self-report. Recent studies using intensive analyses of facial activity have found that deceptive and genuine pain expressions differ in the frequency and intensity of certain facial actions, although relatively few differences have been found and the differences that exist are quite subtle (Craig, Hyde & Patrick, 1991; Hadjistavropoulos & Craig,

1994). When naive undergraduate observers were asked to judge whether these expressions were genuine or deceptive, accuracy was extremely poor (Poole & Craig, 1992), although use of a 3 forced choice design indicated judges were better than chance and sources of error could be identified (Hadjistavropoulos, Craig, Hadjistavropoulos & Poole, 1996). This research suggests that pain patients can be fairly successful in their attempts to alter their facial expressions of pain.

The general aim of the present research was to improve the detection of deceptive pain expressions using both an empirical and a clinical approach. The first study had an empirical focus to pain identification, and provided a more detailed description of genuine and deceptive pain expressions by using a more comprehensive range of facial coding procedures than previous research. A review of research on facial expression, pain research, and deception suggested that the facial analyses of genuine and deceptive pain expressions could be expanded to include new variables such as the temporal patterns and contiguity of specific units in facial expressions, as well as the occurrence of facial cues thought to be indicative of lying.

The second study was more clinical in nature and was designed to see if specific training could improve the detection skills of naive judges by increasing their knowledge and awareness of possible facial cues to deception. The training programe was based on previous research on facial expression, pain, and deception, and attempted to increase the breadth and acuity of judges' decision making procedures. Subjects receiving each of three different training strategies were compared to one another and to a control group of naive observers in their accuracy of detection of deception. 4

LITERATURE REVIEW

Describing Genuine and Deceptive Pain Expressions

The face has long been believed to represent a source of information about an individual's conscious and unconscious experience. Aristotle, for example, noted that characteristic facial

expressions accompanied the experience of many emotions (Russell, 1994). Since the rise of psychological science, research on the facial expression of emotion has flourished, and several

conflicting theories describing the precise role of facial expression in emotion have appeared

(Buck, 1984; Fridlund, 1991; Izard, 1990; Tomkins, 1962). Similar controversies appeared

regarding the role of facial expression and emotion in the experience of pain (Craig, 1994).

Research investigating the inference of pain and emotion through facial expression shares similar

challenges of assessing subjective experience. The study of pain offers an advantage, however, in that the instigating event for these subjective experiences can often be more clearly identified.

It has been difficult to empirically validate facial expression theories because of the

problematic description and measurement of facial actions which are complex and fleeting. The

research literature contains a long history of measurement schemes, including category checklists

of facial behaviour, verbal descriptions of facial gestalts, photographs of movement within specific

facial areas, and electromyographic measurement (Ekman & Friesen, 1978a). The present study

used one of the best-known systems: Ekman & Friesen's (1978a, 1978b) Facial Action Coding

System (FACS).

The FACS method provides a comprehensive, objective analysis of all possible facial

actions which can be distinguished reliably by observers. An anatomically based system, FACS

provides a fine-grained description of 44 visible facial Action Units (AU's) resulting from

activation of individual or units of facial muscles. Each of the AU's represents a discrete,

minimally distinguishable action of a facial muscle or combination of facial muscles (See Appendix

A). As a result of this detailed focus, FACS provides a comprehensive descriptive approach to

facial activity independent of inference regarding the underlying emotions or meanings of the

facial expression. FACS coding provides a means for investigating a broad range of psychological

states, and lends itself to the present study of pain assessment. 5

Facial Expressions of Pain

While the many pain expression studies using FACS have not agreed on the precise

constellation of facial actions constituting the "prototypical" pain response, there have been many consistent findings. In general, only a small subset of the 44 possible AUs has been found to be

responsive during pain. People experiencing pain have been found to exhibit primarily a lowered brow, raised cheeks, tightened eye lids, a raised upper lip or opened mouth, and closed eyes (Craig

et al., 1991; Prkachin & Mercer, 1989). Specifically, all but one study in the area (Galin & Thorn,

1993) have identified brow lowering and tightening of the eyelids as a response to pain. In addition, the majority of studies identified cheek raising, eyes closed or blinking, upper lip raise, parting of the lips, or jaw dropping as pain-related AU"s. In contrast, horizontal or vertical stretching of the

lips, a wrinkled nose, deepening of the nasolabial fold and drooping eyelids were identified in only a

few studies. A breakdown of the particular facial actions identified in each study can be found in

Appendix B. Craig (1992) noted that variations between studies regarding such factors as the type

of pain experienced, the severity of discomfort, methodological differences, and situational and

individual difference variables could account for at least some of the discrepancies in AUs described

above.

Factors Contributing to the Discrepancies in Pain Expression Research

Type and Severity of Pain. Differences between studies on facial expressions associated

with pain could be due to the pain stimulus used. For example, experimentally induced pain studies

often expose university students to novel pain stimuli such as the cold pressor test (Craig & Patrick,

1985) , pressure algometry (Prkachin & Mercer, 1989), or electric shock (Patrick, Craig & Prkachin,

1986) . Subjects in these studies may have little prior experience with the specific type of pain

stimulus. In contrast, clinical pain studies often use patients suffering from varying types of acute

and chronic pain such as low back pain (Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994), shoulder pain (Prkachin

& Mercer, 1989; Prkachin, Solomon, Hwang & Mercer, 1995), cancer pain (Wilkie, 1995), and

temporomandibular disorder pain (LeResche & Dworkin, 1988). The pain stimulus in this case is

often an exacerbation of the acute or chronic pain instigated by medical procedures. For the pain

patient population then, there may have been a fairly lengthy experience with clinical pain and, 6

perhaps more importantly, the pain stimulus itself is likely to have been familiar. As a result, the

clinical pain stimulus probably elicits sensory, cognitive and affective responses that are distinctly

different from responses to the novel, acute pain induced during experimental pain studies. While there has been some consistency across studies, the variability in findings dictates careful delineation

of the parameters of pain-inducing stimuli, as these differences may be reflected in the facial

expression of pain exhibited by experimental and clinical subjects.

Prkachin and Mercer (1989) have provided evidence for the differential influence of pain-

inducing stimuli on pain experience with their comparison of clinical and experimental pain using

healthy volunteer and patient populations. In the clinical population, some facial expressions were

related to patient ratings of the intensity and unpleasantness of the clinical pain stimulus, yet no

relationship was found between facial expression and similar ratings of experimental pain in either

the clinical or experimental subjects (Prkachin & Mercer, 1989). These results contrast with those

of Patrick et al. (1986) who reported a modest correlation between the magnitude of facial activity

and the intensity of the pain stimulus during induced pain in healthy volunteers. Prkachin and

Mercer (1989) also showed that while the facial expressions induced by experimental pain and

clinical pain shared the features of brow lowering and eye closing, the clinical pain expression

contained a greater variety of facial movements. These differences between clinical and

experimental pain expressions may be a reflection of a less complex reaction to the experimental

pain stimulus, due to a lack of previous experience and thus, a different meaning attributed to the

pain experience

Pain expressions of patients may vary in response to various types of clinical pain.

LeResche (1982) examined individuals undergoing severe acute rather than chronic pain and found

such facial actions as horizontal and vertical lip stretches and nasolabial fold deepening were

extremely common, although they occur only rarely in studies of chronic pain patients. It is

noteworthy that both vertical and horizontal lip stretches have been established as part of the

prototypical fear expression. This suggests that the acute pain stimuli in LeResche's photographs

might reasonably have elicited fear as well as pain. Likewise, experimental studies of pain

expressions may differ depending on the qualities of the pain stimulus used. Patrick et al. (1986) 7 found relatively few facial actions were exhibited in response to an acute electric shock, whereas

Craig and Patrick's (1985) cold pressor pain study found a wide assortment of AU's.

Methodological differences. Some of the differences in pain expressions may be due to specific methodological differences among studies. A comparison of Galin and Thorn's (1993) results with those of Craig and Patrick (1985) shows that both studies found a greater incidence of lip part during the pain condition than during baseline, but Galin and Thorn (1993) did not find the increased incidence of several other AU's found by Craig and Patrick (1985). These discrepant results may have resulted simply from the time segment chosen for FACS coding. Whereas Craig

and Patrick (1985) measured facial activity during the first 10 seconds of cold pressor exposure (as well as 40 - 50 seconds after onset, 10 seconds prior to the highest pain rating, and the final 10

seconds of cold pressor exposure), Galin and Thorn (1993) measured only during the final 20

seconds of cold-pressor pain. They assumed that there would be an increase in facial activity with increasing cold pressor exposure, and that there was a need to eliminate any non-pain expressions concomitant with pain expression such as a startle response or orienting behaviour. Craig and

Patrick's (1985) findings indicate that facial activity seems most salient at the onset of exposure, a finding which fits with current pain theories.

Pain theories focus on the interplay between sensory, cognitive and emotional dimensions of

the painful experience. Rather than considering the complex reactions immediately following pain

onset as a confound of the precise measurement of pain, this time period should be considered a

fuller, richer display of the many factors which comprise the experience of pain: methodology which

ignores this interval is missing important information about pain. By examining this initial period,

Craig and Patrick (1985) found AU's most often seen in a surprise or startle expression occurring

most frequently immediately following onset of the pain stimulus. It is clear that the available

research on facial expressions of pain should be evaluated with respect to how particular coding

methodologies were used. Unfortunately, FACS coding is such a well-documented and commonly

used method of facial coding that past studies have often simply stated that FACS coding has been

used, without specifically describing the way that FACS coding has been implemented. 8

Finally, since by definition pain is a complex experience comprised of sensory, cognitive and emotional components, the role and relative contributions of these factors may change radically over the course of the pain stimulation. For example, following the initial pain stimulus, factors

such as the individual's past experience with the pain, coping responses and habituation to the pain

stimulus may lead to marked changes in both the experience and the expression of pain over time.

Research on the timing and pattern of responses to pain in relation to facial expression is clearly needed.

Situational variables. Some of the variation between findings on pain expression may be due to situational factors. For example, research has shown that exposure to social models with higher tolerance to pain will diminish subjects' nonverbal displays of pain (Prkachin et al., 1983; Prkachin

& Craig, 1985). The presence of facial actions which are not pain-related may also be a result of the social situation in which the pain stimulus occurred; it has been suggested that pulling of the lip

corners plus cheek raising may be an indication of embarrassed smiling (Ekman & Friesen, 1982;

LeResche, Ehrlick & Dworkin, 1990). At least three studies reported these individual actions

during facial responses to pain, most likely as a result of being watched or videotaped. (Craig et al.,

1991; Craig & Patrick, 1985; Swalm & Craig, 1991). In addition, LeResche, Ehrlich & Dworkin

(1990) have shown an increase in masking smiles versus genuine smiles during experimental pain but not during clinical pain.

Validity and utility of the pain expression

Despite the inconsistencies and complications found in the research reviewed above, the

concept of a "prototypical" facial expression of pain seems to be both valid and clinically useful,

although what one is likely to observe is a variation in a basic core set of actions along with a

limited range of other actions, an expression that perhaps is better characterized as "fuzzy" than

invariant. Research using both experimental and clinical pain have isolated a parsimonious set of

core facial actions which follow a pairiful stimulus. Evidence suggests that this "core" facial

expression is consistently found in adults experiencing both acute pain and exacerbations of chronic

pain. Prkachin (1992b) has also demonstrated this basic facial expression across four different types

of experimentally induced pain: electric shock, cold, pressure and ischemia. These results suggest 9 that the "core" pain-related facial actions are common to many different pain states. This led

Prkachin (1992b) to suggest that for time-limited experimental and clinical pain stimuli, the assessment of pain via facial expression could be limited to brow lowering, lid tightening / cheek raising, nose wrinkling / lip raising and eye closing, as they provide the bulk of pain-related information. However, there may be other facial actions which are unique to certain pain modalities, arguing for some degree of stimulus specificity. For example, increased bunking rates have been found following pain onset in some studies; the pain stimulus in each case had an abrupt onset, adding a startle component to the pain experience. In addition, clinical pain that is chronic or of higher intensity than that possible during experimental studies may be associated with other facial actions.

In terms of its clinical utility, the magnitude of facial activity has been found to increase with the intensity of noxious stimulation (Prkachin et al., 1983) and to correlate with self reports of pain

severity (Patrick et al., 1986; Prkachin & Mercer, 1989) and unpleasantness (LeResche & Dworkin,

1988; Prkachin & Mercer, 1989). This information on pain magnitude seems to be discriminable by naive observers. Prkachin (1992a) found that the ability to discriminate a painless and a painful

stimuli through facial expression improved reliably and directly with the intensity of the pain stimuli,

suggesting that facial expressions of pain can communicate quantitatively graded pain information.

Despite these findings, the relationship between intensity of noxious events and facial.expression is

not a direct one. For example, in Prkachin's (1992a) study, females responding to a lower pain

intensity than males exhibited more intense facial expressions of pain. Prkachin and Mercer (1989)

have proposed that brow lowering and narrowing of the eyes, signals the probability that the

stimulus intensity has passed some personal threshold of pain. An increase in intensity of these

actions and/or the addition of the other specific facial actions commonly associated with pain would

be recruited as the discomfort approaches and surpasses tolerance levels, providing a mechanism by

which intensity could be coded in the face. Finally, research has shown a relationship between the

intensity and/or duration of some facial actions (brow lowering, cheek raising, lip corner pull and

mouth opening) and self-reports of pain associated disability (Prkachin & Mercer, 1989). 10

Using facial expression as a clinical assessment measure may also provide information that is not available through other channels. Based on the findings that facial expressions of pain are only

moderately correlated with verbal pain measures, pain threshold and pain tolerance levels, several

researchers have concluded that pain expression carries unique information about the pain

experience (LeResche & Dworkin, 1988; Patrick et al., 1986; Prkachin, 1992b; Prkachin & Mercer,

1989).

The facial expressions described above also seem to be relatively specific to pain; research

has not found a relationship between self-report measures of anxiety and depression and the facial

expression of pain (LeResche & Dworkin, 1988), despite consistent findings of a correlation

between anxiety, depression and verbal pain reports (Craig, 1994). In addition, it has been found

that the pain face can be differentiated from several other negative subjective states, such as disgust,

fear, anger and sadness (LeResche, 1982). The facial expression seems to tap fairly specific aspects

of the pain experience, without confounding with other types of psychological distress. However, it

also shares some common elements with other prototypic facial expressions (Ekman & Ffiesen,

1978a), such as brow lowering during fear, sadness and anger, or mouth opening during fear,

disgust and anger, or eyelid tightening during anger. Eye closing or cheek raising do not occur in

any of these prototypes, with the exception of one variant of sadness.

There are two features which discriminate between the constellation of facial actions during

the facial expression of pain and that of other negative emotions. First, there are several AU's

which occur during these other emotions which are not found during pain. For example, fear is

characterized by inner brow raise, outer brow raise, and upper eyelid raise in addition to the brow

lowering and mouth opening; these actions do not occur during pain. Facial actions unique to other

emotions include: 1) inner brow raise, nasolabial deepening and lip corner depression (sadness), 2)

nose wrinkle, lip corner depression, lower lip depressions, and chin raise (disgust), 3) upper lid

raise, upper lip raise, lip funnel, lip tightening, lip press and chin raise (anger) (Ekman & Frieseh,

1978a). Second, although the pain expression shares some common elements with the facial

expressions of other negative states, the pain face represents a unique pattern of facial actions which

is as distinct as any of those previously described for fear, anger, disgust and sadness. 11

Discriminating Genuine versus Deceptive Pain Expressions

It is important to note that the term "genuine" is not meant to represent a "pure" response to a painful stimulus; situational determinants are assumed to influence any pain response. This is a particularly important distinction in light of Fridlund's (1991) view that facial expression reflects contextual demands rather than a direct representation of conscious experience. Genuine pain expressions are defined here as those which are spontaneous or without apparent intention to manipulate the observer; any individual differences are a result of biological variation, situational factors and socialization (familial and cultural). In contrast, during deceptive pain, there is a

deliberate choice to mislead another person. With respect to pain patients, deceptive pain can take two forms, exaggeration / faking or masking / suppression. In some cases, clinicians and others

become concerned that patients are exaggerating or faking their pain for financial,socia l or other benefits. Perhaps less often, but also of considerable importance, are concerns that patients are

trying to conceal their pain. Masking pain may be motivated by issues such as fear of injections,

addiction or side effects due to pain medications (Lander, 1990), fear of unemployment (Kotarba,

1983), or social/interpersonal factors (Craig, Prkachin & Grunau, 1992).

Three studies to date have examined the voluntary control of facial activity during

experimental pain (Galin & Thorn, 1993) and clinical pain (Craig et al., 1991; Hadjistavropoulos &

Craig, 1994). In two of the studies, subjects were videotaped during a neutral "at rest" condition,

and while exhibiting genuine pain in response to a painful stimulus. In addition, they were asked to

mask their pain while undergoing a painful stimulus, and to fake pain without a pain stimulus.

Hadjistavropoulos and Craig (1994) varied the methodology slightly by asking the subjects to

exaggerate their pain rather than faking a nonexistent pain experience. Trained coders then

completed FACS analyses of the baseline, genuine, suppressed and faked (or exaggerated)

expressions of pain to determine if the baseline and the three types of pain expression could be

distinguished based on the frequency of individual facial action units.

Genuine Pain

In both of the studies involving clinical pain, patients suffering from chronic low back pain

performed a specific physiotherapy exercise (straight leg raising) designed to provoke pain by 12 exacerbating their condition (Craig et al., 1991; Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994). The genuine facial expressions of pain exhibited in these two studies were consistent with past research and involved an increased frequency of the following actions: brow lowering, cheek raising, tightening of the eyelids, raising of the upper lip, parting of the lips and eyes closing. In addition,

Hadjistavropoulos and Craig (1994) extended their FACS analyses to include intensity, and found that compared to baseline measures, genuine facial expressions also showed a greater intensity of orbit tightening (cheek raising and eye lid tightening), levator contraction (upper lip raise and nose wrinkle), and mouth opening. The results of Galin and Thorn's (1993) study were distinctly different from the two studies outlined above and past research on genuine pain; subjects' genuine pain expressions differed from the baseline facial expression only in the frequency of Hp corner pull, likely reflecting the substantial differences in their research methodology.

Faked Pain

When asked to fake their pain, subjects in all three studies exhibited the same facial actions as in the genuine pain condition. However, the faked pain expression can be described as a stronger, more "prototypical" or caricatured expression of pain, as all three studies found that brow lowering and cheek raising occurred more often during the faked than during the genuine pain display. In addition, the following facial actions were found to occur more frequently during the faked condition in some studies: closing of the eyes (Craig et al., 1991; Hadjistavropoulos & Craig,

1994), lip corner pull (Craig et al., 1991; Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994), lip stretch

(Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994), and lip tightening (Galin & Thorn, 1993). Evidence was also found to suggest that faked facial expressions showed a reduced frequency of blinking (Craig et al.,

1991). Hadjistavropoulos and Craig's (1994) analysis of facial action intensity also found that faked expressions of pain were characterized by an increased intensity of inner and outer brow raising, brow lowering, eyes narrowing, lip stretch, eyes closing, upper lip raising and nose wrinkling.

Masked Pain

When asked to suppress their pain, people were remarkably successful. FACS analyses indicated that the only facial activity which distinguished masked pain from the baseline facial expression was a reduced rate of blinking (Craig et al., 1991). A closer look at the data suggests 13 that some residual activity remained on the face while the patients were trying to mask pain; masked facial expressions did not differ from either baseline on FACS analysis or the genuine pain

expression (which did differ significantly from baseline) on the frequencyo f the following actions: brow lowering, lids tightening (Craig et al, 1991; Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994), mouth

opening, upper lip raise, nose wrinkling (Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994), and lip corner pull

(Galin & Thorn, 1993). Additional support for the presence of residual facial activity during

masked pain is provided by Poole and Craig's (1992) finding that untrained subjects had little

difficulty identifying pain in the faces of patients masking the display, albeit at levels 50% lower

than the amount of pain attributed to the genuine pain expression.

In essence, the research suggests that certain patterns of facial activity are associated with

attempts to exert voluntary control over the facial activity associated with pain. However, the study

by Galin and Thorn (1993) produced results which were extremely different from those of Craig et

al. (1991) and Hadjistavropoulos and Craig (1994), particularly with respect to their lack of

consensus with the large body of work on genuine pain expressions. There are, however, several

methodological issues which might account for Galin and Thorn's discrepant results, including

differences in pain stimuli, the timing of facial expression measurement, and procedural differences

in FACS coding. While these were noted above, a more detailed analysis follows in order to

highlight improvements in methodology for the present study.

Galin and Thorn (1993) used experimental pain as their pain stimulus rather than the

exacerbation of clinical pain used in the other two studies. This simple difference may account for

many of the differences they found since research has already shown that clinical pain is associated

with a greater variety of facial movements than experimental pain (Prkachin & Mercer, 1989).

However, experimental and clinical pain have been shown to share the key features of brow

lowering and eye closing, and even these features were not replicated in the Galin and Thorn (1993)

study. It seems then, that the nature of the pain stimulus alone is not enough to explain their

findings.

The timing of videotaped segments used for FACS analysis in the various studies may also

have led to some of the discrepancies. Craig et al. (1991) and Hadjistavropoulos and Craig (1994) 14 used a 4 to 6 second interval immediately before and after the instant of maximum facial activity

during the genuine and the faked condition, and the final 4 to 6 seconds of the masked condition. In

contrast, Galin arid Thorn (1993) FACS analysed the final 20 seconds of the genuine, faked and masked cold-pressor procedures; the time periods immediately following the pain stimuli were not

coded. Their choice of segments was based on the assumption that there would be an increase in

facial activity with increasing cold-pressor exposure and that one's immediate reaction to the onset

of a painful stimulus might reflect startle responses or orienting behaviours. However, the work of

Craig and Patrick (1985) has shown that the facial actions decrease in frequency after the first 10

seconds of exposure to experimental pain. Therefore, Galin and Thorn may have missed

considerable information about the genuine pain state and reaction. It also seems likely that there

may be many cues to genuine versus deceptive pain expressions which might appear within the first

few seconds of exposure to the pain stimulus. For example, the immediate impact of a painful event

may be difficult to control, and subjects asked to mask their pain response may allow elements of a

pain expression to "leak" through initially, until they gain control of their expression. Likewise,

genuine, masked and faked pain expressions may differ in the speed of onset.

Finally, slight differences in FACS coding procedures may have resulted in some of the

studies' discrepancies. For example, Craig et al. (1991) found a decreased frequency of blinking in

the deceptive pain conditions, while Galin and Thorn (1993) combined eyes closed and blink into

one facial action variable, and therefore could not investigate the incidence of blinking in isolation.

Individual Difference Variables. Research has shown that there are large individual

differences in the facial actions observed in response to genuine pain. For example, only 45% of

the subjects in one study exhibited one or both of the most common facial actions, brow lowering

and closing of the eyes. Some of these variations have been related to personal dispositions, with

some people being generally impassive, and others very reactive (Craig, 1992). Experimental

studies have shown that subjects with a low pain tolerance exhibited more expressive genuine pain

displays, suggesting that personal pain thresholds also influence nonverbal pain reactions (Galin &

Thorn, 1993). Prkachin and Mercer (1989) found that patients whose pain problem had a greater

psychological and physical impact showed greater facial activity in response to pain. Finally, pain 15 interventions and coping strategies have substantial effects on self-reports of pain, and may have a similar impact on facial expression, although this is an untested proposition.

Similar variables are thought to influence the ability to display deceptive pain expressions.

One such personality variable, role playing ability, influenced the appearance of genuine and deceptive pain displays, such that the intensity of faked pain expressions increased with role- playing ability but was unrelated to the discriminability of the spontaneous and faked pain expressions (Prkachin, 1992a). Similarly, subjects scoring higher on Pilowsky's general hypochondriasis factor of the Illness Behaviour Questionnaire displayed genuine and faked pain expressions which were less intense and harder to discriminate (Prkachin, 1992a). The single study which related pain symptomatology to deception ability discovered that subjects who rated the pain experienced as low intensity were better at masking their pain (Galin & Thorn, 1993). It seems reasonable to suggest that many of the variables found to influence genuine pain expressions would also influence the ability to present deceptive pain expressions.

Summary

Research on the voluntary control of pain expressions has confirmed that subjects are able to pose deceptive pain faces and that several FACS-coded differences in facial actions distinguish between the genuine, faked, masked and no pain displays. The genuine facial expressions of pain

exhibited in the two studies of clinical pain were found to be consistent with past research, involving an increased frequency and/or intensity of brow lowering, cheek raising, tightening of the eyelids,

raising of the upper lip, wrinkling of the nose, parting of the lips, and closing of the eyes. The faked pain expression might be described as a stronger, more "prototypical" or "caricatured" expression of

the genuine pain response, as all of the components of the genuine pain expression have been

observed to occur more frequently and/or more intensely during the faked condition. In addition,

several studies have noted the presence of several non-pain related actions which distinguish faked from genuine pain displays (Craig et al., 1991; Prkachin, 1992a; Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994).

Unlike the faked pain expression, the masked pain expression did not have any uniquely

distinguishing features and is perhaps best described as a mid-point between the neutral baseline

and the genuine pain expression. This may represent residual facial activity which "leaks out" when 16 attempting to neutralize a genuine facial display, a phenomenon described by Ekman and Friesen as a "microexpression" (Ekman & Friesen, 1969). It is noteworthy that Craig et al. (1991) found both types of deceptive pain displays to show decreased rates of blinking, a facial action which the authors postulated to be an indicator of deceptive displays due to the increase in cognitive activity resulting from instructions to fake or mask pain.

These differences are extremely subtle, and would likely be quite difficult for observers to distinguish. There are, however, many other qualities of facial expressions which might conceivably discriminate posed from non-posed reactions. For example, the temporal qualities of a facial display may differ; a genuine facial display may have a shorter latency to onset, a quicker time from onset to peak intensity, and a shorter duration than a deceptive display. A review of the deception literature points to temporal qualities and other facial cues to deception which may be relevant to pain displays.

Deception Theory and Research

According to Ekman (1985), there are few human relationships that do not involve deceit, or at least the possibility of it. Many of these lies are harmless; for example, a "little white lie" meant to protect someone's feelings. Most of us are aware of the commonplace nature of small lies and many people would not want to expose or uncover the truth behind such messages. In contrast, there are many situations in which lying can be legally, financially, and personally harmful. In these situations, uncovering the truth behind the lies becomes extremely important.

The term "lie detection" probably brings to mind criminal activity, political intrigue and the FBI rather than health care. In fact, Ekman's research on deception was originally developed for health care uses; specifically, identifying patients who denied suicidal intentions. The cost of deception in health care settings is high, with "concealers" not receiving the treatment they need and "fakers" getting treatment and tying up unnecessary medical time, money and resources.

Types of Deception

Deception takes many forms arid occurs in many different situations. In general, deception implies that an actor tries to induce a false belief in, or judgement by, a perceiver. As such, deception includes such diverse concepts as practical jokes, criminal fraud, forgery, scams, and 17 white lies. While all of these behaviours share a common goal of deceiving an individual, they may differ from one another in the complexity, degree and duration of the deception, as well as the scope and severity of the intended consequences. As yet, research in the area of deception has not determined how the various types of deception differ in verbal and nonverbal expression.

However, Ekman (1985) has outlined the nonverbal clues that may be present during the two main types of lying: concealment and falsification. In addition, we have general information on the nonverbal behaviour of people who have been asked to lie about their feelings and emotions, or about their reactions to certain situations. Research on emotion has also provided evidence for the difference between genuine and faked facial expressions of emotion (Caccioppo & Petty, 1981;

Ekman, Hager & Friesen, 1981; Ekman & Friesen, 1982).

Theoretical Basis for Differences Between Genuine and Deceptive Facial Expressions

Results from various research areas suggest that genuine and faked facial expressions of emotion may be discriminable (Caccioppo & Petty, 1981; Ekman et al., 1981; Ekman & Friesen,

1982). Neurological research has suggested that there may be separate neural systems responsible for voluntary and involuntary facial movements (Rinn, 1984). The pyramidal system, which descends from cortical areas, seems to play a role in voluntary movements, as lesions in that area lead to deficits in the ability to produce certain facial actions on demand, while the individual retains the ability to exhibit the same expression spontaneously. Involuntary facial movements tend to depend more on the subcortical, extrapyramidal system. Lesions in this area may result in deficits in spontaneous facial expressions of emotion, while leaving unaffected the ability to deliberately produce these movements (Rinn, 1984). These observations may have implications for the detection of deception in facial expression. While it is unclear whether these two systems operate in isolation with, for example, the cortical system only functioning during voluntary facial expression, there is the possibility that cortical mechanisms play a more dominant role in deliberate facial expressions than does the subcortical system. If one assumes that there is separate neural control for genuine and deliberate facial expressions, it is possible that there are identifiable

differences in the topography of genuine and deliberate facial expressions. 18

Cues to Deception

Concealing a Genuine Expression. Deceptive facial expressions may be produced by masking a felt emotion with a deliberately posed neutral facial expression. In order to detect the true pain state, one would have to detect a hypothetical underlying genuine facial expression.

There are several methods by which a genuine facial expression could be "covered up". First, a genuine facial expression may be disguised by tightening antagonistic muscles in the face to

"hold in" the real expression. Alternatively, one could attempt to inhibit any muscle action completely, in order to maintain a neutral facial expression. It has been suggested that either approach may provide clues for deception as the attempt to control muscles may make the face appear stiff, unnatural or controlled. In addition, muscle control is difficult to achieve without some form of leakage of the concealed information (Ekman & Friesen, 1969). This mainly occurs because the face has such a short latency before transmission, with a microexpression of the genuine feeling likely to emerge before there is time to neutralize or mask it properly. These microexpressions, which are so brief as to be barely perceptible, may then be followed by simulated neutral or antithetical expressions (Ekman, 1985).

Falsifying an Expression. A genuine facial expression may also be replaced by another contradictory expression, such as a smile falsified to hide sadness. If one is trying to disguise a negative emotion, it may be very difficult to neutralize the face completely. By posing a positive expression, such as a smile, the deceiver is not only concealing the true emotion but is conveying false information that their emotional state is positive. When this occurs, the posed smile may differ from a genuine smile in some small way, such as the absence of the raised cheeks that give the little crows feet at the edges of the eyes (Ekman & Friesen, 1982; Ekman, Friesen &

O'Sullivan, 1988). However, such subtle cues would be very difficult for the untrained observer to pick up. Another cue to deception in this case could be a display consisting of an unnatural number or type of facial actions. Hager and Ekman (1985) found that a deliberately produced

startle expression contained more facial actions not prototypical of a startle response than during

a genuine startle response. Some of these "extra" facial actions may simply arise because we are

not consciously aware of what a genuine facial expression looks like on our own face. As well, 19 people's feelings about engaging in deception may provide additional cues. For example, some people feel shame or a fear of being caught when lying that may be observed on the face.

Temporal Qualities of Facial Expressions. Ekman and Friesen (1982) suggested that the apex, or the duration of peak intensity, of faked expressions will be longer than during a genuine expression. Indirect evidence for differences in duration between faked and genuine expressions was provided by these authors. The majority (86%) of smiles categorized as "felt" or genuine smiles were found to last between 2/3 of a second and 4 seconds in length, while only 36% of faked smiles fell within this duration range. In addition, they may differ in the peak intensity reached, the timing of apex onset, or in the timing of the initial facial action onset or offset. For example, faked expressions may appear too soon or they may have a delayed onset. Ekman and

Friesen (1982) suggest that faked expressions will have a shorter onset time so that the expression will appear more abruptly than genuine expressions, and that the offset will either be abrupt or irregular. Empirical support for such claims has yet to be gathered.

Many of these assumptions about the timing of deceptive versus genuine facial expressions have been developed through general observation or through anecdotal evidence. Rarely have the temporal qualities of facial expressions in general been investigated; this limitation is particularly true for research on pain expressions. This dissertation will compare genuine pain expressions and two types of deceptive pain expressions (faked pain and masked pain) with respect to their temporal qualities: overall duration, onset duration, offset duration, and apex duration.

Temporal Contiguity of Pain Expressions. By definition, a genuine facial expression consists of a group of individual facial actions which appear together on the face, in a pattern that is associated with some underlying emotional state. As such, temporal contiguity of the component individual facial actions is an important feature of genuine facial expressions (Ekman,

1978a). Evidence for the importance of temporal contiguity on judgements of others' facial expressions has been provided indirectly in pain research. Lee and Craig (1991) had actors portray several pain expressions, manipulating both the number of pain-related AU's and the temporal overlap or non-overlap of the apex intensities of these AU's, Pain ratings by naive observers were found to increase with the number of AU's, but only if temporal overlap of the 20 AU's occurred. Obviously, temporal contiguity serves a very clear communicative function. Theory also suggests that deceptive facial expressions may not have the same degree of temporal contiguity: although they may contain the same component facial actions, the actions may appear serially rather than all at once.

Studies have not yet investigated the degree of temporal contiguity present in genuine pain expressions; however, the relationship among individual facial actions has been investigated. Patrick et al. (1986) and Craig and Patrick (1985) found that pain-related AU's have small, but significant, intercorrelations, ranging from .11 to .33 and .14 to .65 respectively, which suggests that there is substantial independence among them. It is important to note that relatively small intercorrelations among component facial actions found in adult pain expressions to date does not argue against temporal contiguity of genuine pain expressions. Based on the previous review of pain studies, it seems reasonable to assume that these small intercorrelations, as well as some of the differences in facial actions found across pain studies, may have been due to the time segment that was coded. The various facial actions involved in pain reactions may have different temporal qualities. To date, most research has computed correlations among the component facial actions during the entire time segment selected for study in a particular project, a span of time ranging between 4 and 20 seconds. However, since AU's can occur very briefly, in the millisecond range, research which specifically investigates individual facial actions for contiguous temporal occurrence is required. The present study used cluster analysis procedures to compare genuine pain, faked pain and masked pain expressions with respect to the intercorrelations among pain-related AU's which occured within specified time intervals.

Patterns of Facial Expression. The final temporal quality of facial expressions investigated in the present study involves a description of the number and type of facial expression "patterns" exhibited. That is, depending on the nature of the pain stimulus, the individual's reaction may include expressions of pain, fear, startle, anger or sadness . Rather than limiting facial analyses to individual facial action units, or analysis of the temporal contiguity of a single facial expression, one may look for the presence of multiple emotional expressions within the observation period. LeResche and Dworkin's (1988) research found that pain patients often exhibited one expression of negative affect in addition to their pain 21 expression. This second emotional expression was most often contempt, although sadness, disgust, anger and fear were also noted. Likewise, deceptive facial expressions may be composed of an unlikely mix of emotions, as the faked pain face may cover an underlying positive emotion, embarrassment, discomfort or fear of being discovered. Although both pain and deceptive pain are thought to be comprised of a mix of emotions, there may be a difference in the number or type of facial action "patterns" observed.

Rather than looking at the presence of individual facial actions which are not associated with pain, it might be useful to look at the temporal contiguity of any "extra" facial actions which appear with faked pain in order to determine if they form any meaningful emotional expression constellations. With the addition of the temporal analyses mentioned above, FACS analysis of individual facial actions can be grouped into clusters of facial actions occurring together in time.

These clusters can be compared to AU combinations which have been identified for the basic emotions outlined by Ekman and Friesen (1978a). The present research will be the first to attempt a comparison of genuine and deceptive pain responses using cluster analyses and classification of patterns of facial expression.

Blinking. Blinking rates are also thought to change when deception occurs. While some researchers speculate that blinking will increase because of general emotional arousal (Ekman,

1985) or due to a startle response (Prkachin, 1992), others believe that conscious efforts to control emotions will lead to an unconscious decrease in blinking. Research has shown that people blink less often when they are engaged in vigorous cognitive activities (Holland & Tarlow, 1972;

1975). However, looking at the incidence of blinking in deceptive pain states is complicated by the confusion regarding blinking within the genuine pain literature. Studies focusing on changes

in blinking rates during pain have produced evidence to suggest that blinking rates increase

following pain (Craig & Patrick, 1985; Patrick et al., 1986) and decrease following pain (Craig et

al., 1991; Prkachin, 1992b; Prkachin & Mercer, 1989). Prkachin (1992b) noted that increases in

blinking rates during pain tend to occur only when the pain stimulus has an abrupt onset,

therefore producing a startle response. Thus, it seems that decreased blinking may be a

particularly salient cue to deception when the faked pain has a sudden onset, such as with active 22 manipulation of an injured limb. Research by Craig et al. (1991) provided evidence that faked and masked pain were characterized by decreased blinking rates, when compared to both a neutral

face and to a genuine pain condition with an insidious onset. Decreased blinking, then, might be a

cue to pain deception regardless of the pain stimuli involved. The present research will attempt to

replicate these results.

Asymmetry. Past research has suggested that the incidence of asymmetrical facial activity

might differentiate genuine and deceptive facial displays (Ekman, 1981; Hager & Ekman,1985).

Some studies have suggested that right hemispheric specialization should occur since the right

hemisphere, has an important function in emotional processes, and facial expressions are an

integral part of emotion (Ley & Bryden, 1981). Others assume that asymmetry results from right

hemisphere specialization for cognitive, nonverbal processes, such as the recognition of faces

(Benton, 1980; Heller & Levy, 1981). Both views would suggest that asymmetry should consist

of stronger facial action on the left side of the face during spontaneous or genuine emotional

states.

Other theorists have suggested that the lateralization of facial asymmetry will depend on

the emotion being expressed; right hemispheric specialization would occur only for negative

emotions, while left hemispheric specialization would occur for positive emotions (Schwartz,

Ahern & Brown, 1979; Reuter-Lorenz & Davidson, 1981; Sackeim & Gur, 1978). Based on this

theory, positive emotions would show more activity on the right side of the face, and negative

emotions would be stronger on the left side of the face.

Ekman (1980) suggested that asymmetry would be more prevalent in deliberate, requested

facial actions (e.g. nonemotional) than during facial movements related to emotion. Ekman et al.

(1981) found that children asked to imitate facial actions showed stronger facial movement on the

left side of the face. Compared to faked smiles, genuine smiles were found to be more

symmetrical than those performed on request, and "genuine" asymmetries were not lateralized.

However, spontaneous negative emotions weren't elicited, and as asymmetries occur more

frequently during spontaneous negative emotions than during positive emotions, a comparison of

spontaneous and deliberate negative emotions is needed (Ekman et al., 1981). 23

A second study by Hager and Ekman (1985) compared facial asymmetry during deliberate facial movements, spontaneous movements and startle expressions. Genuine facial expressions of

enjoyment and startle were elicited from subjects, after which they were required to simulate

emotional expressions, the startle response and individual facial actions. Results showed that

there were no asymmetry differences between the genuine and simulated startle responses.

However, spontaneous smiles were significantly more symmetrical than were the simulated ones.

Further, the finding that simulated enjoyment expressions were intermediate in asymmetry

between spontaneous happy expressions and deliberate smile action units suggests that the degree

of asymmetry is a function of the degree to which movements are emotional versus deliberate

(Hager & Ekman, 1985). There was no evidence for hemispheric specialization for emotion. In

fact, two of the deliberate facial actions, AU9 (nose wrinkle) and AU15( lip corner depress), most

often associated with negative emotions such as disgust and sadness, were stronger on the right

side of the face, while AU12 (smile), a positive facial action, was lateralized more strongly on the

left side of the face. These results suggest that facial asymmetry occurs during the directed

control of actions, rather than as a function of emotion per se (Hager & Ekman, 1985).

Ekman and Friesen (1979) suggest that lateralization of facial control may depend on the

type of control, inhibition or faking, which was occurring. Ekman et al. (1981) extended this

hypothesis by suggesting that the right hemisphere might be specialized for inhibiting or

modulating emotional expression, which fits with the findings that smiling, which is often feigned

or repressed, shows left laterality. Further research is still needed to determine if the actions

showing left laterality are controlled more often, or in a different manner than rightlateralize d

actions (Hager & Ekman, 1985). A further argument for the need to consider facial actions

individually is that the various facial actions may have different neural innervations (such as the

brow versus the lower face) (Rinn, 1984) and/or the underlying muscles may serve different

functions, such as talking versus emotional expressions, all of which may lead to asymmetry which

differs in intensity or lateralization (Hager & Ekman, 1985).

The asymmetry research has several implications for the present study. First, it seems that

asymmetry may occur more frequently during deliberate facial displays. If so, we should see an 24 increased frequency of asymmetry in both the faked expressions of pain and masked pain expressions in comparison to a genuine pain response. Past research has not included a masked condition, however, and therefore the assumption that asymmetries will occur during this particular type of facial control is speculative. Asymmetries during a masked condition may appear in different facial actions than during a faked condition, may differ in temporal qualities arid may appear on the same or on the opposite side of the face. It is clear that the cues to deception may be very different depending on the type of facial control being attempted.

In summary, based on a review of the deception literature, several cues to deception may be useful in improving our description of deceptive facial expressions of pain. First, faking pain may be exhibited in the face as a mix of emotional expressions. For instance, faked pain hiay be exhibited as a pain face overlying the true emotional state, while masked pain may be either covered by a positive emotional expression, or by a stiff, unnatural neutral face, with "leakage" of the pain expression. Second, faking may occur with the presence of certain emotional expressions or signals which are incongruous or seem out of context, such as shame, guilt, or smiles. Such cues may be difficult to pick up, as pain itself is a complex experience which may include or be influenced by many emotions (Craig, 1994). There is a possibility, however, that certain emotional expressions may appear more often in deceptive versus genuine pain displays. Third, there may be certain cues seen in the face which are associated with deception, such as blushing, sweating, blinking, or asymmetrical actions. Fourth, faked pain expressions may consist of an exaggerated display, with all of the facial action components of genuine pain being displayed with greater intensity. Fifth, certain regions of the face may be crucial in the identification of deceptive pain displays. Research has shown that there may be less differentiated control of the musculature in the upper face than of the lower face, which may explain the finding of "leaked action" around the eyes in masked pain displays. Finally, research has suggested that faked facial expressions may differ with respect to temporal qualities. They may have a delayed onset, a longer duration, or the individual facial actions may appear serially, rather than in a "constellation" typical of genuine facial expressions. 25

Describing and Quantifying Facial Expressions

In order to compare genuine and deceptive pain expressions based on the predictions above, there needs to be a detailed analysis of the numerous facial cues which occur during each condition. The Facial Action Coding System (FACS) developed by Ekman and Friesen (1978a,

1978b) provides a comprehensive, objective analysis of all of the facial actions which are distinguished reliably by observers. Although the FACS system itself is well established and requires highly trained coders to adhere to strict rules regarding the type and degree of facial action required to reach minimal requirements for coding, the system may be implemented in several ways. Coders may simply note the presence of any facial action unit which meets the minimum coding requirements during the coding interval, providing a measure of the frequency of each individual AU. While this is the most economical approach in terms of time and effort, it does not provide important information on either the intensity or the duration of the AU's. In addition, it has been hypothesized that the temporal qualities of facial actions may differentiate between genuine and deceptive expressions, and this information is not derived using the foregoing simple FACS approach. In order to tap this information, the basic FACS coding approach was modified.

Increasing the comprehensiveness of FACS coding

To isolate information on the timing, intensity and duration of facial actions, coding can take one of two forms. The coding interval may be divided into several smaller segments, with coders rating the presence or absence of each AU and their intensity during each segment.

Although this approach allows for the collection of detailed information on the changes in intensity of facial actions over time, it provides only a crude estimate of the timing of facial activity. A further disadvantage of this approach is that it is extremely labour-intensive to code the intensity of each facial action repeatedly during a single coding interval.

Another option is to directly code the times of onset and offset of each occurrence of an individual facial action unit, as well as coding the intensity and duration of the apex. In addition to providing information on the presence or absence of specific AU's, these coding components 26 provide several parameters of facial activity descriptive of the "pattern" of facial activation: 1) response latency (the time from event onset to the time of facial action onset), 2) speed of onset

(the time from facial action onset to the start of the apex), 3) maximum intensity (intensity at the apex), 4) duration of the apex, 5) response duration (the time from facial action onset to the time of facial action offset) and 6) the speed of offset (the time from the end of the apex to facial action offset).

There are several advantages to this approach. It provides a wealth of information on the timing of facial actions and will allow for detailed analyses of temporal patterns during genuine and deceptive facial expressions. Further, it provides a means of investigating the patterns of overlap and timing among individual AU's. Albeit time consuming and rarely used by others for this reason, it is the most comprehensive approach. The one pain study which investigated some of these parameters suggests that apex intensity, onset times and offset times can be identified very reliably by trained FACS coders (LeResche & Dworkin, 1988) The present study is the first to analyse genuine and deceptive pain expressions using the comprehensive FACS coding system.

Detecting Pain Through Facial Expression

Health care practitioners are often concerned with ascertaining the degree of pain experienced by their patients, as are family and friendso f those suffering from acute or chronic pain.

Due to the complex, subjective nature of pain, this can be a very difficult judgement to make. While people may take into account the many sources of information available to them when making decisions about others' experiences of pain, it has been shown that both naive judges and clinicians assign greater weight to nonverbals than to verbal self-report when judging pain (Poole & Craig,

1992). This is most likely due to the commonly held assumption that nonverbal behaviour is less amenable to deception (Ekman & Friesen, 1969, 1974). Craig and Prkachin (1984) argue that nonverbal expression may be less subject to response bias because people appear to monitor it less rigorously than the contents of their verbal report. Based on these arguments, it is clear that it is important to know how accurately people can judge the pain experience of another through facial expression. It is also of interest to determine what types of information observers use when making 27

such judgements, and how successfully they can distinguish genuine from deceptive expressions of

pain.

Judging genuine pain using facial expression

Most people believe that they can distinguish a facial expression of pain and recent research

generally supports this belief. It has been shown that people can discriminate facial expressions of

pain from various emotional states, and are able to identify the individual's degree of suffering based

on facial information (Boucher, 1969; Prkachin & Craig, 1985; von Baeyer, Johnson & McMillan,

1984). Research shows that observers attribute higher pain levels to facial expressions in response

to high intensity shocks than to those expressions following low and medium shock intensities

(Patrick et al., 1986; Prkachin & Craig, 1985; Prkachin et al., 1983). This suggests that expressions

do transmit information on pain intensity which is detectable by naive observers.

Patrick etal. (1986) also provided evidence that observers used specific facial cues to judge

pain levels. In their study, pain-related facial actions accounted for 74% of the variance in observer judgements of pain, an impressively strong relationship when one considers the degree of individual

differences found in expressive pain behaviour. Brow lowering and eye blinking accounted for the

the largest proportion of variance, followed by upper lip raise and cheek raise.

A further findingo f the Patrick et al. (1986) study suggests that observers using facial

expression as the basis of pain judgements are tapping different aspects of the pain reaction than is

provided by self-report alone. Subjects who underwent the pain stimuli were in the presence of

either a pain-tolerant model or a pain-intolerant model throughout the procedure. Results showed

that subjects who were exposed to a pain-tolerant model accepted more intense shocks than other

subjects, without increasing their levels of self-reported discomfort. That is, compared to control

subjects and those receiving shocks in the presence of an intolerant pain model, these subjects' self-

reports were inconsistent with the actual shock intensities they received. Despite these subjects'

lower self-reports of pain, observers rated the pain experience of these subjects higher than the

other subject groups, a result which was more consistent with the actual intensity level of the

noxious stimuli administered, than with the subjects' self-reports of pain. This suggests that the

social modeling manipulation exerted an effect oh the self-report measure of pain but did not affect 28 the nonverbal pain display, hence reflecting different aspects of the pain experience. In contrast, results of a second study found that exposure to a tolerant model led to differences in the magnitude of facial activity (Prkachin et al., 1983). These results suggest that while self-report is probably more plastic and responsive to context, situational variables may also have an impact upon nonverbal expression.

Detecting genuine versus deceptive pain

Two studies that examined the differences between genuine and deceptive pain displays

(Craig et al., 1991; Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994) used the videotaped data in subsequent

studies to investigate observer pain judgements based on these facial expressions

(Hadjistavropoulos et al., 1996; Poole & Craig, 1992). Poole & Craig (1992) had untrained judges rate the severity of pain being experienced by the patients in the Craig et al. (1991) videoclips. In this study, the judges were fooled by the patients' attempts to fake their pain experience and

consistently attributed more pain to the faked pain faces than to the genuine and masked pain faces.

Similarly, judges consistently rated masked pain faces to be lower in pain intensity than genuine pain faces. However, the judges were not completely fooled by the suppressed pain face, as they rated

the suppressed pain face to fall approximately halfway between the genuine and baseline facial

expressions on the pain severity scale. Hence, the judges were able to discern some pain, an

impressive feat considering that the only facial action which statistically discriminated the

suppressed pain condition from the baseline expression was a decreased incidence of blinking.

These results suggest that the judges could identify the residual pain activity which remained on the

face during the masked pain condition.

From the Poole and Craig (1992) study, we can conclude that when untrained observers are

asked to rate the amount of pain experienced by the patient, based on his/her facial expression, their

responses seem to correspond to the magnitude of pain-related facial activity that has been isolated

through FACS coding, regardless of whether that activity level was genuine or deceptive. Both the

empirical analysis and the judgement task determined that facial activity was greatest in the faked

pain condition, followed by the genuine pain condition, the masked pain condition and the neutral

baseline. 29

An additional focus of the Poole and Craig (1992) study was to determine the effect of priming on the observers' judgements of pain. One half of the observers were told that they were

seeing baseline, genuine, faked and masked pain expressions, the remaining observers were told

simply that they were seeing pain and no pain conditions. Results showed that making the

observers aware of the possibility of deception did not increase their ability to estimate the actual

pain experienced. Instead, priming was found to reduce the observers' willingness to attribute pain

to the patients' facial expressions. So, although observers who were primed attributed less pain to the faked faces, they also showed a reduced tendency to attribute pain during other conditions.

This finding fits with past research showing that increasing the base rate expectation of deception

has little effect on the success with which deception is detected (Faust, Hart, Guilmette & Arkes,

1988):

Hadjistavropoulos et al. (1996) asked naive observers to rate the pain experience of the

patients in the Hadjistavropoulos and Craig (1994) videoclips. In addition to being asked to rate

the severity and unpleasantness of the pain experienced in each videoclip, subjects were asked to

determine if the videoclip represented a neutral, genuine pain, masked pain or exaggerated pain

expression using a forced choice format. Unlike the Poole and Craig (1992) study, these subjects

rated the intensity and unpleasantness of the genuine, masked and exaggerated pain to be equal,

and significantly greater than the neutral expression. Similar to Poole and Craig's (1992) priming

effect, the knowledge that patients were performing the same movement in all three conditions

and were exhibiting deceptive displays tempered the trend to assign pain intensity ratings which

correspond to the magnitude of pain-related facial activity.

When asked to categorize the various pain expressions, subjects were 53% accurate in

their discrimination of the spontaneous and deliberate facial displays, which is significantly greater

than the 25% accuracy expected by chance. Unfortunately, the use of a forced choice format in

itself would lead to inflated judgement accuracy, and thus does not provide clearcut evidence

about naive judges' ability to categorize spontaneous and deliberate facial expressions. Unlike

past research, Hadjistavropoulos et al. (1996) found very little variation in judges' ability to 30 identify the various expressions, which suggests that the use of a forced choice design may also obscure individual differences in detection ability.

In general, research has shown that observers perform poorly when asked to discriminate among genuine and deceptive pain displays. Since deception can have serious consequences in

certain situations, it is important and useful to determine if observers' detection accuracy could be improved. In order to answer this question, research is needed to determine: (1) the

characteristics that differentiate which deceptive facial expressions are easier or harder to detect,

(2) the facial cues and detection strategies used by successful "lie detectors" versus unsuccessful

ones and (3) whether various training strategies could improve detection abilities. As research of this type has not been done using pain patients and judgements of pain expressions, a review of

"lie detection" studies in the deception literature provides a starting point for an investigation of variables related to detection abilities.

Detecting Deception in Others

Research in the area of deception has shown that, in general, people are quite poor at "lie

detection", defined here as judgements based on another's behaviour, as opposed to being based

on psychophysiological data. A review of the available research has determined that the average

accuracy of subjects in these studies has rarely exceeded 60%, which is barely above chance levels

when the base rate is 50% (Ekman & O'Sullivan, 1991). However, Ekman and O'Sullivan (1991)

determined that these accuracy levels might have been improved, as subjects in lie detection

studies were often college students who might not have had any reason to learn how to detect

deception. Ekman and O'Sullivan (1991) assumed that people in occupations which require the

detection of deception would show greater accuracy than the standard college student population.

Surprisingly, three earlier studies, which set out to investigate this assumption, did not show any

improvements in accuracy when using police officers, customs officials and federal law

enforcement officers (Kraut & Poe, 1980; DePaulo & Pfeifer, 1986; Kohnken, 1987). However,

one important design flaw present in all three studies was posited to account for the lack of

accuracy by "professional lie catchers". There was no evidence in two of the studies that the

videotaped behaviour differed between the tmthful and deceptive messages. In the third study, 31 the materials did differ between truthful and deceptive communications, but the authors did not relate the detectability of the patients' deceptive messages to the degree of observably "different" behaviour (DePaulo & Pfeifer,1986). It was concluded that the general lack of accuracy and the lack of advantage for professionals found in past studies was due to a lack of information in the videotaped deceptive behaviours (Ekman & O'Sullivan, 1991).

Ekman and O'Sullivan (1991) improved upon studies in the area of lie detection by using videotaped samples of behaviour which were shown to differentiate between truthful and lying messages. Measures of facial activity using FACS showed that people displayed more masking

smiles when they lied and more enjoyment smiles when they told the truth. These two types of smiles are discriminate because facial activity in deceptive smiles is restricted to the mouth region, while felt smiles also involve activity in the eye region. There was also a vocal cue to deception; when subjects lied, there was an increase in fundamental pitch. The combined use of facial and vocal cues correctly classified 86% of the subjects in terms of the tmthfulness of their messages (Ekman, O'Sullivan, Friesen & Scherer, 1991). Ekman and O'Sullivan (1991) selected

subjects from several professional groups expected to have some experience in lie detection: the

U.S. Secret Service, Central Intelligence Agency, Federal Bureau of Investigation, National

Security Agency, Drug Enforcement Agency, police, judges, and psychiatrists. In addition, college students and other adults assumed to be inexperienced in lie detection were tested.

Results of the study showed that most of the subject groups had average accuracy scores ranging from 53-58% . In contrast, the Secret Service agents, the only group to score

significantly above chance (50%) levels, had an average accuracy score of 64%. Splitting the individuals within each group into low (0 to 30%), medium (40 to 60%) and high scorers (70 to

100%) further highlights the differences between the groups. None of the Secret Service agents

scored below chance levels, while all of the remaining groups had members who performed worse

than chance. In addition, 53% of the Secret Service agents scored above 70% accuracy and 29% were above 80% accuracy. The next best group, the psychiatrists, had Only 12% of their subjects

reach 80% accuracy. 32

Ekman and O'Sullivan have provided evidence to suggest that some people are better at detecting deception than others, a result which suggests that there is some skill or "trick" to lie detection that many observers do not possess. However, the state of current research in the lie detection area leaves several questions unanswered, many of which may also apply to the question of how one might best detect deceptive pain expressions. To begin, although Ekman and

O'Sullivan's (1991) study suggests that there are substantial individual differences in lie detection ability, there has been relatively little work done to determine what makes these people perform above average levels. An additional question of interest is whether individuals can be trained to become better lie detectors. The following sections address both of these questions in turn, looking at evidence first within the lie detection literature, and then within pain research.

Individual differences in detection accuracy

Individual differences in lie detection accuracy has been yet another relatively unexplored area in the deception literature. Past studies have focused on group averages when investigating lie detection accuracy, and have concluded that people can detect deception at only slightly better than chance levels. While people as a whole might be poor at detecting deception, there are individuals who are relatively good at it. Ekman and O'Sullivan (1991) found a large range in detection abilities, with accuracy levels ranging from highly successful (greater than 80% accuracy) to performance below chance levels. Although pain research has not investigated the possibility of individual differences in detection abilities, it seems reasonable to assume that some people may be be better than others in their ability to detect deceptive pain expressions. Variables which might influence the ability to detect genuine pain include: the cues used by the detector, past experience with pain patients, and interpersonal sensitivity skills in general.

Possible Individual Difference Variables

Cue Utilization. Ekman and O'Sullivan (1991) made the first attempt to isolate the differences in decision-making which differentiate good and bad lie detectors. All subjects in their study were asked to describe the behavioural cues they used to detect deception both before participating in the study and again on two occasions during the judgement process. No relationship was found between accuracy and cues reported prior to beginning the study. 33

However, the accurate and inaccurate groups were found to use different cues when actually describing how they made their judgements about two individual cases. Those who were good at detecting deception listed more varied behaviours and emphasized nonverbal behaviours more than verbal behaviours. In addition, the ability to recognize microexpressions was correlated with detection accuracy. These results, based on one type of deception (the concealment of strong negative emotions), suggest that successful "lie catchers" use different information than do unsuccessful lie detectors, despite their lack of awareness of these cues at pretest.

As stated previously, pain research has not looked specifically at individual differences in

detection accuracy. We do not know if individuals who are relatively good at detecting deceptive differ in their method of "lie detection" from those who perform worse than chance. Preliminary research has asked judges to rank the relative importance of various facial and body elements to their pain ratings and have found that movements of the eyes, eyebrows, eyelids and the mouth were the most important sources of information (Prkachin et al., 1983). Further information is needed on the specific cues judges use when judging the veracity of others' pain.

Situational cues are also important determinants of pain judgements. Research has shown that observers attribute greater pain to facial expressions when told that the patient is

hypersensitive to pain than when under the impression that the patient had received an analgesic

(Prkachin & Craig, 1985). This study show that situational information which is independent of the pain expression and the actual pain experience of the patient, can bias pain judgements. Based on this research, we may assume that there are various situational constraints which might influence observers' judgements of pain, such as information that compensation is pending, that the self-report of pain is incongruent with the degree of observable tissue damage, etc.

Decision-Making Confidence. There is little evidence for a relationship between subjects'

confidence about their detection ability and actual detection accuracy. Some studies found that

subjects' confidence in their abilities was not correlated to their subsequent performance (DePaulo

& Pfeifer, 1986; Kohnken, 1987). However, this research has simply correlated overall accuracy

with the subjects' retrospective rating of their general performance. As subjects presumably

predict some "deceivers" better than others, relating overall accuracy and confidence measures 34 makes little sense; research needs to look at the relationship between subject's accuracy on individual cases with their rated confidence in each decision.

Sex Differences. Differences in ability to detect deception related to sex have not been consistently found. However, these results should be interpreted with caution, as the majority of

studies have looked solely at overall accuracy of detection. In order to investigate sex differences thoroughly, one should not only look for sex differences in accuracy, but should also use an individual differences approach to compare the number of highly accurate individuals belonging to each sex, as well as overall group effects. Also, sex differences may show up in the process of detection rather than in the overall accuracy.

Hurd and Noller (1988) have done the most thorough investigation of sex differences in deception decoding. They investigated not only sex differences in accuracy but also looked at process variables, such as awareness of the possibility of deception, confidence, cue utilization and response time. In this study, females were found to use more cues, to have a longer latency to response time, and to be judged as less confident by raters who coded their decision making process. The finding that females were more likely to mention the use of cues contradicts Buck's

(1984) research which concludes that females use a perception-based decoding strategy versus males who use an analytic cognitively based decoding strategy. It would seem from Hurd and

Noller (1988) that men use an intuitive coding strategy more often, resulting in quicker, more confident decisions based less on cues. In contrast, females used a slower, more cue-based process, which may suggest an analytic approach. The alternative explanation for these sex

differences is, of course, that males simply are not reporting their use of cues. Accuracy scores

were completely unrelated to any of the process variables measured in this study. However,

accuracy scores in general were very low and this lack of variability alone could explain the low

correlations.

Although past research on the ability to detect deceptive facial expressions of pain has not

found sex differences, the present study provided a more thorough investigation of sex

differences. Males and females were compared for overall accuracy, number and type of cues

used, effectiveness of feedback and/or training, and decision-making confidence. 35

Empathy. While research in the area of deception has not investigated the relationship between empathy and the ability to detect deception, there are several reasons to suggest that individuals high in empathy may be better "lie detectors". Currently held definitions of empathy

stress not only an emotional identification with others but also an increased social acuity

(Chlopan, McCain, Carbonell & Hagen, 1985). That is, empathy seems to be composed of both emotional arousability and a more accurate person perception. Intuitively, it seems likely that

person perception would be related to accuracy in judging others, while the emotional arousability component might be more highly related to the probability of actual helping behaviour.

Past attempts to relate empathy to actual skills, such as judgements of others or helping behaviour, have been complicated by the lack of consensus across studies on the precise definition

of empathy. Researchers' conceptualizations vary widely, with disagreements on whether

empathy is an active or passive ability and a cognitive or emotional ability (Chlopan et al., 1985).

Empathy has been regarded as both an involuntary vicarious experience of emotion (Clark, 1980;

Mehrabian & Epstein, 1972) and as an active construction of another's mental state (Hogan,

1969). In general, the majority of research has been based on two different definitions of empathy

and their corresponding measurements, The Questionnaire Measure of Emotional Empathy

(Mehrabian & Epstein, 1972) and Hogan's Empathy Scale (EM) respectively (Hogan, 1969).

It is likely that empathy is a much more complicated process than even a combination of

the cognitive or affective approaches. One aspect of empathy, perspective-taking, can be divided

into three distinct categories: perceptual (to predict the visual perspective of another), social-

cognitive (the identification of another's thoughts, intentions, motivations or behaviour), and

affective (the inference of another's feelings, reactions or concerns). Although these components

of empathy are conceptually different, they are probably interrelated and very likely tap

overlapping processes (Moore, 1990). In order to clarify the relationship between empathy and

behaviour, the complex processes involved in an empathic response must be made more explicit.

Davis (1980) has developed a multidimensional measure of empathy, The Interpersonal

Reactivity Index (IRI), which focuses on four separate subscales: Perspective-Taking, Fantasy,

Empathic Concern and Personal Distress. Two of the IRI scales are relevant to the judgement 36 task in the proposed study. Perspective-Taking (PT), the tendency to adopt the psychological point of view of others, has been found to correlate highly with the "cognitive" Hogan measure

(Davis, 1983) and with accuracy in person perception (Bernstein & Davis, 1982). The Empathic

Concern (EC) scale, a measure of other oriented feelings of sympathy and concern, has been found to correlate highly with the "emotional" Mehrabian and Epstein measure and with emotional reactions and helping behaviours towards someone in distress (Davis, 1983). Scores on the Empathic Concern Scale were not found to correlate with accuracy of person perception

(Bernstein & Davis, 1982). For the current study, it is predicted that the PT scale will be

correlated with individuals' ability to detect deception, while the EC scale will not show such a relationship.

Summary

The "lie detection" literature suggests that individuals are not highly skilled at

discriminating between genuine and deceptive messages, although there are indications that some people may be more successful "lie detectors" than others. Several variables have been postulated to be associated with these variations in detection ability, including cue utilization, decision• making confidence, sex differences and empathy. This study examines the relationship between these variables and the ability to detect deceptive pain expressions.

Training to Improve Lie Detection

Several studies on lie detection have shown that naive observers are very poor at detecting

deception; however, it is possible that observers can be trained to improve their detection abilities.

There have been many debates about the feasibility of such training (Kohnken, 1987; Zuckerman,

Koestner & Colella, 1985). Evidence against the utility of training to improve "lie detection"

includes the surprising finding that law enforcement groups and psychologists are no better at

detecting deception than college students (Ekman & O'Sullivan, 1991). In addition, the few

studies that have tried to improve detection through the use of training programs have failed

(Kohnken, 1987; Zuckerman, Koestner & Alton, 1984).

Despite the lack of empirical support, there are several reasons to assume that training is

still a viable approach to improve lie detection accuracy. Lie detection studies published to date 37 show that "training" thus far has meant the provision of corrective feedback to observers. The assumption behind the use of corrective feedback is that subjects who are made aware of their errors will look for alternative strategies to improve their performance. In general, corrective feedback has not been found to improve lie detection, resulting in the conclusion that subjects

simply do not see, or do not use, the facial cues which distinguish deceptive and truthful communications.

In addition, although naive observers have performed no better than chance at lie detection, Ekman and O'Sullivan (1991) have provided evidence to suggest that some individuals perform considerably better than chance, suggesting that the task is possible if a certain approach is used. Further support is provided by the findings of one study which showed that experienced

Facial Affect Scoring Technique (FAST) trained coders were able to correctly classify the honest

and deceptive behaviour of almost all of the subjects (Ekman & Friesen, 1974). This suggests that observers may be trained to pick up the cues which distinguish truthful from deceptive facial behaviour. Although it is not practical to train observers to FACS standards due to the hours of training required, it would be interesting to determine if short-term training programs based on

FACS coding analyses of deception would improve detection accuracy.

Prospects for Deception Training Specific to Pain

A preliminary investigation of the relative merits of corrective feedback versus information-based training to improve discrimination between genuine and deceptive pain displays

has recently been provided (Galin & Thorn, 1993). The authors investigated the effects of two

deception training approaches on judges' accuracies in identifying genuine cold-pressor induced

pain, masked pain, posed pain or no pain. Subjects in the feedback group were given corrective feedback on the first half of their judgements. The training group subjects were given a short

description of the FACS facial action units which had been found to discriminate between the pain

conditions in the stimulus clips. They were then shown photographs portraying these

characteristics and were taught how to reproduce these movements on their own faces. Finally,

subjects in this group were given feedback on the first 10% of the judgements they made. 38

During the pretrial task, the subjects in the four groups did not differ in their detection accuracy and were significantly poorer at identifying genuine pain than the masked, posed and no pain conditions. This pattern of detection accuracy held for control subjects on trial two, as did their overall accuracy levels. However, the feedback group were more accurate in identifying the posed pain condition than the other three pain conditions. In contrast, the training group showed a different pattern, with the no pain and posed pain conditions identified more often than the masked and genuine pain conditions. The identification of genuine pain was below chance levels for all but the feedback group, with the overall accuracy levels for the remaining pain conditions ranging from 31.5% accuracy for the training group's identification of the masked condition to

55.4% accuracy for the feedback group's identification of the posed condition.

Analyzing overall levels of accuracy for each of the pain conditions yielded surprising results. In general, subjects in the feedback group were the most accurate in detecting the genuine and posed pain conditions, while the control group was most accurate at detecting the masked pain condition. The no-pain condition was detected most often by the control and training groups. It is noteworthy that the training group was superior to the control condition only on the posed pain segments.

Several design flaws might have influenced the results of Galin and Thorn's (1993) study.

To begin with, the length of the pretraining task was very long (120 judgements), likely leading to fatigue effects. More importantly, the type of feedback used did not allow for the most effective test of its effectiveness as a training strategy. The feedback used in the study was delayed, providing subjects with the correct categorization for the facial expressions only after all 120 had been completed. Feedback might be more effective if it were given immediately after each decision-making process, at which time subjects would be more aware of the strategy they used to classify each facial expression, and immediate feedback would help to identify the most and least effective strategies. The feedback only and training plus feedback conditions did not utilize the same feedback procedures, therefore not providing a true comparison of the possibility of additive effects. The feedback only group received feedback on all 120 segments, while those in the training plus feedback condition only received feedback on the first 20 segments. In addition to 39 being provided with feedback on fewer judgements, as the segments were in random order, we do not know if subjects in this group were given equal amounts of feedback for all four types of

facial expression.

Additional methodological concerns are specific to the training condition used by Galin

and Thorn (1993). First, training alone was not used as a condition, making it difficult to

determine if any of the improvements shown by the training group were in fact due to training, or

due to the limited feedback training they received. Second, the information contained in the

FAGS based training program was sketchy at best, as it was based solely on the FACS analysis of the stimulus tapes. Unfortunately, contrary to past research^ FACS analysis in this study isolated few differences among genuine and deceptive pain expressions. The largest number of facial cues

differed between genuine and posed pain; therefore the training package would have contained the

most information about this particular pain condition. In fact, the training group were successful

at discriminating only these two conditions. Since the pattern of AU's identified using FACS

coding did not clearly distinguish among the remaining three pain conditions, subjects would not

have been presented with enough information to allow further discrimination. Despite the

authors' conclusions to the contrary, these results clearly provide support for training.

Finally, the individual differences that exist in pain expressions are an important factor to

take into consideration when designing a training program for the detection of deception in such

patients. Galin and Thorn's (1993) training package was based on the AU's which were found to

discriminate between the various pain conditions. They found very few differences in facial

activity between the groups; their training program did not seem to mention individual differences.

No mention was made of the fact that all people do not show the "prototypical" pain face, nor

that deceptive pain expressions do not look the same for all people. Training judges to look

specifically for the prototypes of pain may be detrimental to performance.

Of course, it is difficult to devise a deception training program when we have no clear idea

about what makes an effective perceiver. We know that FACS coding appears to provide some

cues judges could use to facilitate the discrimination between genuine and deceptive expressions,

but FACS training is hardly a practical and cost-effective detection strategy. However, we do 40 have some general knowledge regarding the elements of facial activity which occur frequently during deception. In addition, training subjects to detect faked pain should include information on the typical pain face, as well as the facial elements which may change during masked or faked conditions.

Summary

This review suggests that attempting to train individuals in the detection of deceptive pain might be a worthwhile endeavor. Based on the reliance of corrective feedback as a "training program" in the past, it would be reasonable to compare the efficacy of feedback with that of an information-based training program. Galin and Thorn (1993) attempted to do this, and found that both training and corrective feedback led to very limited improvements in detecting deceptive pain. Unfortunately, they did not provide a clear test of the accuracy of training versus feedback due to the content of their training session, the manner in which feedback was provided, and methodological difficulties. Improvements that could be made upon their work include: (1) providing more information in the training session, including information on the genuine, masked and no pain conditions based on all past research findings, an emphasis on individual differences in pain displays, a focus on the facial expressions thought to be related to deception in general, and consideration of temporal cues; (2) providing feedback following each judgement, in order to highlight the errors being made and to allow the opportunity to improve their decision-making process; and (3) accurately assessing the possibility of additive effects when training and feedback are combined by having a third training group which receives the full training package as well as the complete feedback procedure. 41

Study One: A comprehensive description of genuine and deceptive facial expressions of pain

The general purpose of the first study is to extend the description of genuine and

deceptive facial expressions of pain by investigating the possibility that the deceptive pain states

differ in the frequency with which deception cues occur. The study will use a clinical population

of low back pain patients who are undergoing physiotherapy for their injuries. Patients suffering from low back pain were chosen as the focus of this study due to the high prevalence rates and

social impact of the condition. Back pain is one of the most frequently reported types of pain in the general population, with lifetime estimates in the North American population of 80%, and a

yearly incidence of 4% (Crook, Rideout & Brown, 1984; Mayer et al., 1986). Low back pain

may also be the most expensive condition in North America, with extremely high costs associated

with medical and surgical care, injury litigation, worker's compensation, long term disability

insurance, social security payments and lost work time (Mayer et al., 1986). In addition,

diagnoses of low back pain are notoriously difficult, involving a large subjective component, poor

correlation between degeneration and symptomatology, and a lack of objective physical findings

(Mayer et al., 1986). Based on these facts, it is understandable that both physicians and insurance

companies might be more aware of the possibility of deception in this population, as well as being

highly invested in conducting an accurate assessment of clients' pain experiences.

The pain stimulus will involve an exacerbation of chronic low back pain, using a standard

range of motion exercise performed during physiotherapy assessments. While capable of

instigating sharp, intense pain, range of motion exercises have negligible risk of harming the

patients. These patients will be videotaped while being asked to perform these exercises

repeatedly under instructions to genuinely express their pain, mask their pain, exaggerate their

pain and fake their pain following established protocols (e.g. Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994).

The facial expressions observed within these conditions will be compared on the following

measures: 1) the number, type, and intensity of individual facial actions; 2) the temporal qualities

of the individual facial actions, such as the time of onset, duration, duration of peak intensity, and

speed of onset and offset; 3) the contiguity in time, or the cohesiveness of the facial actions

making up the facial expression; 4) the number and type of facial expessions or constellations 42 exhibited; and 5) the incidence of actions thought to be indicative of deception, such as blinking, smiling and asymmetry of facial actions.

The specific hypotheses of the study are:

1. The faked expression will appear as a stronger version of the genuine pain face. It will consist of a significantly larger number of pain-related facial actions than will the genuine pain expression, and these facial actions will be displayed more intensely in the faked pain expression.

2. The faked expression will have a significantly higher incidence of facial actions not typically associated with genuine pain.

3. The masked pain expression will be indistinguishable from the baseline neutral

expression except for low intensity activity in the region of the eye orbit.

4. The masked and faked pain conditions will show decreased frequency of blinking, and an increase in asymmetry.

5. Genuine pain expressions will have a shorter period of peak intensity, and will be of

shorter overall duration than the faked pain expression. However, the speed of onset (i.e., the time to peak intensity) and the speed of offset (time from peak intensity offset to facial action

offset) may be longer in the genuine expression.

6. There will be greater cohesiveness, or temporal contiguity, of the pain-related facial

actions making up the genuine pain expression than those in the faked pain condition.

7. Faked pain expressions will be composed of either more "constellations" of facial

actions representing multiple emotional expressions or of a different mix of emotional expressions. 43

Study Two: Effectiveness of Training Strategies to Improve the Discrimination of Genuine and Deceptive Pain Expressions The purpose of this study is twofold: (1) to determine if there are individual differences in the ability to detect deceptive pain displays and (2) to determine if people's ability to detect faked or masked pain can be improved through the use of various training strategies. The following study will be the first to investigate individual differences in the judgement of deception in pain patients, by comparing successful and unsuccessful judges on such variables as the number and type of cues used, their decision-making confidence, and empathy scores. As an attempt to assess the feasibility of training to improve detection accuracies, the present study will fill a gap in the literature by utilizing a training program based on the results of recent research in the areas of pain and deception.

The specific hypotheses of the study are as follows: 1. There will be individual differences in the ability to discriminate genuine and deceptive pain expressions. That is, the percent of patients' facial expressions which can be correctly classified into neutral* genuine, faked and masked pain will differ across subjects, ranging from accuracy levels well below chance to better than chance accuracy. 2. Detection ability will be related to the subjects' score on the Perspective-taking subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index, but will not be related to scores on the other three subscales.

3. Detection ability will be correlated with the subjects' degree of past experience with pain. 4. Detection ability will also be related to several aspects of the judges' decision-making process. Specifically, successful "detectors" will report that they use more facial cues, and will utilize different judgement cues.

5. Detection accuracy will be correlated with decision-making confidence for each rating.

6. Accuracy levels will differ between training groups. Subjects given training plus corrective feedback will perform best, followed by the training group, the feedback only group and controls. Similar changes in the number and type of cues used, and decision making confidence are expected to occur. Due to the proposed individual differences in detection accuracy, there will be considerable overlap in the range of accuracies across the training groups. 44

Study 1: Analysis of Genuine, Faked, and Masked Pain Expressions

Method

Subjects

The 40 subjects who provided the facial expression data in this study were patients undergoing treatment for low back pain at either the Point Grey Physiotherapy Clinic in Vancouver,

B.C. or the Lansdowne Physiotherapy Clinic in Richmond, B.C. To be included in the study, the patients were required to: (a) be experiencing low back pain at the time of the assessment, and (b) to have sufficient command of the English language to complete the questionnaire packages and to

follow study instructions. Subjects were not excluded because of pending litigation, compensation

claims or medication use. All patients were paid $10 for their participation.

Videotape Equipment

Subjects were videotaped on Sony Ffi-8 metal-P NTSC 120 tapes using a Sony Hi-8 CCD-

TR81 NTSC video camera. The video data was then be transfered to 1/2 inch video cassettes (Fuji

HQ T-120) and dubbed with a digital time display (minutes, seconds, 60ths of a second) using an

RCA video time/date generator, model TC-1440-B. The time display allowed coders to identify the time segments to be coded without listening to the audio portion of the tape, which identified the

instruction set. The video data was edited into the final form required for coding, using two

Panasonic PC14A51R 14"" Colour TV Monitors, two Panasonic AG 1970 S-VHS Editing VCR's

and a Future Video EC-1000 Pro MK 11 editing board. A Videomics TM-1 Title Maker was used

to label each segment with an A, B, C or D in order of presentation.

The viewing system used for coding the facial expression data was a 20" Sharp MTS Stereo

Monitor hooked up to a Panasonic AG-6300 video cassette recorder. A remote control equipped

with a shuttle allowed coders to view the segments in frame by frame slow motion and stop frame

playback when necessary.

Coding Videotape

In order to ensure that coders' ratings were not influenced by the patients' behaviour

between events, these sections of the tape were edited out, leaving 5 seconds of blank tape between 45

each coding segment. The 160 segments were coded in random order, with the coder blind to the neutral, genuine or deceptive conditions.

Measures

Each patient completed a questionnaire package consisting of a personal data sheet and the

Descriptor Differential Scales (See Appendix C).

Personal Data Sheet. The personal data sheet asked simple questions about the patient's

sex, age, occupation, marital status and employment status. In addition, questions were asked

about the intensity, location, and duration of their pain condition, as well as about the frequency and

type of medication used for pain.

Descriptor Differential Scales (DPS). The patients were asked to rate the unpleasantness

and intensity of the pain sensations they experienced on an average day using the DDS (Gracely,

Dubner & McGrath, 1979). The scale provides 13 descriptive adjectives on two scales, ranging

from "not painful" to extremely painful", and "not unpleasant" to "extremely unpleasant",

respectively. These descriptors are reliable measures of the pain experience and have been ratio-

scaled using cross-modality matching procedures to provide numerical equivalents for the verbal

descriptors (Gracely, Dubner & McGrath, 1982).

Procedure

Potential subjects were informed of the study by the physiotherapist when they called to

make appointments for treatment sessions. If a patient indicated interest on the phone, they were

approached by the investigators upon their arrival for their scheduled session. The investigators

followed a standard protocol for both recruitment and study procedures. The investigators

approached patients by introducing themselves and stating their affiliation with the Psychology

Department at the University of British Columbia. They then proceeded to describe the purpose of

the study and the procedures which would be involved.

The patients were told that the study had two aims: 1) to see if factors such as nonverbal

pain expressions and have an effect on other's judgements of the patient's

pain intensity and overall health and 2) to see how closely these judgements compare to the patient's

actual pain and health status. The patients were told that they would be asked to complete a 46 number of questionnaires on their pain experience and coping strategies. They were also informed that they would be asked to perform certain movements during their normal physiotherapy session which might cause discomfort and that these movements would be repeated following different instructions and their facial expressions would be videotaped. Permission to obtain additional information from their physiotherapy charts was also obtained.

If the patients were still willing to participate at this point, they were given a consent form to

read which reiterated the information given by the investigator (see Appendix D). Patients were told that the information was for research purposes only, and that their participation would remain

confidential and anonymous. They were also be advised that only their videotaped session would be viewed by university students and health professionals. Finally, all patients were informed that their

participation was voluntary and that they could withdraw from the study at any time. Patients were then required to sign the consent form before proceeding with the study.

Patients were given the questionnaire package to complete while they were in the physiotherapy examination room, awaiting the start of their physiotherapy session. During this

time, the investigator set up the video equipment and adjusted the height and focus of the

equipment based on the height of the patient. When the patient had completed the package, the

physiotherapist was called in and the second portion of the study began. At this point, the

physiotherapist was given his script and the presentation order for the range of motion procedures.

Videotaping protocol. Each patient was videotaped during a standard range of motion

assessment conducted by their physiotherapist. Patients were videotaped in one of the clinic's

examination rooms, while lying on their backs on the examining table, with head and shoulders

propped up slightly with a head rest. The video camera was mounted on a tripod positioned at the

foot of the examining table, and was focused on the head and shoulders of the patients.

To begin the session, patients were videotaped during a neutral "at rest" condition, in both a

sitting up and lying down position. Once baseline recording was completed, the range of motion

exercise was introduced. During this test, the physiotherapist asked patients to straighten their legs

and raise them 10 inches off the examination table, a movement which low back pain patients report

to be very painful. A series of instructions were provided in varying orders. Patients performed the 47 leg raise two times, with instructions to: 1) genuinely express any pain that they felt, and 2) pretend that it didn't hurt. In a third set of instructions, the patients were asked to give a faked expression, by pretending that they were in a lot of pain without actually moving their legs (see Appendix E).

The three pain conditions or sets of instructions were counterbalanced for each patient to reduce order effects, as past research has shown that subjects who fake pain first exhibit more intense faked and genuine facial expressions (Prkachin, 1992a). The patient's facial expressions were videotaped for 10 seconds following the initiation of each movement / expression condition. Following completion of the range of motion procedures, the patients completed another set of Descriptive

Differential Scales to rate the unpleasantness and intensity of the most severe pain they experienced during the leg-lifting exercises (see Appendix F).

Quantification of Facial Activity. Each patient's five videotaped segments were coded by a trained FACS coder to describe and quantify their facial activity. For each of the 44 FACS action units (AU's), the following parameters were measured during the 10 second interval: the time of

AU onset, the time of AU offset, the maximum intensity or "apex" of the AU, and the duration of the apex. The apex of an AU is defined as "the period during which the movement was held at the highest intensity it reached". In accordance with past research, certain AU's which involve the same

muscle action were combined to produce new AU's which would allow for more meaningful intensity and temporal coding: AU1 (inner brow raise) and AU2 (outer brow raise) produced the variable "brow raise", AU 6 (cheek raise) and AU 7 (lid tighten) produced the variable "orbit tightening", AU 9 (nose wrinkle) and 10 (upper lip raise) produced the variable "levator

contraction" and AU 25 (lips apart), 26 (jaw drop) and 27 (mouth stretch) produced the variable

"mouth opening" (Hadjistavropolous & Craig, 1994; Prkachin, 1992a).

Intensity for each AU was rated oh a standardized 5-point rating scale, ranging from "trace" to "maximum". Three AU's which do not lend themselves to intensity coding (AU 11, nasolabial

deepening; AU 38, nostril dilation; AU45, blinking) were coded simply for time of onset and time of

offset, while the mouth opening variable was coded on a 3 point scale, with a score of 1 for AU 25,

a 2 for AU26 and a 3 for AU 27 (Ekman & Friesen, 1983; Prkachin, 1992a). Finally, coders noted

the occurence of any asymmetry in individual facial actions. 48

FACS coding was performed by two trained coders who had completed FACS training and reached the certified proficiency level (Ekman & Friesen, 1978). A third trained FACS coder

completed reliability coding for 20% of the videotaped segments, including reliability estimates for the frequency, intensity and temporal parameters for each AU. To avoid inflated reliability

estimates, agreements were not be counted for the nonoccurence of AU's, as nonoccurence of AU's tends to be far more frequent. For a more conservative measure of reliability, percent agreement

was calculated as the proportion of agreements on the presence of AU's relative to the total number of AU's scored using the following formula:

Percent Agreement = (# of AU's on which coder 1 and coder 2 agreed) X 2 # of AU's scored by coder 1 + # of AU's scored by coder 2

Agreements were scored only if both coders agreed that an AU occured within a 0.2 second time frame. The proper method for scoring the reliability for AU onset and offset is less well

established. Based on past work, it was likely that acceptable reliability would be attained using

agreements within 0.10 seconds, however, due to the relative lack of information on temporal

coding, reliability was calculated a second time for both onset and offset, using the percent

agreement within 0.50 seconds. The reliability of intensity judgements was calculated using Pearson

correlation coefficients comparing the intensity scoring of both coders for each agreed upon AU. 49

Results

Description of Patient Population

Subjects were 23 males and 17 females, ranging from 19 to 75 years of age, with an

average age of 32.6 years (s.d = 10.89). 60.0% of the subjects were single, 20.0% were married,

2.86% widowed, 8.57% living common-law and 2.86% were separated. 31.43 % of subjects

experienced constant pain, while 65.71% experienced their pain intermittently. Subjects had

experienced pain for an average of 3.01 years, with 74.29% of subjects suffering from low back

pain, 40.0% from upper back pain, 31.4% from neck pain, 11.4% from shoulder pain, 11.4% from

leg pain, 5.7% from hip pain, and 2.9% each from chest pain, TMJ, pelvis pain, headaches, and

sciatica. When asked about pain onset, 42.9% did not respond or could not identify the

precipitant, 22.9% of subjects were injured on the job, 22.9% in a car accident, and 5.7% each in

a fall or while walking. 42.9% of patients used prescription or non-prescription drugs on a daily basis to cope with their pain.

Reliability Coding

The reliability of FACS coding was analyzed for the frequency, intensity and temporal

parameters of each AU. The FACS coders showed 82.5% agreement for the occurrence of

individual AU's, which is slightly higher than reliability's reported by Ekman and Friesen (1978a).

The reliability of intensity ratings of agreed upon AU's, measured using a pearson correlation, was

.85. Both the frequencyan d intensity reliabilities were determined to be acceptable. Given the

infrequent use of temporal coding, and thus a lack of information regarding acceptable reliability,

the percent agreement between coders was calculated for varying degrees of temporal overlap. The

reliability of coding on the temporal parameters varied widely. The coding of AU onset was highly

reliable, with 95.2% agreement, calculated when AU's coincided within 0.1 second. The remaining

temporal parameters (onset duration, apex duration, response duration) which involve more subtle

distinctions of intensity and/or two temporal estimations, were not reliably coded within 0.1

seconds, with percent agreements of 62.7%, 52.0% and 54.7% respectively. Widening the degree

of temporal overlap to within 0.2 seconds resulted in acceptable reliability estimates of 70.7% for

both onset duration and overall response duration, however, apex duration was coded with only 50

66.7% agreement at this level of overlap. Using the upper limit of temporal overlap proposed for the temporal coding (0.5 seconds), all temporal indices fell well within acceptable ranges for reliability (onset duration (82.7%), apex duration (80.0%), response duration (84.0%)). Given that duration estimates involve two temporal estimations, and that apex duration involves subtle distinctions in intensity compared to coding the presence or absence of an AU, it was determined that widening the range of temporal overlap was reasonable, and that acceptable levels of reliability

had been reached for all FACS variables.

Preliminary Analyses

One FACS variable, offset duration, was omitted from analysis. Offset duration was not

codeable in several instances, due to the continuation of AU's beyond the end of the recorded

segment. The amount of missing data for the remaining FACS variables was negligible.

Initial analyses of the FACS data established that 15 facial actions (AU's) were observed in more than 5% of the coded segments. These were retained for the analyses of facial activity: inner

and outer brow raise (AU1 & 2), lowered brow (AU4), orbit tightening (AU6 & 7), levator

contraction (AU9 & 10), lip corner pull (AU12), dimpler (AU14), chin raise (AU17), tongue show

(AU19), lip press (AU24), lip part, jaw drop, mouth stretch (AU25/26/27), nostril dilate (AU38),

eye slit (AU42), squint (AU44), eyes closed (AU43) and blink (AU45).

Frequency

A mixed-model MANOVA was carried out to determine if the frequency of individual facial

actions differed across pain conditions. It was predicted that: (1) the faked pain expressions would

have a greater incidence of both pain-related and noh-pain-related facial actions than the genuine

pain expression, (2) genuine pain expressions would have a greater frequency of pain-related facial

actions than either the neutral or masked pain expressions, (3) the masked pain expression would

have a greater frequency of movements in the eye area, but would be otherwise indistinguishable from the neutral expression, and (4) that the masked and faked pain conditions would show

significantly less blinking than neutral and genuine pain conditions. The results of the analysis are

presented in Table 1, while mean frequency scores across pain conditions are presented in Table 2. Table 1

Results of MANOVA and Followup ANOVA's Examining Frequency of Facial Actions Across

Pain Conditions

Df F E<

Multivariate Main Effect 45,315 2.18 .001

Univariate F-tests

Brow Raise (AU 1/AU 2) 3,117 6.22 .001

Brow Lowering (AU4) 3, 117 16.85 .001

Orbit Tightening (AU6 / AU7) 3, 117 7.82 .001

Levator Contraction (AU9 / AU10) 3, 117 5.05 .003

Lip Corner Pull (AU12) 3, 117 1.89 .136

Dimpler (AU14) 3, 117 1.95 .125

Chin Raise (AU17) 3, 117 1.66 .180

Tongue Show (AU19) 3, 117 1.72 .168

Lip Press (AU24) 3, 117 1.05 .373

Opened Mouth (AU25/26/27) 3, 117 6.52 .001

Nostril Flare (AU38) 3, 117 0.75 .525

Eye Slit (AU42) 3, 117 2.16 .096

Eyes Closed (AU43) 3, 117 7.37 .001

Eye Squint (AU44) 3,117 3.33 .022

Blinking (AU45) 3, 117 3.20 .026

Note: Bonferroni adjustments suggest a significance level of .003 52

Table 2

Mean Facial Action Frequency Across Pain Conditions

(AU) Facial Action Neutral Genuine Masked Faked

M SD M SD M SD M SD

Brow Raise 0.12 0 56 0.20 0.56 0.10 0 44 0 75a,b,c 1 32

Brow Lowering 0.00 0 00 0.50a 0.99 0.10 0 38 1 20a>b>c 1 38

Orbit Tightening 0.00 0 00 0.22 0.53 0.20 0 46 0 55a-c 0 75

Levator Contraction 0.00 0 00 0.25 0.63 0.08 0 35 0 35a>c 0 66

Lip Corner Pull 0.05 0 22 0.20 0.46 0.20 0 46 0 25 0 44

Dimpler 0.00 0 00 0.12 0.33 0.08 0 27 0 15 0 43

Chin Raise 0.02 0 16 0.22 0.58 0.20 0 56 0 12 0 40

Tongue Show 0.00 0 00 0.12 0.40 0.10 0 30 0 05 0 22

Lip Press 0.02 0 16 0.12 0.33 0.12 0 40 0 10 0 30

Open Mouth 0.18 0 45 0.70a 0.85 0.62a 0 87 0 90a,b,c 0 98

Nostril Dilate 0.00 0 00 0.08 0.27 0.10 0 50 0 10 0 38

Eye Slit 0.00 0 00 0.08 0.27 0.02 0 16 0 15 0 48

Closed Eyes 0.00 0 00 0.25 0.54 0.05 0 32 0 42a>c 0 75

Eye Squint 0.00 0 00 0.10 0.38 0.00 0 00 0 20 0 56

Blinking 2.50 2 70 3.95 3.44 2.60 2 35 3 28 3 43 a Mean is significantly greater than neutral mean (p<05) b Mean is significantly greater than genuine mean (p<05) c Mean is significantly greater than masked mean (p<05) 53

Multivariate analyses showed that the overall frequency of facial actions differed significantly across pain conditions. Univariate F-tests examining the frequency of individual facial actions across pain conditions provided partial support for the study hypotheses. The incidence of one usually non pain-related facial action (brow raise) was found to vary across pain condition, while the remaining non pain-related facial action (eye slit, eye squint, lip corner pull, dimpler, chin raise, tongue show, lip press and nostril flare) occurred equally frequently in all conditions. Tukey's post hoc analyses for AUs yielding significant univariate ANOVA's showed that brow raise occurred more often in faked pain than in genuine pain expressions (p < .05), masked pain expressions (p < .01) or neutral expressions (p < .01). Genuine pain expressions, in comparison, showed as many brow raises as masked pain and neutral expressions. The incidence of blinking, predicted to be an indicator of deception, did not vary across pain condition.

Five pain-related facial actions (actions observed in past pain research) also differed across pain condition (brow lowering, opened mouth, orbit tightening, levator contraction, and closed eyes). Tukey's post hoc analyses provided comparisons of individual facial actions between pain expressions. Brow lowering occurred more often in faked pain expressions than in either the genuine pain, masked pain or neutral expressions (p < .01). The genuine pain expression did not differ from the masked pain expression in the frequency of brow lowering, but exhibited a greater frequency of brow lowering than the neutral expression. Masked pain and neutral expressions did not differ with respect to the frequency of brow lowering. An opened mouth was also seen more frequently in faked pain expressions than in either the genuine pain, masked pain, or neutral expressions (p < .01). Genuine and masked pain expressions did not differ in the frequency of mouth opening, but both exhibited a greater frequency of mouth opening than the neutral expression (p < .01). Orbit tightening, levator contraction and eye closure movements all occured more frequently in the faked pain expressions than in either the masked pain (p < .05) or neutral expressions (p < .01), but occured as frequently in the genuine pain expressions. There was however, no significant difference between the genuine, masked and neutral expressions in any of these three facial actions. 54

Results of the frequency analyses provided some support for the study's hypotheses.

Compared to the genuine pain expression, the faked pain expressions showed a greater incidence of

one non pain-related facial actions (brow raise), as well as a greater frequency of two pain-related facial actions (brow lowering, open mouth). Contrary to expectation, there was no difference in the

incidence of pain-related facial actions between the genuine and masked pain expressions, although the frequency of brow lowering did not differentiate masked and neutral expressions, whereas

genuine expressions were assicated with a greater incidence than neutral expressions. Genuine pain

expressions differed from the neutral expression only in the frequency of two pain-related facial

actions: showing more brow lowering and mouth opening. As expected, the masked pain

expression and the neutral expression were almost indistinguishable, differing only in the frequency

of mouth opening.

Although patients' pain expressions were comprised of facial actions that have been

associated with the experience of pain, there was substantial variation in the facial expressions

exhibited. For example, only 30% of patients exhibited brow lowering in response to genuine pain,

while 45% of patients responded with mouth opening. In comparison, there appeared to be less

variation in the display of faked pain expressions, with 62.5% of patients exhibiting brow lowering,

and 55% of patients showing mouth opening.

Intensity

A mixed-model MANOVA was carried out to determine if the intensity of individual facial

actions differed across pain conditions. Significant multivariate main effects indicated that the

overall intensity of facial actions differed significantly across pain condition (See Table 3).

Univariate F-tests examining individual facial actions showed that the intensity of one non pain-

related facial action (brow raise) differed across condition. Tukey's post hoc analyses indicated that

the intensity brow raise movements was greater in the faked pain condition than either the genuine,

masked or neutral expressions (p < .01). The intensity of brow raise movements observed in the

genuine pain, masked pain and neutral expressions did not differ. The intensity of the remaining non

pain-related facial actions (eye slit, eye squint, lip corner pull, dimpler, chin raise, tongue show, and

lip press) did not vary across pain condition. Mean intensity scores are presented in Table 4. 55

Table 3

Results of MANOVA and Followup ANO V A's Examining Intensity of Facial Actions Across Pain

Conditions

Df F E<

Multivariate Main Effect 39, 312 2.35 .001

Univariate F-tests

Brow Raise (AU 1/AU2) 3, 114 7.33 .001

Brow Lowering (AU4) 3, 114 22.70 .001

Orbit Tightening (AU6 / AU7) 3, 114 6.26 :001

Levator Contraction (AU9 / AU10) 3, 114 8.10 .001

Lip Corner Pull (AU12) 3, 114 2.17 .095

Dimpler (AU14) 3, 114 1.85 .141

Chin Raise (AU17) 3, 114 1.83 .145

Tongue Show (AU19) 3, 114 2.52 .061

Lip Press (AU24) 3, 114 1.53 .211

Opened Mouth (AU25/26/27) 3, 114 4.41 .006

Eye Slit (AU42) 3, 114 1.42 .240

Eyes Closed (AU43) 3, 114 4.52 .005

Eye Squint (AU44) 3,114 3.81 .012

Note: Bonferroni adjustments suggest a significance level of .003. 56

Table 4

Mean Facial Action Intensity Across Pain Conditions

(AU) Facial Action Neutral Genuine Masked Faked

M SD M SD M SD M SD

Brow Raise 0.08 0 35 0 13 0 57 0 05 0 32 0 80a>b>c 1.5

Brow Lowering 0.00 0 00 0 85a 1 60 0 13 0 57 2 36a,b,c 2.33

Orbit Tightening 0.00 0 00 0 54 1 37 0 44 1 11 1 15a>c 1.71

Levator Contraction 0.00 0 00 0 62a 1 39 0 13 0 57 0 95a,c 1.64

Lip Corner Pull 0.15 0 67 0 38 0 99 0 62 1 40 0 80 1.59

Dimpler 0.00 0 00 0 38 1 04 0 33 1 06 0 41 1.12

Chin Raise 0.3 0 16 0 46 1 12 0 44 1 10 0 28 0.97

Tongue Show 0.00 0 00 0 26 0 78 0 36 1 09 0 05 0.32

Lip Press 0.03 0 16 0 33 0 93 0 28 0 89 0 23 0.71

Open Mouth 0.23 0 58 0 69 0 86 0 74 0 91 0 80 0.83

Eye Slit 0.00 0 00 0 20 0 92 0 10 0 64 0 36 1.18

Closed Eyes 0.00 0 00 0 18 0 82 0 00 0 00 0 62 1.48

Eye Squint 0.00 0 oo 0 33 1 18 0 00 0 00 0 62 1.52

a Mean is significantly greater than neutral mean b Mean is significantly greater than genuine mean

c Mean is significantly greater than masked mean 57

The intensity analyses of the pain-related AU's yielded more complex results, with three of the pain-related facial actions varying across condition (brow lowering, orbit tightening and levator contraction) and the remaining facial actions (opened mouth, and closed eyes) maintaining a consistent intensity across condition. The intensity of brow lowering was greater in the faked pain expressions than in either the genuine pain, masked pain or neutral expressions (p < .01). The intensity of brow lowering in the genuine pain expressions was equivalent to that in the masked pain expressions, but greater than that seen in neutral expressions (p < .05), while the masked pain and neutral expressions did not differ. Orbit tightening intensity was found to be greater during faked pain than during a masked pain (p < .05) or neutral expression (p < .01), with no significant differences in intensity between the masked pain and neutral expressions. The intensity of orbit tightening movements in genuine pain expressions did not differ from either the faked pain expressions or the masked pain and neutral expressions. The intensity of levator contraction movements were also higher in the faked condition than in either the neutral or the masked pain expression (p < .01), with no differences in intensity between the masked pain and neutral expressions. Faked pain and genuine pain expressions were equal in the intensity of these movements however, as were genuine pain and masked pain expressions. Genuine pain expressions, however, exhibited a greater intensity of levator contraction than did the neutral

expressions.

Results of the intensity analyses provided some support for the study's hypotheses.

Compared to the genuine pain expression, faked pain showed a greater intensity of one non pain- related facial actions (brow raise), as well as a more intense display of several pain-related facial

actions (brow lowering, orbit tightening and levator contraction), In terms of facial action intensity, the distinction between genuine pain, masked pain and neutral expressions was very subtle.

Genuine pain expressions exhibited a greater intensity of brow lowering and levator contraction than seen in the neutral expressions, while the remaining facial actions (pain-related and non pain-

related) did not differ among these three conditions.

Apex Duration. The average duration of peak intensity for each individual AU was

compared across pain conditions using a mixedTinodel MANOVA. Multivariate analyses showed 58 that apex duration differed significantly across pain conditions (See Table 5). Univariate F tests examining the duration of peak intensity for individual facial actions provided partial support for the

study hypotheses. The apex duration for all of the non pain-related facial actions (brow raise, eye

slit, eye squint, lip corner pull, dimpler, chin raise, tongue show, and lip press) was found to be consistent across pain conditions. Mean apex duration scores are presented in Table 6.

Significant differences in apex duration across pain condition were found in three pain-

related facial actions: brow lowering, orbit tightening, and eye closure. Tukey's post-hoc analyses

showed that the apex duration brow lowering intensity was significantly longer in the faked pain

condition (p< 01) than in the genuine, masked and neutral facial expressions, which were equivalent. The apex duration of orbit tightening and eye closure were equivalent in the faked and

genuine condition, with the apex in faked pain faces being of longer duration then either the masked

or neutral expression (p < .01). Apex durations in the genuine pain expression did not differ from

either the masked pain or neutral expressions. The apex duration of the remaining pain-related facial

actions (opened mouth, and levator contraction) did not vary across pain condition.

Onset Duration. To determine the onset duration, the average time lag between the initial

onset of a facial action and peak intensity was calculated across all AU's, and was compared across

pain condition using a mixed-model ANOVA. A significant difference in overall onset duration was

found (F (3,37) = 15.02, p < .001). Tukey's post hoc analyses did not support study hypotheses,

showing that genuine and faked pain displays were equivalent in the time from facial expression

onset to peak intensity. Neutral facial expressions showed a longer onset duration (X = 1.44 sec,

s.d = 0.38, p < .01) than either the faked (X = 0.89 sec, s.d. = 0.61) or genuine pain expressions (X

= 0.93 sec, s.d. = 0.49). The onset duration of masked pain expressions were equivalent to both the

neutral and genuine pain expressions, but were of significantly longer onset duration than the faked

pain expressions (X = 1.24, s.d = 0.70, p < .05).

Facial Action Duration. A mixed-model MANOVA was used to test the hypothesis that

faked facial actions would last significantly longer than genuine facial actions. Multivariate

analyses showed that the overall response duration of facial actions varied across pain condition

(See Tables 7 & 8). Univariate F-tests examining the duration of individual AU's showed that the 59

Table 5

Results of MANOVA and Followup ANOVA's Examining Apex Duration of Facial Actions

Across Pain Conditions

Df F E<

Multivariate Main Effect 42, 318 2.35 .001

Univariate F-tests

Brow Raise (AU 1/AU2) 3, 117 0.79 .499

Brow Lowering (AU4) 3, 117 13.60 .001

Orbit Tightening (AU6 / AU7) 3, 117 4.66 .004

Levator Contraction (AU9 / AU10) 3, 117 1.88 .136

Lip Corner Pull (AU12) 3, 117 0.99 .398

Dimpler (AU14) 3, 117 2.07 .108

Chin Raise (AU17) 3, 117 0.18 .909

Tongue Show (AU19) 3, 117 0.99 .402

Lip Press (AU24) 3, 117 0.35 .792

Opened Mouth (AU25/26/27) 3, 117 1.76 .159

Eye Slit (AU42) 3, 117 2.27 .084

Eyes Closed (AU43) 3, 117 5.26 .002

Eye Squint (AU44) 3, 117 2.44 .068

Note: Bonferroni adjustments suggest a significance level of .004. 60

Table 6

Mean Facial Action Apex Duration Across Pain Conditions

(AU) Facial Action Neutral Genuine Masked Faked

M (sec) SD M(sec) SD M (sec) SD M (sec) SD

Brow Raise 0 09 0.53 0.05 0 22 0.03 0.21 0.14 0 36

Brow Lowering 0 00 0.00 0.05 0 18 0.01 0.02 0.24a>b>c 0 37

Orbit Tightening 0 00 0.00 0.07 0 31 0.04 0.16 0.23a>c 0 50

Levator Contraction 0 83 0.00 0.82 0 13 0.83 0.04 0.82 0 13

Lip Corner Pull 0 06 0.27 0.07 0 21 0.17 0.61 0.22 0 73

Dimpler 0 00 0.00 0.02 0 07 0.02 0.06 0.10 0 39

Chin Raise 0 04 0.23 0.04 0 17 0.02 0.07 0.05 0 31

Tongue Show 0 00 0.00 0.00 0 01 0.00 0.01 0.00 0 01

Lip Press 0 06 0.40 0.11 0 59 0.05 0.21 0.03 0 10

Open Mouth 0 13 0.54 0.37 0 74 0.32 0.74 0.48 0 82

Eye Slit 0 00 0.00 0.00 0 01 0.00 0.00 0.20 0 82

Closed Eyes 0 00 0.00 0.07 0 22 0.00 0.00 0.18a>c 0 44

Eye Squint 0 00 0.00 0.01 0 03 0.00 0.00 0.12 0 48

a Mean is significantly greater than neutral mean

b Mean is significantly greater than genuine mean

c Mean is significantly greater than masked mean 61

Table 7

Results of MANOVA and Followup ANOVA's Examining the Duration of Facial Actions Across

Pain Conditions

Df F

Multivariate Main Effect 42, 318 2.26 .001

> Univariate F-tests

Brow Raise (AU 1/AU 2) 3, 117 2.02 .115

Brow Lowering (AU4) 3, 117 20.07 .001

Orbit Tightening (AU6 / AU7) 3, 117 8.80 .001

Levator Contraction (AU9 / AU10) 3, 117 4.64 .004

Lip Corner Pull (AU 12) 3, 117 1.86 .140

Dimpler (AU14) 3, 117 2.19 .093

Chin Raise (AU17) 3, 117 0.27 .848

Tongue Show (AU19) 3, 117 1.16 .326

Lip Press (AU24) 3, 117 0.35 .785

Opened Mouth (AU25/26/27) 3, 117 2.82 .042 •

Nostril Flare (AU38) 3, 117 0.82 .484

Eye Slit (AU42) 3, 117 2.57 .058

Eyes Closed (AU43) 3, 117 7.75 .001

Eye Squint (AU44) 3, 117 4.61 .004

Note: Bonferroni adjustments suggest a significance level of .004 62

Table 8

Mean Facial Action Duration Across Pain Conditions

(AU) Facial Action Neutral Genuine Masked Faked

M(sec) SD M(sec) SD M(sec) SD M(sec) SD

Brow Raise 0 09 0 53 0 10 0.36 0 08 0 38 0.30 0 58

Brow Lowering 0 00 0 00 0 13 0.28 0 01 0 04 0.46a>b>c 0 56

Orbit Tightening 0 00 0 00 0 16 0.48 0 06 0 18 0.55a>b>c 0 98

Levator Contraction 0 41 0 00 0 57 0.53 0 44 0 11 0.68a>c 0 63

Lip Corner Pull 0 11 0 41 0 14 0.38 0 36 1 02 0.45 1 15

Dimpler 0 00 0 00 0 12 0.38 0 04 0 12 0.16 0 56

Chin Raise 0 04 0 23 0 06 0.18 0 04 0 12 0.09 0 50

Tongue Show 0 00 0 00 0 02 0.08 0 03 0 10 0.01 0 07

Lip Press 0 06 0 40 0 15 0.62 0 19 0 83 0.11 0 38

Open Mouth 0 16 0 56 0 48 0.81 0 45 0 92 0.66 0 98

Eye Slit 0 00 0 00 0 01 0.03 0 00 0 02 0.22 0 83

Closed Eyes 0 00 0 00 0 13 0.31 0 02 0 12 0.47a,b,c 0 97

Eye Squint 0 00 0 00 0 03 0.15 0 00 0 00 0.30a>b>c 0 84

a Mean is significantly greater than neutral mean

b Mean is significantly greater than genuine mean

c Mean is significantly greater than masked mean 63 changes in AU duration were limited to four pain-related facial actions (brow lowering, orbit tightening, levator contraction, and eye closure) and one non pain-related facial action (eye squint).

Tukey's post hoc analyses determined that the duration of brow lowering (p < .01), orbit tightening

(p < .01), and eye squint (p < .05) were significantly longer in the faked pain expressions than in the genuine pain, masked pain and neutral expressions, with no differences in duration between the other three facial expressions. The duration of eye closure showed a similar pattern, being longer in the faked pain expression than in either the genuine pain expression (p < .05), masked pain (p < .01) or neutral expressions (p < .01), with no differences between the final three expressions. The duration of levator contraction was also longer in the faked pain expression than in either the masked (p < .05) or neutral (p < .01) expressions, however, the faked and genuine pain expressions were of equal duration.

Asymmetry & Pulsating Apexes. A mixed-model MANOVA determined that the overall frequency of asymmetrical facial actions and pulsating apexes varied across pain conditions

(F = 2.58, p < .03). Univariate F tests and post-hoc analyses showed that study hypotheses were not supported: genuine and faked pain expressions exhibited similar frequencies of both asymmetrical facial actions and pulsating apices. However, faked facial actions showed asymmetry and pulsating apices significantly (p < .01) more frequently than either the masked pain or neutral expressions, while there were no significant differences between the genuine pain, masked pain and neutral expressions.

Temporal Contiguity of Facial Actions. The current study proposed to use cluster analysis techniques to determine if there would be greater temporal contiguity of pain-related facial actions in genuine versus faked pain expressions, as well as investigating the idea that faked pain

expressions would contain either a greater number of facial action "constellations" or a different mix of facial actions in the isolated clusters. Unfortunately, established procedures for performing

cluster analysis on FACS data (Friesen, 1982) utilize both facial action onset and offset, and facial action offset was not analyzed in the current study due to missing data. A second difficulty involved the large variation in facial actions exhibited by the subjects, which resulted in temporal variables being coded as "missing data" when a facial action was not exhibited by a subject. Cluster 64 analysis procedures are not appropriate for small sample sizes, and do not tolerate large amounts of missing data, therefore missing data was replaced by group means. This allowed for a generalized investigation of temporal patterns using agglomerative (within group) average linkage cluster analysis, with pearson correlations as the similarity grouping measure.

The time of onset for the most frequently occuring AU's were analysed separately for the genuine and faked pain expressions. As replacing large amounts of missing data with group means makes a detailed investigation of temporal contiguity questionable, clusters were simply examined to determine if onset time of pain-related AU's were similar. Results showed that for genuine pain expressions, a four cluster solution produced two clusters containing pain-related and non pain- related facial actions, and two clusters containing only non pain-related facial actions. The first

"pain" cluster contained orbit tightening, levator contraction, and eyes closed, as well as blinking and eyebrow raise. The second "pain" cluster contained brow lowering and mouth opening, as well

as squinting and lip corner pull. The first "no pain" cluster included eye slit, nostril dilate, lip press and chin raise, while the second was comprosed of dimpler and tongue show movements. Results

suggest that the facial actions identified by past research as "pain-related" appeared fairly closely together in time during genuine pain expressions. In comparison, a four cluster solution for faked

pain expressions showed pain-related facial actions spread across all four clusters. In addition, brow lowering and mouth opening, which appeared together and were the most frequent facial

response to genuine pain, did not occur within the same cluster. Faked pain expressions then,

seemed to be comprised of pain-related facial actions which did not appear closely together in time. 65

Study 2: Training to detect deception

Methods

Subjects

Judges were 120 undergraduate students (60 male, 60 female) attending the University of

British Columbia, who were either given course credit or were paid $10 for their participation. To be included in the study, subjects were required to have a sufficient command of the English language to complete the questionnaire packages. The subjects ranged from 17 to 43 years of age, with a mean age of 21.7 years (s.d. = 4.6).

Materials

Video Equipment. The video data collected in Study 1 was edited into stimulus tapes using

a Panasonic editing system comprised of two Panasonic PC14A51R 14"" Colour TV Monitors, two

Panasonic AG 1970 S-VHS Editing VCR's and a Future Video EC-1000 Pro MK 11 editing board.

A Videomics TM-1 Title Maker was used to label each segment with an A, B, C or D in order of

presentation.

The judges' viewing system consisted of a 20" Sharp MTS Stereo Monitor hooked up to a

Panasonic AG-6300 videocassette recorder equipped with a shuttle board which allowed the

subjects to pause, stop, rewind and review the tape in frameb y frame slow motion.

Videotape. The stimulus tape for the study contained the video data of 30 of the subjects

from study one. The remaining subjects were excluded due to imperfect data: six patients because

they were inclined on the bed during their neutral videoclip instead of lying down flat and four

because at least one of their four videoclips was less than 10 seconds in length. To construct the

stimulus tape, the video clips of the 30 patients were rearranged with editing equipment in

randomized order, with the limiting condition that successive clips alternated between male and

female patients. As it was determined that there was insufficient time available for subjects to make

the full set of 30 judgements and there was a risk of fatigue effects, each subject only viewed the

videotaped data for 20 of the subjects on the stimulus tape. Subjects were then assigned a

particular patient's videoclip as their first to rate. They then watched the next 20 subjects on the

tape. Subject's starting points were assigned in counterbalanced order to eliminate any order 66 effects, and so that each of the 30 patients' data was viewed by the same number of subjects. The

stimulus tape was dubbed so that 2 copies of the 30 patients' clips were on the same tape, which

allowed the experimenter to begin the judgement task with any of the 30 patients. Subjects beginning on the latter part of the first set of 30 continued with the beginning clips of the second set

of 30.

The four video clips for each patient (baseline, genuine, faked and masked) were presented

at once, with the order of presentation being the counterbalanced order in which the segments were filmed. The videotape consisted of 120 segments, each of which were approximately 10 seconds in

length. Each clip was followed by 5 seconds of blank tape to allow the judges time to pause the tape, rate the intensity of the patient's pain experience and to decide which category the expression

represented.

Training Manual. The training manual provided a written description of the typical facial

expression of pain, as well as current information on the facial cues which may differentiate genuine,

faked and masked pain expressions. Subjects were provided with information on the changes in

frequency and intensity of facial expressions found in past studies. The postulated temporal factors being investigated in study one were also described as possible cues to deception. In addition, they

were provided with a description of the facial cues thought to be indicative of deceit in general,

including asymmetry^ timing and contiguity of facial expressions and multiple emotion expressions.

This information constituted the best of our knowledge to date about the cues which might enable

us to detect deceit in pain patients. Where possible, photographs or drawings of the various facial

expressions were included in the manual. After reading the training manual, subjects completed a

14-item training test, as a measure of their retention of the material (See Appendix G).

Measures

Each patient was asked to complete a questionnaire package containing, a personal

information sheet, the Interpersonal Reactivity Index and the consent form (See Appendix H).

Personal Information Sheet. Each subject was asked to fill out a personal information sheet

containing questions regarding their age, sex, ethnic background and previous experience with pain,

or relationship with others experiencing pain. 67

Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRD. Subjects' responsivity / empathy to others was be measured using the IRI, a 28-item self-report measure. The IRI consists of four 7-item subscales,

each tapping a separate aspect of the global concept of empathy: Perspective-Taking (PT), Fantasy

(FT), Empathic Concern (EC) and Personal Distress (PD).

Procedure

Upon the subject's arrival, the experimenter described the purpose of the study and the

procedures involved. The subjects were told that the aims of the study were: (1) to determine how

skilled people are at judging when someone is in pain through their facial expression, and (2) to

determine what qualities makes someone a good judge of others' pain experiences. They were also told that they would be asked to complete a questionnaire on their own pain experience and another which asks questions about their personality. Subjects were assured that their participation would

remain confidential and anonymous. In addition, they were informed that their participation was

completely voluntary and that they would be able to withdraw from the study at any time. If

subjects were still willing to participate, they were given a consent form to read which reiterated the

information provided by the experimenter. Subjects were required to sign the form before

proceeding with the study. At this point, subjects were brought into the viewing room and were

asked to complete the questionnaire package. When the package was completed, all subjects were

given a verbal description of the videotape they were about to see and instructions on the judgement

task, the rating materials and the use of the shuttle board.

Training Conditions. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of four experimental

groups: (1) control, (2) feedback, (3) deception training, and (4) deception training plus feedback.

The subjects in the control group viewed the videotape and made their judgements without any

further information on how to make their decisions, nor did they receive any feedback on their

performance. The feedback groups were given corrective feedback on the type of facial

expression depicted in each segment after each patient's four clips had been viewed and

categorized. That is, they were informed of their performance and the correct categorizations

following the judgement tasks for each patient. This information was provided in written format,

to ensure that differences in performance among the groups were not a result of the 68 experimenter's presence during the judgement task. The deception training groups were required to read the training manual and complete the training test before beginning the judgement task.

Finally, the deception training plus feedback were given feedback on their performance for each patient as well as being given the training manual to read beforehand.

Judgement Task Procedure. Each subject was shown the videotape data of 20 patients, with all patients' data being viewed by an equal number of subjects in each training group. Subjects were

allowed to view all four clips for each patient before making their judgements. In addition, the four clips could be viewed again if the subject felt they require a second viewing in order to come to a

decision. For each videoclip, the subjects rated the intensity and unpleasantness of the pain experienced by the patient. Ratings were made using the DDS, which provides 13 pain adjectives

on two scales, ranging from "not painful" to "extremely painful", and "not unpleasant" to "extremely unpleasant", respectively, and which have been ratio-scaled using cross-modality procedures to

provide numerical equivalents (Gracely et al., 1978; Gracely et al., 1979; Gracely et al., 1982). The

subjects were also asked to decide which category each of the four videoclips represented: baseline, genuine pain, faked pain or masked pain. It is important to note that this was not a forced choice

design, in that subjects were not informed that all four categories were represented for each patient.

Following their categorization of the four clips, the subjects were asked to describe which cues they used to make their decision. Using this format, each subject was required to make two judgements

about the facial expressions: 1) to estimate the degree of intensity and unpleasantness of pain

experienced by the patient and 2) to make a decision about the category of each facial expression.

Judgements for each patient were made before proceeding to the next (See Appendix I). 69

Results

Judgements of Pain

The overall accuracy of study participants ranged from 18.3% to 63.3%, with the mean judgement accuracy of students being 32.9% (S.D. = 7.5). A mixed-model MANOVA was conducted in order to determine if pain judgements differed across sex, training group or pain condition. Analyses were performed for judgement accuracy, decision-making confidence, and ratings of pain unpleasantness and intensity. The results of the analyses are presented in Table 9. Mean accuracy scores and decision-making confidence are presented in Table 10, and mean ratings of pain intensity and unpleasantness are presented in Table 11.

Sex. Multivariate analyses showed that pain judgements differed significantly across sex. Univariate F-tests suggest that females overall judgement accuracy was significantly higher than males. A comparison of means shows that females classified 35.6% of pain expressions accurately, as compared to 30.2% accuracy for males, both of which are significantly above chance levels (t = 10.29, p< .01; t = 7.94, p < .01). There was no sex difference for decision• making confidence, nor did males and females differ in the degree of unpleasantness or intensity they ascribed to various pain expressions. Training Group. Multivariate and univariate F-tests suggest that both judgement accuracy and decision-making confidence were influenced by the training group of the judges. Planned comparisons showed that subjects in the feedback and feedback plus deception training groups were equally accurate, correctly classifying 34.4% and 37.1% of pain expressions respectively (p < .086), both of which are significantly above chance levels (t = 6.33, p< .01; t = 8.49, p < .01). Subjects in the control and deception training only groups were significantly less accurate in their judgements, correctly classifying 30.4% (p< .001, p<.001) and 29.7% (p< .008, p<.001) of expressions, both of which are significantly above chance levels (t = 4.88, p< .01; t = 4.76, p < .01). With respect to decision-making confidence, subjects in the control group were significantly more confident (70.4%) in their decision-making than were the groups receiving feedback (63.9%; p<.005) or feedback plus deception training (58.8%; p<.002). The deception training subjects, with 67.6% decision-making confidence, did not differ significantly from 70

Table 9

Results of MANOVA on the Effects of Sex, Training and Pain Condition on Judgements of Pain.

df F p<

Multivariate Results followed by Univariate F-tests

Sex 4, 109 7.10 .001

Accuracy 1, 112 21.15 .001

Confidence 1, 112 0.34 .563

Pain Sensation 1, 112 4.27 .041

Pain Unpleasantness 1, 112 3.75 .055

Group 12,333 2.94 .001

Accuracy 3, 112 8.63 .001

Confidence 3, 112 3.84 .012

Pain Sensation 3, 112 0.45 .714

Pain Unpleasantness 3, 112 0.92 .433

Condition 12, 101 97.34 .001

Accuracy 3, 110 151.05 .001

Confidence 3, 110 31.28 .001

Pain Sensation 3, 110 110.54 .001

Pain Unpleasantness 3, 110 167.33 .001

Sex x Group 12, 333 1.52 .116

Sex x Condition 12, 101 2.12 .022

Group x Condition 36, 309 1.37 .085

Sex x Group x Condition 36, 309 1.54 .029 Note. Bonferroni adjustments suggest a significance level of .02 71

Table 10

Mean Accuracy and Decision-Making Confidence Across Training Group and Pain Condition

Accuracy Confidence

M SD M SD

Training Group

Control 30.44 6.11 70.36 13.29

Neutral 49.11 9.14 72.13 17.10

Genuine 24.89 8.92 70.47 10.99

Faked 25.00 10.24 71.00 12.62

Masked 22.78 8.93 67.82 14.30

Feedback 34.25 8.00 63.92 13.32

Neutral 47.33 11.02 66.58 13.82

Genuine 28.33 10.39 62.88 14.59

Faked 31.67 11.51 65.15 12.73

Masked 29.67 9.36 61.06 14.56

Training 29.72 5.47 67.63 12.02

Neutral 46.89 8.92 69.75 13.07

Genuine 23.67 11.08 66.73 11.94

Faked 27.33 8.99 69.39 12.32

Masked 21.00 9.02 64.66 12.47

Feedback / Training 37.14 7.82 58.75 16.99

Neutral 48.33 8.20 60.94 17.35

Genuine 31.22 11.86 57.80 17.87

Faked 34.22 10.47 61.20 16.82

Masked 34.78 12.46 55.07 17.55 72

Table 11

Mean Ratings of Pain Intensity and Unpleasantness Across Training Group and Pain Condition

Pain Intensity Pain Unpleasantness

M SD M SD

Training Group

Control 7.41 3.70 3.46 0.97

Neutral 1.78 1.67 1.79 1.52

Genuine 11.58 6.18 4.41 0.79

Faked 9.12 5.89 3.77 1.08

Masked 7.16 3.84 3.86 1.29

Feedback 8.16 5.61 3.75 1.18

Neutral 2.38 2.80 1.93 1.65

Genuine 11.79 8.19 4.72 1.27

Faked 8.32 4.39 3.96 1.56

Masked 10.13 10.04 4.38 1.21

Training 7.96 4.27 3.51 0.92

Neutral 2.25 2.19 1.74 1.23

Genuine 10.94 5.65 4.40 1.01

Faked 10.56 6.54 3.89 1.21

Masked 8.09 5.23 4.00 1.24

Feedback / Training 8.81 5.63 3.80 0.83

Neutral 2.56 2.07 1.96 0.94

Genuine 12.86 9.15 4.62 1.23

Faked 9.39 7.10 4.05 1.48

Masked 10.44 6.26 4.55 1.15 73

either the control (p < .454) or feedback groups (p < .308), although they were significantly more

confident than subjects in the feedback plus training group (p<02) (see Table 10). Univariate F-

tests determined that subjects in the various training groups did not differ in the degree of unpleasantness or intensity they ascribed to various pain expressions.

In order to determine if subjects in the training conditions read, understood and retained the information presented, they were given a training test prior to beginning the judgement task.

Subjects scores on the training tests ranged from 7 to 14, out of a total of 14 points, with an

average score of 11.0 (s.d. = 2.1). Further analyses were conducted to determine if judgement

accuracy was influenced by the subjects' training test scores. Pearson correlations showed that

subjects' scores on the training test were not correlated with judgement accuracy (r = .0826).

Finally, there was no correlation between training test scores and subjects' confidence in their

decision-making ability (r = -.0592). Given the relatively high test scores, it is obvious that the

majority of training group subjects understood and remembered the material presented, however,

the information provided did not improve the subject's judgement accuracy.

Pain Condition. Multivariate analyses showed that pain judgements varied significantly

across neutral, genuine, faked and masked pain conditions. Univariate F-tests determined that judgement accuracy, decision-making confidence, and ratings of pain unpleasantness and intensity

experienced by the patient all differed significantly across pain condition. Tukey's post-hoc

analyses indicated that judges were more accurate at determining a neutral expression (X - 47.9,

s.d. = 9.3) than a faked (X = 29.6, s.d. - 10.8), genuine (X = 27.03, s.d. - 10.9) or masked (X

- 27.06, s.d. = 11.4) pain expression (p < .01), while the detection accuracy for faked, genuine

and masked pain expressions did not differ.

Decision-making confidence was also found to vary with pain condition. Tukey's post hoc

analyses indicated that judges were more confident in their ability to detect neutral (X = 67.35,

s.d. = 15.83) and faked pain (X = 66.69, s.d. = 14.10) expressions than they were for their judgements of genuine pain (X = 64.47, s.d. = 14.70) and masked pain (X = 62.15, s.d. = 15.40)

expressions (p< .01). Judges were also significantly more confident in their identification of

genuine pain expressions than of masked pain expressions. 74

Judge's estimates of both pain intensity and unpleasantness differed depending on pain condition. Tukey's post hoc analyses showed that genuine pain faces were rated as exhibiting pain that was both more intense and unpleasant than either faked, masked or neutral pain expressions.

An approximate comparison of the average intensity and unpleasantness scores to the Gracely

descriptor terms indicates that genuine pain expressions were rated as "moderate" in intensity and

"unpleasant". Both the masked and faked pain expressions were thought to be experiencing more intense and unpleasant pain than the neutral expression, while faked and masked pain expressions were rated to be equal in perceived pain intensity and unpleasantness. The approximate Gracely

equivalents for the faked and masked expressions were "moderate" intensity and "slightly annoying", while neutral expressions were rated to be of "very weak" intensity and "slightly unpleasant".

Perceived Judgement Accuracy

Upon completion of their judgement task, subjects were asked to rate their overall

accuracy in identifying the four types of facial expression. Subjects identified an accuracy level of

70.67% for neutral facial expressions, 56.75% for genuine pain expressions, 59.34% for faked

pain expressions, and 54.78% accuracy for masked pain expressions.

Decision-Making Process Variables

Number of Cues Used. In an attempt to explain the influence of sex differences, training

group and pain condition on judgement accuracy, analyses were conducted to determine if pain judgements differed in terms of the number or type of cues used to reach each decision. A mixed-

model ANOVA was carried out to determine if the number of cues identified by each subject varied across sex, training group and pain condition. The results of the analysis are presented in

Table 12, while the mean cue use scores are presented in Table 13.

Results of the analysis showed that, despite their greater accuracy, females identified the

same number of cues as males when making their judgements. Similarly, although overall

accuracy differed across training groups, training condition did not significantly influence the

average number of cues used in judging facial expressions. Predictably, the number of facial cues

identified by the subjects differed significantly depending on the pain condition being judged. 75

Table 12

Results of MANOVA on the Effects of Sex, Training and Pain Condition on Number of Cues

Used.

df F P<

Sex 1, 112 116 .284

Group 3, 112 0.41 .743

Condition 3, 110 113.54 .001

Sex x Group 3, 112 0.14 .936

Sex x Condition 3, 110 1.10 .354 i Group x Condition 9, 336 1.82 .063

Sex x Group x Condition 9,336 1.05 .397 76

Table 13

Mean Number of Facial Cues Detected Across Treatment Groups and Pain Condition

Pain Condition

Treatment Groups Neutral Genuine Masked Faked

M SD M SD M SD M SD

Control Group 1.12 0.35 1.48 0.35 1.44 0.29 1.45 0.36

Feedback 1.09 0.19 1.29 0.29 1.30 0.24 1.30 0.22

Training 1.07 0.27 1.47 0.32 1.34 0.34 1.45 0.33

Training and Feedback 1.00 0.33 1.44 0.38 1.39 0.33 1.47 0.32 77

Tukey's post hoc analyses showed that subjects identified a greater number of cues for genuine and faked pain expressions than for either masked pain or neutral expressions (p < .01). Subjects

also identified significantly more facial cues in the masked pain condition than in the neutral expressions (p < .01).

Type of Cues Used. Subjects identified a total of 299 different facial cues that were used to make their judgements. The number of times each of these cues was used across all judgements was calculated for each subject, with the frequency of individual cues ranging from

.01 to 10.18 occurrences per subject (See Appendix J). Two types of analyses were conducted oh facial cue use: 1) by using the cues individually as identified by the subjects, and 2) by grouping

similar cues into cue use categories.

Of the 299 facial cues identified by the subjects, 15 facial cues were identified in greater

than 3% of the pain expressions (faked, genuine, masked) and were retained in two separate

MANOVAs to investigate the change in frequency across training group, sex and pain condition

(See Table 14). The interaction effects of training group, gender and pain condition on cue use

were not investigated, as separate analyses yielded more power. Multivariate analyses showed

that the use of individual facial cues differed significantly across training group (F = 2.54, p <

.001), and across pain condition (F = 16.85, p < .001), but did not vary with the judges' gender (F

- 1.19, p < .287). Univariate analyses determined that the following facial actions were identified

more or less frequently depending on the training group: lowered eyebrows, closed eyes,

exaggerated facial movements, and eye squinting (See Table 14). Tukey's post-hoc analyses

indicated that lowered eyebrows were identified more frequentlyb y subjects in the training group

than in the training plus feedback group (p < .05), with both the training (p < .01) and training

plus feedback (p < .05) groups using this cue more frequentlytha n either the control or feedback

groups. Eye closure and eye squinting were also identified more frequentlyb y the training group

than either the control Or feedback groups (p < .01), while the training plus feedback group used

these cues a moderate amount, with no difference in frequencybetwee n either the training group

or the control and feedback groups. Exaggerated facial movements were identified more

frequently by the training plus feedback group than either the control or feedback groups (p < Table 14

Results of MANOVA on the Effects of Training Group on the use of Individual Cues.

_df F p_<

Multivariate Results followed by Univariate F-tests

45,312 2.54 .001

Smile 3, 116 3.09 .030

Neutral Expression 3,116 1.54 .208

Lowered Eyebrow 3, 116 14.99 .001

Eyebrow Movements 3, 116 2.21 .090

Mouth Movements 3, 116 1.87 .138

Relaxed 3, 116 1.26 .291

Eye Movement 3, 116 2.32 .079

Eyes Closed 3, 116 4.77 .003

Exaggeration 3, 116 9.07 .001

Eyes Squinted 3, 116 6.51 .001

Tight Lips 3, 116 3.14 .028

Blinking 3, 116 3.31 .023

Tense 3, 116 0.69 .556

No Change in Expression 3,116 0.99 .402

Looking Around 3, 116 0.81 .488

Note. Bonferroni adjustments suggest a significance level of .003 79

.01), while the training group identified exaggeration with a frequency that fell between the training plus feedback group and the control and feedback groups. Finally, the frequency of

smiling, lack of facial movement, eyebrow movements, mouth movements, eye movements, relaxed features, tight lips, muscle tension, looking around, blinking and "no change in facial

expression" did not differ across training group.

Multivariate analyses determined that the use of facial cues varied across pain conditions,

while univariate analyses indicated that all 15 facial cues differed in frequency across the four pain

conditions (see Tables 15 and 16). Tukey's post hoc analyses indicated that subjects identified

"neutral", "relaxed" and "no change in expression" as cues when judging neutral facial expressions

more frequently than the masked, genuine or faked pain expressions (p < .01). In contrast, mouth

movements, closed eyes and eye movements were identified as facial cues more often when judging masked, faked and genuine pain expressions than for neutral facial expressions (p < .01).

Lowered eyebrows and exaggerated facial actions were identified most often while observing

faked pain expressions, followed by genuine and masked pain expressions. These cues were

identified equally as frequently in masked pain and neutral expressions, but were identified more

frequently in genuine pain than in neutral expressions (p < .01). Eyebrow movement in general

was identified most frequently during faked pain expressions, followed by genuine pain, with the

least frequency in masked pain and neutral expressions (p < .01). Eye squint was also identified

most frequently in faked pain expressions, followed by genuine pain and masked pain expressions,

with the least frequent cue use during neutral expressions. Blinking was identified with equal

frequency in faked pain, genuine pain and neutral expressions, and was identified less frequently

during masked pain conditions (p < .05). Smiling was observed most frequently during the two

deceptive conditions, faked pain and masked pain, and with less frequency during the genuine pain

and neutral expressions. Tension and looking around were identified most frequently when

observing actual pain experiences (genuine, masked), and less frequentlyi n faked pain and neutral

expressions (p < .01). Finally, tight lips were identified more frequently during genuine pain than

during faked pain (p < .05), with the frequency of tight lips identified during masked pain differing

neither from genuine or faked pain expressions. Tight lips were observed less frequently during Table 15

Results of MANOVA on the Effects of Pain Condition on the use of Individual Cues.

df F p_<

Multivariate Results followed by Univariate F-tests

Pain Condition 45, 1035 2.54 .001

Smile 3, 357 67.53 .001

Neutral Expression 3, 357 134.62 .001

Lowered Eyebrow 3, 357 21.29 .001

Eyebrow Movements 3, 357 31.70 .001

Mouth Movements 3, 357 18.88 .001

Relaxed 3, 357 38.85 .001

Eye Movement 3, 357 11.61 .001

Eyes Closed 3,357 23.74 .001

Exaggeration 3, 357 46.48 .001

Eyes Squinted 3, 357 25.80 .001

Tight Lips 3,357 13.01 .001

Blinking 3,357 5.62 .001

Tense 3, 357 8.80 .001

No Change in Expression 3,357 16.07 .001

Looking Around 3, 357 16.24 .001

Note. Bonferroni adjustments suggest a significance level of .003 81

Table 16

Mean Individual Cue Use Across Pain Condition

Facial Action (AU) Neutral Genuine Faked Masked

M SD M SD M SD . M SD

Smile 0.24 0.52 0.54 0.85 1.75 1.75 1.76 1.51

Neutral Face 6.43 5.41 1.20 1.41 1.22 1.34 1.48 1.67

Lowered Eyebrows 0.15 0.40 0.53 1.20 0.94 1.69 0.26 0.76

Eyebrow Movement 0.13 0.37 0.67 1.13 1.15 1.67 0.30 0.76

Mouth Movement 0.11 0.48 0.83 1.59 0.83 1.69 0.68 1.32

Relaxed 2.40 3.75 0.57 1.16 0.4? 1.08 0.69 1.44

Eye Movement 1.17 2.69 2.11 3.17 1.93 2.76 2.25 2.81

Eyes Closed 0.16 0.57 0.89 1.39 0.93 1.45 0.68 1.05

Exaggerated 0.05 0.22 0.43 0.76 1.50 2.05 0.20 0.46

Eye Squint 0.09 0.32 0.57 1.19 1.02 1.69 0.38 0.83

Tight Lips 0.13 0.43 0.66 1.34 0.38 0.79 0.59 1.17

Blinking 0.70 1.21 0.74 1.21 0.82 1.31 0.38 0.75

Tense 0.37 1.01 0.82 1.73 0.40 0.85 0.77 1.78

No Change in Face 1.18 2.98 0.27 0.77 0.31 0.85 0.29 1.09

Looking Around 0.23 0.53 0.68 1.20 0.30 0.73 0.92 1.62

3 Mean is significantly greater than control group b Mean is significantly greater then feedback group

c Mean is significantly greater than training group

d Mean is significantly greater than training / feedback group 82 neutral expressions than during either genuine (p < .01), masked (p < .01) or faked pain expressions (p < .05).

In order to provide a more comprehensive analysis of subject's judgement cues, the 299 facial cues identified by the judges were grouped according to muscle group or conceptual

similarity . For example, the facial cue group labeled "eyebrows" consisted of the following cues: lowered eyebrows, eyebrow movements, eyebrow twitch, pinched eyebrows, and raised eyebrows. Similarly, the category labeled "timing" included the following cues: short and long

duration, sudden movements, delayed onset, slow movements, early onset and actions which are not simultaneous. Facial actions which were identified fewer than five times were deleted from the analyses, as were several miscellaneous facial cues. Additional cues of interest, such as those involving asymmetry and leakage were also deleted due to infrequency. The final categorization

scheme is outlined in Appendix K.

Grouping judgement cues yielded fifteen facial cue categories: eyes, eyebrows,

cheeks/nose, mouth, smile, repressed facial actions, breathing pattern, incongruent facial actions,

pain expression, blinking, timing of facial actions, exaggeration, tension, sudden movements and

relaxed muscles. As above, the judgement cue categories were used in two separate MANOVAs

investigating the frequency of identification across training group and gender. Similar to the

results for individual judgement cues, multivariate analyses isolated a significant difference in judgement cue use across training groups, but not across gender (See Tables 17 and 18).

Univariate analyses determined that the use of the following judgement cue categories differed

across training group: eyebrow movements, cheek/nose movements, breathing patterns, and

timing of facial movements.

Post hoc analyses indicated that the training and training plus feedback groups identified

eyebrow movements more frequently then either the control or feedback groups (p < .01).

Movement of the cheeks or nose were identified most frequently by subjects in the training group,

with the remaining groups showing an equal frequency. Breathing patterns were identified more

frequently by the control (p < .01) and feedback groups (p < .05) than by subjects in the training

group. Subjects who received training.and feedback identified breathing patterns with moderate Table 17

Results of MANOVA on the Effects of Training Group on the use of Cue Type.

df F p<

Multivariate Results followed by Univariate F-tests

45, 312 2.84 .001

Eye Movements 3, 116 0.39 .757

Eyebrow Movements 3, 116 13.08 .001

Nose / Cheek Movements 3, 116 11.08 .001

Mouth Movements 3, 116 0.84 .475

Smiles 3, 116 2.76 .045

Repressed Expressions 3, 116 1.97 .122

Breathing Patterns 3, 116 5.36 .002

Incongruent Facial Actions 3, 116 4.14 .008

Pain Expressions 3, 116 0.31 .818

Timing 3, 116 6.50 .001

Blinking 3, 116 3.73 .013

Exaggerated Movements 3, 116 3.83 .012

Tension 3, 116 1.36 .259

Sudden Movements 3, 116 0.85 .471

Relaxed 3, 116 0.74 .528

Note. Bonferroni adjustments suggest a significance level of .003 84

Table 18

Mean Cue Type Use Across Training Group

Facial Action (AU) Control Feedback Training Train/Feed

M SD M SD M SD M SD

Eyes 19.80 10.11 22.43 13.78 22.57 9.74 22.00 10.92

Eyebrows 3.40 3.47 2.60 2.58 8.70a>b 6.44 8.67a>b 6.29

Nasolabial 0.60 1.19 0.83 1.78 447a,b,d 4.42 1.93 3.13

Mouth 15.57 10.12 15.20 10.92 18.13 8.62 14.60 7:15

Smile 6.73 4.55 4.93 3.04 4.80 3.60 4.23 2.84

Repressed Actions 4.77 4.77 4.70 5.17 7.27 6.20 7.60 7.81

Breathing Patterns 8.97c 6.19 8.37C 9.56 3.13 4.73 4.77 5.03

Incongruent 1.00 1.36 0.37 0.93 2.00b 3.01 1.00 1.20

Pain Expression 3.30 4.91 3.33 5.59 2.60 3.49 2.50 3.00

Timing of Actions 0.97 1.43 0.73 1.62 2.90b 3.13 3.83^b 5.23

Blinking 4.20 3.63 3.67 3.68 5.67 4.92 7.23b 5.64

Exaggerated 4.70 3.92 4.00 4.01 6.37 5.24 7.70b 5.31

Tense 7.77 7.65 7.87 9.21 11.77 10.12 9.60 8.09

Sudden Movements 2.17 3.60 1.43 191 1.13 1.78 1.77 2.86

Relaxed 6.60 7.90 4.13 5.30 6.47 9.21 5.20 6.58

a Mean is significantly greater than control group

b Mean is significantly greater then feedback group

c Mean is significantly greater than training group

d Mean is significantly greater than training / feedback group 85 frequency, and did not differ significantly from the other three groups. The timing of facial

actions was identified as a judgement cue more frequently by the training plus feedback group

than by either the control or feedback groups (p < .01). Subjects in the training group identified timing more frequently than the feedback group (p < .05), while not differing from either the training plus feedback or control groups.

Individual Difference Variables

Pain Experience. In order to determine if subject's degree of experience with pain

influenced their ability to judge pain in others, subjects were asked about the their own incidence

of acute, recurrent and chronic pain, as well as about the pain experiences of their immediate

family. Results showed that 73.3% of subjects reported having experienced acute pain, with

subjects citing surgery, a broken bone, sports injuries and wisdom teeth as the most common

causes. Recurrent pain was experienced by 40.8% of subjects, with the most frequent causes being headaches, stomach aches, ulcers, back pain and cramps. Chronic pain was the least

frequent pain experience, endorsed by only 19.2% of subjects. Chronic pain was most often due

to chronic back pain, sports injuries, broken bones and torn ligaments. With respect to their

family member's pain experiences, 65.8% of subjects stated that someone in their family had

experienced an acute pain episode, with surgery, broken bones and gallstones cited as the most

common causes. Recurrent pain had been observed in the families of 48.3% of subjects, most

often due to headaches and ulcers. Chronic pain was again the least commonly reported pain

experience, reported by 39.2% of subjects. Family members with chronic pain most often

suffered from chronic back pain. (See Appendix L).

An investigation of sex differences determined that females and males reported a similar

amount of experience with pain (selves and family) (t=l .67, p<098). Separating out the various

types of pain however, showed that fewer females (65.0%) than males (81.7%) reported having

an incident of acute pain (x2 = 3.74, p< .05), while more females (53.3%) than males (28.3%)

reported experiencing recurrent pain {y} = 8.24, p< .004). The incidence of chronic pain was

comparable in females and males (21.7%) (x2 = 0.32, p<58), p< .04) With respect to their

family members' pain experiences, more females (56.7%) than males (40.0%) had observed a 86

family member suffering from recurrent pain {y} = 4.09, p< .04), while females and males

observed similar frequencieso f acute (68.3%; 63.3%) and chronic pain ( 46.7%; 31.7%) within

their families.

A measure of overall pain experience was created by adding the number of pain

experiences endorsed by subjects, and correlation analyses were conducted to determine if overall

pain experience was related to subject's judgement accuracy, decision-making confidence or

ratings of pain intensity and unpleasantness. Results showed that general pain experience was not

significantly correlated with any aspect of the subjects' facial expression judgements. Performing

separate analyses for males and females yielded similar results (See Appendix M).

Empathy. Analyses were conducted to determine if there was a relationship between

subject scores on a multidimensional empathy measure and various aspects of facial expression judgements: judgement accuracy, decision-making confidence, and ratings of pain unpleasantness

and intensity. It was predicted that the Perspective-taking subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity

Index would correlate significantly with an individual's ability to detect deception, while the total

IRI score and other sub scale scores (Empathic Concern, Personal Distress and Fantasy) would

not be related to judgement accuracy. Results showed that the total empathy score and the

various subscales were not significantly correlated with either subjects' judgement accuracy,

decision-making confidence or ratings of unpleasantness and intensity (p > .05) (Appendix N).

In order to determine if there were sex difference with respect to the relationship between

empathy and facial expression judgements, correlation analyses were conducted separately for

male and female subjects. Results of the correlation analyses for female subjects paralleled the

overall analyses; neither the overall empathy scores nor the various subscale scores were

significantly correlated with judgement accuracy, decision-making confidence, or ratings of pain

intensity or unpleasantness. Correlation analyses for the male subjects, however, yielded a

different pattern of results. Total scores on the IRI, representing a general empathy score, were

found to be positively correlated with subjects' ratings of the unpleasantness of pain in the patients

facial expressions (p < .05). In addition, scores on the personal distress subscale were found to be

negatively correlated with subject's decision-making confidence (p < .05) and positively correlated 87 with the ratings of pain unpleasantness (p < .01). In general, males who described themselves as more empathic rated other's pain experience as more unpleasant, however, their level of empathy was unrelated to their judgement accuracy, decision-making confidence or their ratings of the intensity of pain experienced. In addition, males who tend to experience feelings of anxiety and unease in their own interpersonal interactions also rated other's pain experience as more unpleasant. Interestingly, they also tended to have less confidence in their own ability to judge

other's facial expressions. The degree of personal distress experienced, however, was not related to judgement accuracy or to ratings of pain intensity.

Comparison of Successful and Unsuccessful Judges

Further explanation for the individual differences in judgement accuracy was sought by

exploring the decision-making process of the most successful judges, those scoring 45% accuracy or above. The comparison group for these analyses was comprised of eleven subjects whose judgement accuracy was 32.5%, which is the median accuracy score, and is approximately equal to the mean for all subjects (32.9%). The distributions of sexes were skewed in both groups;

Eight of the top nine judges were females, as were nine of the eleven subjects with average judgement accuracies. The judgement variables chosen for the analysis included the total number

of cues used per decision, and the seven categories of facial expression cues whose use was found to differ among training groups: eyebrow movements, cheek and nose movements, breathing

patterns, incongruent facial actions, blinking, timing of facial actions and exaggerated facial

actions. Results of the between subjects MANOVA suggested that the groups did not differ

significantly with respect to these aspects of the decision-making process (F = 1.36, p < .306). 88

Discussion

Describing Genuine and Deceptive Pain Expressions

The general purpose of the first study was to provide a comprehensive description of genuine and deceptive pain expressions, including the frequencies and qualitative features of specific facial actions, and relationships among them. This study was the first attempt to analyze genuine and deceptive pain expressions using a more comprehensive version of the FACS coding system which incorporates temporal coding. Results provided support for the position that the empirical description of genuine and deceptive pain expressions could be enhanced by an investigation of the temporal qualities of facial expressions.

Confirming past studies, genuine and deceptive pain expressions were found to differ in the type, frequency, and intensity of facial actions. Deceptive and genuine pain expressions also differed with respect to several temporal parameters, and there was support for the hypothesis that temporal contiguity among facial action components would differ between genuine and deceptive pain expressions. In contrast, results did not support the idea that facial cues thought to be

indicative of lying would be seen with increased frequency in deceptive facial expressions.

Frequency and Intensity of Pain-Related Facial Actions

Genuine Pain Expressions. Past research has identified several facial actions which are

consistently exhibited during genuine facial expressions of pain. These have included an increased

frequency of brow lowering, cheek raising, tightening of the eyelids, raising of the upper lip, parting

of the lips and eye closure (Craig et al., 1991; Prkachin & Mercer, 1989). In the present study,

genuine pain expressions exhibited a greater frequency of brow lowering and parting of the lips than the neutral expression. However, cheek raising / eyelid tightening, nose wrinkle / upper lip raising,

and eye closure were exhibited with equal frequency in genuine pain and neutral expressions.

Previous research has also demonstrated that several pain-related facial actibns were exhibited at a

higher intensity in genuine pain expressions than in neutral expressions. Hadjistavropoulos and

Craig (1994), for example, identified cheek raising / eyelid tightening, nose wrinkle / upper lip raise

and mouth opening as more intense in genuine pain expressions. The current study also isolated 89 differences in the intensity of pain-related facial actions, with genuine pain showing an increased intensity of brow lowering and nose wrinkle / upper lip raise.

Genuine facial expressions in the present study, then, were characterized by an increased frequency and intensity of several pain-related facial actions identified in past research; however, the facial displays did not include the full range of predicted facial actions. One explanation for the reduced range of facial actions differentiating genuine and neutral conditions is that some of the

pain-related facial actions may also have been present in the neutral condition, possibly in anticipation of the upcoming painful event. Facial expressions appear to change systematically

during painful events, with movements in the brow and eye area first evident, followed successively by other facial actions (Prkachin & Mercer, 1989). The current finding that the neutral condition

did not differ in the frequency or intensity of pain-related movements in cheek raise / eyelid tightening or eye closure, along with the finding that genuine facial displays consisted of a more

intense movement in the region of the nose and lips, suggests that neutral expressions may have

exhibited some anticipatory pain-related facial activity. Examination of the mean frequencies of

facial actions across conditions contradicts this interpretation, as neutral facial expressions did not

exhibit any pain-related facial actions other than mouth opening.

Another explanation for the reduced range of facial actions could be that the patients in the

current study did not find the range of motion procedures to be extremely painful. An approximate

comparison of the average intensity and unpleasantness scores to the Gracely descriptor terms

indicates that the range of motion exercises produced pain rated as "barely strong" in intensity and

"slightly distressing" in unpleasantness. Nevertheless, the fact that patients found the pain stimulus

only moderately painful does not account for the reduced range of facial actions during genuine

pain, as patients in the Hadjistavropoulos and Craig (1994) study reported the stimulus to be of

similar intensity, these patients, in comparison, exhibited a much fuller pain display. It is

interesting to note, however, that the patients in the two studies did differ when rating the intensity

and unpleasantness of their daily pain. Patients in the current study described their chronic pain to

be "barely strong" in intensity and "very unpleasant". In comparison, patients in the 90

Hadjistavropoulos and Craig (1994) study rated their daily pain as "strong" and "slightly intolerable".

It is possible that a patient's past pain experience might influence his/her facial displays of pain. Past research provides some support for this explanation. Patients whose pain problems have had a greater psychological and physical impact on their lives have been shown to exhibit a greater intensity and duration of pain-related facial actions in response to a painful stimulus (Prkachin &

Mercer, 1989). Although an increased intensity and unpleasantness of pain does not suggest increased pain-related disability, there are reasons to believe that there may be similar effects on facial expressions of pain. It seems reasonable to assume that patients who experience more intense

pain on a daily basis would approach the range of motion procedure with greater trepidation, for fear of exacerbating their pain. Experiencing anxiety about pain has the effect of directing attention to the pain experience, which in turn leads to stronger pain responses (Arntz, Dreesen &

Merckelbach, 1991). Research has also shown that the increased muscle tension caused by anxiety

can exacerbate musculoskeletal disorders, leading to increased pain nociception (Keefe & Gil,

1986). Similarly, experiencing pain as more unpleasant, may lead to increased negative affect in

response to future pain, which will, in turn, intensify the unpleasant affect and help to perpetuate the

pain (Eich, Rachman & Lopatka, 1990).

Faked Pain Expressions. Faked pain expressions in the present study were comprised of

many facial actions which have been identified as part of the "prototypical" genuine pain display. In

accordance with past research, some of these pain-related facial actions were exhibited with a

greater frequency or intensity than in the genuine pain expression. Two pain-related facial actions,

brow lowering and opened mouth, were seen with greater frequency in the faked pain expression

compared to the genuine pain expressions. Brow lowering was also exhibited at a higher intensity

in the faked pain expressions versus the genuine pain expressions. The remaining pain-related facial

actions were seen with equal frequency and intensity in the genuine and deceptive pain expressions.

Patients in the Hadjistavropoulos and Craig (1994) study produced faked pain expressions which

were more exaggerated than those found in the current study, with several other pain-related facial

actions appearing with greater frequency and intensity. Again, it is possible that a more negative or 91 intense history of pain could exaggerate one's deceptive pain display, due to differences in the meaning and interpretation of pain.

The instruction to fake a pain display also led to an increased frequency of facial actions which are not associated with genuine pain. Results showed that faked pain expressions exhibited an increased frequency and intensity of one non pain-related facial action, brow raise. The frequency and intensity of the remaining non pain-related facial actions investigated did not vary across pain condition. These findings provided some support for the hypothesis that faked expression may contain some "extra" facial actions that are not typical of a genuine facial expression. These non pain-related facial actions may arise because the poser is not consciously aware of what a genuine expression looks like on his/her face. It may also be a result of a person's state or emotions while engaging in deception; for example, there may be individual facial actions present which are usually associated with surprise or embarrassment. It is interesting to note that the "extra" facial actions observed in this study, inner / outer brow raise, is a facial action typically associated with the experience of fear or a startle response. If subjects were faking a pain expression in response to an acute pain stimulus with rapid onset, one might expect elements of a surprise or "startle" expression; however, when faking a response to a familiar pain stimulus which causes an exacerbation of a chronic pain condition, "surprise" would not be an appropriate response. This suggests that people may base their deceptive pain displays on reactions to acute pain, which people have typically experienced and observed more frequently.

In general, patients were fairly good at fabricating a pain display, in that faked pain expressions were, on the whole, appropriately comprised of pain-related facial actions. Based on the amount of experience most people have with pain and pain behaviours, through their personal experiences and observations of others, it is not surprising that pain expressions can be readily assumed. The attempt to fake pain expressions was not wholly successful, however, as a number of pain-related facial actions were seen more frequently and at a greater intensity than during the genuine expression. This suggests that, even when asked to recreate a reaction to a very familiar pain stimulus (physiotherapy exercises), people seem to have a difficult time reconstructing the pain 92 experience. It would be reasonable to assume, then, that people with less frequentan d less recent pain experience would have greater difficulties dissimulating pain.

Masked Pain Expressions. When asked to mask their pain expression during a painful leg lift, patients were very successful at minimizing their pain display, as differences between the masked pain and neutral expression were very subtle. Unlike the faked pain expression, the masking form of deception did not result in any uniquely distinguishing facial features. Masked pain expressions might best be described as being a midpoint between the genuine pain and neutral expressions. When masking pain, patients exhibited a greater frequencyo f mouth opening than during a neutral expression. In addition to mouth opening, there appears to be residual brow lowering, as the frequencyan d intensity of this facial action in masked expressions did not differ

significantly either from the genuine or neutral expressions, while the latter two were significantly different. This picture of masked pain expressions as a midpoint between genuine pain and neutral

expressions may represent the residual facial activity which "leaks out" when attempting to neutralize a genuine pain display. This also suggests that there are subtle facial cues available which would allow the sensitive observer to question a patient's reported lack of pain.

It was hypothesized that any differences in the masked and neutral conditions would consist

of activity in the eye region, which is thought to be more difficult to control. Although the presence

of residual brow lowering provides some evidence for "leakage" in the eye region, it is interesting to

note that the most significant difference between the masked and neutral conditions was increased

mouth movements. When making judgements of another's emotional state based on his/her facial

expressions, observers tend to attach more importance to movements in the area of the eyes and upper face (Lee & Craig, 1991). When deceiving others, there may be a similar awareness that

greater effort should be expended to control movements around the eyes.

The theory that pain expressions change systematically during painful events may provide a

second explanation for the differences between the masked and neutral expressions. Prkachin and

Mercer (1989) suggest that brow lowering and eye closing will be the first facial actions to appear

in response to pain, followed by mid-face actions such as upper lip raise and nose wrinkling. The

final facial actions to appear would be mouth opening, followed by a horizontal stretching of the lips 93 during extreme pain. The increased frequencyo f mouth opening during masked pain suggests that deceivers might exert a reasonable amount of control over their facial actions immediately, with a gradual lessening of control as the urgency of the pain response decreases.

Individual Differences in the Facial Expression of Pain.

Past research has shown that there are large individual differences in the facial actions observed in response to genuine pain. For example, only 45% of the subjects in one study

exhibited one Or both of the most common facial actions, brow lowering and closing of the eyes.

In the current study, the facial actions found to discriminate between genuine pain and neutral

facial expressions (brow lowering arid mouth opening) were exhibited by only 30% and 45% of patients respectively. Some of the variations in pain expression have been related to personal

dispositions, with some people being generally impassive, and others very reactive (Craig, 1992).

Experimental studies have shown that subjects with a low pain tolerance exhibited more

expressive genuine pain displays, suggesting that personal pain thresholds also influence nonverbal pain reactions (Galin & Thorn, 1993). Prkachin and Mercer (1989) found that patients whose

pain problem had a greater psychological and physical impact showed greater facial activity in

response to pain. Finally, pain interventions and coping strategies have substantial effects on self-

reports of pain, and may have a similar impact on facial expression, although this remains an untested proposition.

Similar variables are thought to influence the ability to display deceptive pain expressions.

One such personality variable, role-playing ability, influenced the appearance of genuine and

deceptive pain displays, such that the intensity of faked pain expressions increased with role-

playing ability but was unrelated to the discriminability of the spontaneous and faked pain

expressions (Prkachin, 1992a). Similarly, subjects scoring higher on Pilowsky's general

hypochondriasis factor of the Illness Behaviour Questionnaire displayed genuine and faked pain

expressions which were less intense and harder to discriminate (Prkachin, 1992a). The single

study which related pain symptomatology to deception ability discovered that subjects who rated

the pain experienced as low intensity were better at masking their pain (Galin & Thorn, 1993). It 94 seems reasonable to suggest that many of the variables found to influence genuine pain expressions would also influence the ability to present deceptive pain expressions.

Temporal Qualities of Deceptive Versus Genuine Pain Expressions

Another study hypothesis was that deceptive pain expressions would differ from genuine pain displays in temporal qualities. Ekman and Friesen (1982) suggested that faked facial expressions might have a shorter onset time, a longer period of peak intensity and a longer overall response duration. Results of the present study partially support these findings. Faked pain expressions were found to have an onset time, the length of time from facial action onset to peak intensity, similar to that of genuine pain expressions. The findingso n apex duration were more complicated. Brow lowering had a longer peak intensity in the faked condition; however, there were no differences in apex duration for the remaining pain-related facial actions. Finally, the overall response duration of two pain-related facial actions in the faked pain expression (brow lowering, mouth opening) was significantly longer than that of the genuine pain expressions. These analyses suggest that, when expressions are faked, there is a tendency to exaggerate not only the number and intensity of pain-related facial actions but also the length of peak intensity and the overall length of the facial expression. An investigation of the temporal qualities of the second type of deceptive pain display, masked pain expressions, further reinforces the subtlety of differences among genuine pain and masked pain expressions. None of the temporal variables were found to

differentiate between the three expressions.

Another temporal quality of facial expressions which was of interest in the present study was temporal contiguity, or the closeness with which facial actions appear together on the face.

Unfortunately, the available data did not allow for the full use of established clustering procedures

for facial expressions. Preliminary analyses, in which the non-occurrence of a facial actions was

coded with the group mean, yielded cluster analyses which must be interpreted with caution.

Results provided some support for study hypotheses, however, as pain-related facial actions

appeared together in two of four clusters based on onset times. In comparison, cluster analysis of

faked pain expressions yielded a four factor solution in which pain-related facial actions were

scattered throughout all four clusters. It seems then, that when asked to fake pain, people tend to 95 exhibit the component facial actions in more of a sequential manner than during genuine pain when pain-related facial actions appear more closely together on the face. Again, it appears that the temporal "errors" made when faking pain may arise because the poser is not consciously aware of the timing of genuine pain expressions, and how facial actions appear on his/her face.

Incidence of Deception Cues in Deceptive versus Genuine Pain Expressions

The final analysis investigated the incidence of facial cues thought to be indicative of deception: blinking, facial asymmetry and pulsating apexes. Researchers have suggested that blinking rates will change during deception, either increasing due to an increase in general emotional arousal (Ekman, 1985), or decreasing due to conscious efforts to control one's emotions. The present study did not support either of these hypotheses, as blinking rates did not differ across pain conditions. However, the issue of blinking as an indicator of deception in pain expressions is complicated by the confusion regarding the incidence of blinking during genuine pain, as past

research has suggested both that blinking rates increase (Craig & Patrick, 1985; Patrick et al, 1986) and decrease following pain (Craig et al, 1991; Prkachin, 1992b; Prkachin & Mercer, 1989).

Visual inspection of blinking frequency showed almost identical rates of blinking in neutral, genuine pain, faked pain and masked pain expressions. It might be hypothesized that the unnatural

experimental situation, particularly the prospect of being videotaped, led to an increase in general

arousal which affected blinking rates across all pain conditions. Two other hypothesized deception

cues, facial action asymmetry and pulsating apexes, did not differentiate among neutral, genuine

pain, faked pain and masked pain expressions. Cues thought to be indicative of deception, then, did

not provide additional information to improve the detectability of deceptive and genuine pains

expressions.

Summary

The present study confirmed the hypothesis that increasing the comprehensiveness of FACS

coding to include temporal parameters of facial expressions would provide cues to improve our

description and discrimination of deceptive pain expressions from genuine pain expressions. This

investigation was the first to provide evidence that the facial actions presented during deliberately

dissimulated pain expressions show significantly different temporal patterns than do genuine pain 96 expressions. Patients were fairly good at fabricating a pain display, in that faked pain expressions were appropriately comprised of pain-related facial actions. The attempt to fake pain expressions was not wholly successful, however, as a number of pain-related facial actions were seen more frequently and at a greater intensity than during the genuine expression. Faked pain expressions were also found to remain at peak intensity longer, and last for a longer period of time. In addition, pain-related facial actions did not seem to appear as closely together in time as did those exhibited in a genuine pain expression. Faked pain expressions were also found to have a greater incidence of facial actions unrelated to pain; in this case exhibiting brow raising, a movement often associated with fear.

The differences between genuine pain expressions, masked pain expressions and neutral expressions were much more subtle. Masked pain expressions might best be described as being a midpoint between the genuine pain and neutral expressions, consisting of a greater frequencyo f mouth opening than during a neutral expression, as well as some residual brow lowering. This may represent facial activity which "leaks out" when attempting to neutralize a genuine pain display.

The results suggest that it would be very difficult to discriminate neutral and masked pain expressions; however, there are subtle facial cues available which would allow the sensitive observer to question a patient's reported lack of pain.

It is obvious that the temporal qualities of facial expressions provide information both when patients are projecting pain they don't feel, and when they are attempting to mask a genuine pain experience. Further research is needed to determine the level of discrimination among the pain conditions that these additional cues will provide. The aim of the second study was to determine if observers' judgement skills could be improved by increasing their knowledge and awareness of the various cues to deception elicited in the present study.

Detecting Genuine and Deceptive Pain Displays

The purpose of the second study was twofold: (1) to determine if there are individual differences in the ability to detect deceptive pain displays and (2) to determine if people's ability to detect faked or masked pain can be improved through the use of various training strategies. The present study was the first to investigate individual differences in the judgement of deception in 97 pain patients, by comparing successful and unsuccessful judges on variables postulated to influence the decision-making process. Results indicated that there were significant individual differences in judgement accuracy, ranging from accuracy levels well below chance to a 63% accuracy level, when the base rate was 25%. However, none of the decision-making variables explained the differences between good "lie detectors" and average judges.

The training program used in the current study was based on the various cues to deception elicited in the first study, and attempted to increase the breadth and acuity of judges' decision• making procedures. Subjects receiving training, corrective feedback or training plus feedback were compared to one another and to a control group of naive observers on their ratings of the intensity and unpleasantness of pain experienced by the patient, as well as accuracy of detecting deception. Results indicated that providing corrective feedback led to significant improvements in detection accuracy across all types of pain expressions. In contrast, an information-based training program was ineffective, as the average judgement accuracy of this group was no better than that of control subjects.

Judgements of Intensity and Unpleasantness

In previous research, judges were largely fooled by patients' attempts to fake their pain, and consistently attributed more pain to the faked pain faces than to the genuine and masked pain faces (Poole & Craig, 1992). In the present study, judges attributed more pain to the genuine pain expressions than they did to the faked pain expressions, despite the fact that the faked pain expressions were shown to be comprised of an equal, if not greater frequency and intensity of pain-related facial actions in study one. Using the DDS descriptor terms closest to the mean ratings observed, subjects rated genuine pain to be "moderate" in intensity and "unpleasant", while faked pain displays were describes as "moderate" and only "slightly annoying". Obviously, subjects were not fooled by the faked pain display, in that they did not equate a stronger pain display with either increased intensity or unpleasantness. Subjects rated the faked pain expressions to be equal in pain intensity and unpleasantness to the masked pain expressions. It would seem that while the judges did not believe the exaggerated pain displays, neither were they comfortable stating that the patient was experiencing no pain. 98

Similar to the judges in the Poole and Craig (1992) study, judges in the current study rated the pain intensity and unpleasantness of masked pain expressions to be greater than the neutral

facial expressions, but less than that of the genuine pain expressions. The descriptor term

equivalents for their ratings were "moderate" intensity and "slightly annoying" for the masked pain

expressions, while neutral expressions were rated to be of "very weak" intensity and only "slightly unpleasant". These results suggest that subjects were able to discern some pain in the masked

pain expressions, an impressive distinction given the relative scarcity of facial actions which

differentiated the three expressions in the FACS analysis.

For the neutral, masked pain and genuine pain expressions, subjects' ratings of pain intensity and unpleasantness roughly corresponded to the frequency and number of pain related

facial actions isolated through FACS coding of the facial expressions. While rating the faked pain

display, however, they did not equate a stronger pain display with either increased pain intensity

or unpleasantness. One way to clarify the way in which subjects' judgements of genuine pain and

faked pain differed would be to determine which cues helped them to make their decisions

regarding pain intensity and unpleasantness. Further analyses could look at the FACS coding of

patients' genuine and deceptive pain expressions in order to determine which cues or combination

of cues best predicted subjects' pain ratings.

Compared to the judges in the Poole and Craig (1992) study, therefore, subjects were less

likely to be fooled by patients' attempts to fake their pain experience. The increased accuracy of

pain judgements in the current study may be explained by the explicit focus on detecting

deception. The awareness of deception alone should not account for the increased accuracy in

ratings of faked pain. Past research has shown that "priming" a subject to expect deception does

not increase his/her accuracy, but instead leads to a decreased willingness to attribute pain to both

genuine and deceptive pain expressions (Poole & Craig, 1992). Nonetheless, the improved

accuracy cannot be attributed to detection training, as there were no differences in judgements of

pain intensity or unpleasantness between the control group and any of the training groups.

Finally, different study methodologies might account for the difference in results between the two

studies. For example, it is possible that being asked to identify the facial cues they used to make 99 their judgements might have influenced the decision-making process Of subjects in the current

study.

Discrimination of Genuine and Deceptive Pain Expressions

When asked to categorize observed facial expressions as neutral, genuine pain, faked pain

or masked pain, judges' average judgement accuracy was 32.9%, with their accuracy for neutral

expressions being significantly higher than that for any of the three pain expressions. In fact, the

categorization of neutral facial expressions was the only expression identified at higher than

chance levels. This may, in part, be explained by subjects' tendency to overuse the neutral

expression category. Judges were not informed that all four expressions were presented for each

patient, and they identified expressions as neutral more frequently than other categories. Subjects' tendency to use the neutral category excessively suggests that the patients' facial expressions often

did not provide much information with which to make a decision. Past research, which has

established that people have a general tendency to underestimate patients' reports of pain (Lander,

1990), may also help to explain an increased likelihood of rating a facial expression as neutral.

It is interesting to note that subjects' perceived judgement accuracy mirrored their actual judgement accuracy. Judges felt that they were most accurate at identifying neutral expressions, with the three pain expressions identified much less accurately. Although the relative detection

accuracies of the various facial expression types was correct, their perceived accuracy was twice

that of their actual judgement accuracy for all four facial expression categories.

Past research has shown little evidence for a relationship between subjects' overall

confidence in their detection ability and their actual overall detection accuracy (DePaulo &

Pfeiffer, 1986; Kohnken, 1987). Based on the assumption that subjects would presumably predict

some "deceivers" better than others, the present study looked at subjects' decision-making

confidence for each individual case. Results showed that judges were most confident in their

ability to detect neutral and faked pain expressions, followed by judgements of genuine pain, with

decisions about masked pain expressions being the least confident. Overall, subjects were overly

confident in their judgement ability, suggesting that they did not fully appreciate the difficulty of

the judgement task. 100

It is interesting to note that although subjects' perceived accuracy ratings suggest that they were aware that their judgement of faked expressions was less accurate then that of neutral

expressions, on an individual case basis, they were equally confident in neutral and faked expressions. This divergence between decision-making confidence and judgement accuracy

across pain conditions may be explained by the clarity of decision-making strategies subjects use for each pain condition. Similar to the current study, subjects in Hadjistavropoulos et al's (1996)

study reported greater overall decision-making confidence for neutral and faked pain expressions

compared to genuine and masked pain expressions. An investigation of the facial cues which

were present on the patients' faces when subjects rated expressions as neutral, genuine, masked or faked pain allowed investigators to describe the decision-making strategies used for each

condition. Judges were found to have well defined strategies for making decisions of faked pain

or neutral expressions, while they had less well defined methods of identifying the genuine and

masked pain conditions. It is iniportant to note, however, that the Hadjistavropoulos et al (1996)

results describe cues which were present on the face when certain judgements were made;

therefore, it does not necessarily identify the cues actually used by the judges, nor did it answer the question of whether judges would be able to articulate their decision-making strategies.

In the current study, judges were asked to describe the facial cues they used to make each judgement, providing the first in-depth investigation of subjects' actual decision-making process.

Results indicated that subjects identified a greater number of facial cues in the faked and genuine

facial expressions than in either the masked or neutral expressions. Although the first study

determined that faked pain expressions were comprised of a greater frequencyan d intensity of

facial actions than genuine pain, subjects in the current study reported using the same number of

facial cues to identify faked and genuine pain expressions. Study methodology may account for

this result, as subjects were asked to list the cues they used in their judgements rather than being

asked to list all cues available on the face. Subjects also identified more facial cues in the masked

pain expression than in the neutral expression, which suggests that observers could perceive the

very subtle facial cues which differentiated the masked pain and neutral facial expressions in the

first study. In general, comparing these results to the FACS description of patients' pain 101 expressions suggests that subjects were fairly perceptive in identifying and reporting the quantitative increase in facial expressions corresponding to neutral, masked, genuine and faked pain expressions.

A comparison of the facial cues subjects identified with the various pain expressions and the facial actions which actually comprise the various expressions provides some information on the efficiency of subjects' decision-making process. For example, in the first study, brow lowering was found to occur more frequently in genuine and masked pain expressions than in neutral expressions, with faked pain expressions showing the greatest frequency of brow lowering. The subjects' use of brow lowering as a facial cue in judging facial expressions shows a similar pattern. Brow lowering was identified as a facial cue most frequently when subjects rated a facial expression as faked, followed by genuine and masked pain expressions, with brow lowering being identified as a cue less frequently in the neutral expression than in the genuine pain expression. The use of brow raise, a non pain-related facial action also showed a pattern similar to that seen in the actual facial expressions. Subjects use of cues which were empirically shown not to differentiate among pain conditions was less accurate. Eye closure was correctly used with equal frequency when judging the various pain conditions, however, other facial actions, such as eye squint and tight lips were identified with progressively greater frequency in neutral, masked, genuine and faked pain expressions. In general then, it seems that in many cases, subjects may have made their judgements based on the number of facial actions they perceived.

The Effects of Training on Facial Expression Judgements

Results of the second study provided partial support for the position that deception training would improve detection of the various types of facial expressions. As opposed to studies in the general deception field (Kohnken, 1987; Zuckerman et al., 1985), providing corrective feedback led to significant improvements in detection accuracy. Subjects in the feedback group showed a significantly greater judgement accuracy than either control group or training group subjects, and the advantage of feedback appeared to be maintained across all types of pain expressions, unlike the 102

Galin and Thorn (1993) study, where feedback subjects were more accurate than control subjects only when identifying faked and genuine pain expressions.

Changes in the feedback procedure may account for the increased utility of feedback as a training strategy. As opposed to studies in the deception and pain fields (Galin & Thorn, 1993;

Kohnken, 1987; Zuckerman et al., 1985), the current study used immediate rather than delayed

feedback; therefore the information was more salient to the judgement task and allowed subjects

to improve their detection technique on a case by case basis. In addition, when provided with

feedback, subjects were not simply informed whether or not their judgements were correct.

Instead, they were told precisely which type of face was being depicted in each video clip.

Therefore, subjects in the current study were provided with more information than typical of past

feedback paradigms. As a result, subjects were given an opportunity to compare the faces

associated with the different pain categories. Results would suggest that subjects were then

successful at abstracting unique cues associated with the different pain conditions. These results

suggest that immediate feedback is more effective than the delayed feedback used in past studies,

and suggests that with detailed information, judges can be made aware of the decision-making

cues which have or have not been effective for them in their judgement task and can benefit from

this information.

In contrast, results of the present study did not provide support for an information-based

training program, as the average judgement accuracy of this group was no better than that of

control subjects. There was also no support for the possibility of additive effects of providing

both training and corrective feedback, as the feedback only group performed equally well. Thus,

a training program providing detailed information about the cues that would facilitate the

discrimination between the categories of facial expressions was not effective in the absence of

opportunities to carefully inspect the faces and deliberate upon the usefulness of the various cues

available. As well, the training program did not enhance the successful discriminations that could

be made on the strength of feedback alone. It is likely that the feedback condition was sufficiently

instructive that the information provided by the training program could add little to that which 103

could be learned through inspecting instances of the various expressions and receiving corrective feedback.

Several aspects of the decision-making process were investigated in an attempt to explain the relative superiority of subjects in the feedback groups. For example, it was hypothesized that more effective judges would report the use of both a greater number of facial cues and different types of facial cues in making their decisions. However, despite their greater accuracy, subjects in the feedback group believed they used the same number of facial cues as subjects in any of the other training groups. A comparison of the training groups' reported cue use shows that the

feedback group did not identify any unique facial cues not used by the remaining groups. In particular, the types of cues reported by subjects in the feedback group did not differ significantly

from those reported by the control group, which suggests that the cues reported by the feedback

subjects were also those used intuitively by naive judges. As the judgement accuracies of the two

groups were significantly different, subjects' reported cue use does not explain the relative

superiority of the feedback group. It is possible that feedback subjects may not have been able to

perceive and/or articulate their decision-making strategies. It is also possible that although the two groups used similar facial cues, these cues either assumed a different meaning for the

feedback group, or were used more effectively. In order to answer these questions, subjects' judgements and their reported cue use would have to be directly compared to the facial cues

available in the patients' various expressions.

The poor performance of subjects in the training groups was not due to their inability to

learn and retain the material presented in the training package, as scores on a training test were

fairly high. In addition, an examination of the types of cues reported by the training groups

suggests that they actively used the information presented in the training manual. For example,

subjects in the training groups reported the greatest use of pain-related facial cues, such as brow

lowering, eye closure, eye squinting, nose / cheek movements. Other cues to deception

presented in the training manual were also identified more frequently by the training groups, such

as exaggerated facial actions, and the timing of facial actions. It is also interesting to note that the

feedback and control groups used only one type of detection cue, breathing patterns, more 104 frequently than the groups which received training. Breathing patterns, a detection cue not included in the training package, and therefore disregarded by subjects in the training groups, could then be considered to be used intuitively.

Compared to the feedback group, the treatment group reported an increased frequency of the following facial cues: eyebrow movements, nose / cheek movements, incongruent facial actions, temporal cues, and lowered eyebrows. Compared to the feedback group, the treatment plus feedback group reported using an increased frequency of the following facial cues: eyebrow movements, temporal cues, blinking, exaggerated movements, eye squint, and lowered eyebrows.

The training group differed from the training plus feedback only in an increased use of nose/cheek movements and lowered eyebrows. It appears that, when given the chance to deliberate upon the usefulness of cues presented in the training program, subjects in the training plus feedback group

decreased their reported use of nose/cheek movements and lowered eyebrows. It is interesting to

note that nose/cheek and lowered eyebrow movements helped to differentiate faked pain expressions from genuine, masked and neutral expressions in study one, while lowered eyebrows

helped to differentiate genuine pain expressions from neutral expressions. Perhaps it was difficult to perceive the difference in frequencyo f these variables among faked, genuine and masked pain

expressions.

Individual Differences in Judgement Accuracy

Despite the discouragingly low level of judgement accuracy observed in the current study,

it would be misleading to conclude that subjects were unable to discriminate genuine and

deceptive pain displays, as there were substantial individual differences in judgement accuracy.

Judgement accuracy ranged from a dismal 18% to 63%; which is an impressive four standard

deviations above the mean. There was a fair amount of variation in subjects' ability to identify the

various expressions, with 22 subjects scoring a standard deviation below the mean, and 16 scoring

higher than one standard deviation above the mean. The findingo f substantial individual

differences in detection accuracy corroborates findings in the general deception literature (Ekman

& O'Sullivan, 1991), but is in opposition to the lack of variation in the recent Hadjistavropoulos et

al (1996) study. This discrepancy could easily be an artifact of the forced choice design used in 105 the latter study. Several variables were investigated in order to explain the variation in accuracy

scores found in the current study.

Sex Differences. Unlike past research (Poole & Craig, 1992), the current study found

significant sex differences in the ability to detect deceptive facial expressions of pain, with female judges having a significantly higher overall accuracy level than males across all facial expression types. It is difficult to explain the observed sex difference in judgement accuracy, however, as there were no gender differences in the other facial expression judgements. For example, males'

and females' ratings of the pain intensity and unpleasantness exhibited in patients' facial expression were similar across all four types of facial expression, therefore, gender differences in accuracy were not due to differences in the ability to perceive a quantitative degree of "distress" in facial

expressions. It is also interesting to note that there were no gender differences in subjects'

subjective ratings of their own performance. Despite females' greater judgement accuracy, males

and females did not differ with respect to their confidence in their categorization of facial

expressions, nor did their perception of their overall accuracy differ.

The observed sex differences in judgement accuracy were also unrelated to any of the

decision-making process variables included in the study. For example, the lack of interaction

effects between sex and training group indicates that there were no gender differences in the

ability to benefit from the various training approaches. With respect to the use of facial cues, the

Hurd and Noller (1988) study suggested that females use a slower, cue-based decision-making

approach, while males used a quicker, intuitive approach and reported fewer cues, results which

contradict Buck's (1984) theory. In the present study, males and females reported using the same

number of cues during their judgement process. There was also no significant difference in the type of facial cues identified by males and females. The current study then, provides no support

for the idea that men and women differ in their use of an intuitive versus cue-based decision•

making approach. Nevertheless, men and women did differ in their detection ability. It is

possible, therefore, that while there were no gender differences in the perception of facial cues,

males and females might differ in their interpretation of facial movements. 106

Popular stereotypes portray women as being more sensitive, more understanding, and more aware of others' feelings (Hall & Briton, 1993). Research has isolated gender differences in the area of emotional expression which support stereotypes of nonverbal behaviour. Survey data has shown that women are believed to use more expressive and involved nonverbal behaviours than men, and to be more skilled at both sending and receiving nonverbal messages (Briton &

Hall, 1995). Observational studies have generally confirmed people's perceptions of gender differences in nonverbal behaviour. Women were found to be more alert to nonverbal behaviour in general (Hall, 1984), and were better able to decode nonverbal cues of emotion (Hall, 1978;

Hall, 1984; Costanzo & Archer, 1989). The process by which females become better encoders and decoders of nonverbal behaviour is unclear; however, it is thought that gender role

socialization plays an important role. In Western cultures, girls and women are encouraged to be emotionally expressive, while boys are often taught to suppress their emotions. As a result, men may learn that the nonverbal communication of emotion is relatively unimportant, while for women nonverbal behaviours may merit a greater focus of attention (Briton & Hall, 1995).

Pain Experience & Empathy. It was hypothesized that subjects who had more personal experience with various types of pain, or who had observed pain more often in others might be

more accurate in their judgements of deceptive and genuine pain. Results did not support this hypothesis, nor was there any relationship between pain experience and subjects' ratings of pain

intensity and unpleasantness.

It was also thought that individuals high in empathy might be better "lie detectors", as the

concept of empathy suggests not only an emotional identification with others, but also an increased social acuity. A multidimensional measure of empathy, the IRI, which includes a person

perception type subscale was used to correlate various aspects of empathy with judgements of pain. Results of the correlation analyses for female subjects paralleled the overall analyses; neither

the overall empathy scores nor the various subscale scores were significantly correlated with judgement accuracy, decision-making confidence, or ratings of pain intensity or unpleasantness.

Correlation analyses for the male subjects, however, yielded a different pattern of results. In general, males who described themselves as more empathic rated others' pain experience as more 107

unpleasant; however, their level of empathy had no effect on their judgement accuracy, decision•

making confidence or their ratings of the intensity of pain experienced. In addition, males who

tend to experience feelings of anxiety and unease in their own interpersonal interactions also rated

others' pain experience as more unpleasant. Interestingly, they also tended to have less confidence

in their own ability to judge others' facial expressions. The degree of personal distress

experienced, however, was not related to judgement accuracy or to ratings of pain intensity.

Comparison of Successful and Unsuccessful Judges. Further explanation for the individual

differences in judgement accuracy was sought by comparing the decision-making process of the

most successful judges, those scoring 45% accuracy or above, and those subjects whose

judgement accuracy was at the mean/median for all subjects (32.5%). The distribution of sexes

were skewed in both groups; Eight of the top nine judges were females, as were nine of the

\ eleven subjects with average judgement accuracy's. The decision-making process for these two

groups did not differ either in the number or type of facial cues reported.

Summary

In the current study, subjects' ratings of pain intensity and unpleasantness roughly

corresponded to the frequency and number of pain-related facial actions isolated through FACS

coding of the facial expressions. While rating the faked pain display, however, they did not

equate a stronger pain display with either increased pain intensity or unpleasantness. Unlike the

subjects in Poole and Craig's (1992) study, then, subjects in the current study were not fooled by

the faked pain display. However, when asked to categorize facial expressions as neutral, genuine

pain, faked pain or masked pain, the judges' accuracy levels were disappointingly low, averaging

only 32.9% when the base rate would have been 25%. However, it would be misleading to

conclude that subjects were unable to discriminate among genuine and deceptive pain displays, as

there were significant differences in judgement accuracy, ranging from 18.0 to 63.0%.

Several variables were analyzed in an attempt to explain the wide variation in judgement

accuracy, however, accuracy was unrelated to either the number or type of reported facial cues

used, the subjects previous pain experience or their empathy scores on the IRI. Females' 108 judgement accuracy was significantly higher than males' accuracy, but again, there were no sex differences in any of the decision-making process variables measured.

Results of the study provided partial support for the position that training would improve detection of the various types of facial expressions. Providing immediate, rather than the delayed feedback used in past research, led to significant improvements in subjects' detection accuracy. It would appear that, when given the opportunity to compare the faces associated with the different pain categories, subjects became aware of the decision-making cues which have or have not been effective for them and can benefit from this information. In comparison, there was no support for an information-based training program. Once again, the number and type of facial cues used in the decision-making process of teh feedback group did not differ from that of the control group,

suggesting that while feedback subjects used only the cues used intuitively by naive judges, they may have assumed a different meaning or simply have been used more effectively.

Future Directions

In general, future research needs to further explore individual differences in: a) people's

ability to deceive others through facial expression, and b) people's ability to detect deception. The first study increased the comprehensiveness of the current description of genuine and deceptive

pain displays by incorporating temporal parameters, as well as an analysis of the frequency and intensity of pain-related and non pain-related facial actions. The study did not analyze the

difference in these parameters between patients who were good deceivers and those who were bad deceivers, nor did it investigate pain history variables as a possible predictor of deception

ability. It would be interesting to know what makes a patient more accurate at posing a pain

display, and therefore more difficult to detect. Do they experience less pain-related worry and

emotionality? Do their strategies for coping with pain differ? For example, if someone tends to

cope with pain by ignoring it, are they better able to mask their pain expressions? If someone

tends to exhibit considerable pain behaviour in an attempt to communicate their distress, will their faked pain expressions be more exaggerated? It would also be interesting to look at faked pain

expressions which were successful to see how closely the FACS coding approximates genuine

pain expressions on the various parameters: frequency, intensity and temporally. 109

With respect to the ability to detect deception, further research is needed to investigate the decision-making strategies which differentiate successful versus unsuccesful detectors. Reported

cue use did not provide any information which would help explain the superiority of females' judgement accuracy or that of the feedback group. It seems likely that while they report using the

same facial cues, they may be using them in a more effective manner. A comparison of empirical prediction using FACS coding, and the clinical prediction of judges might allow us to determine whether judges were using the available facial cues efficiently.

Finally, the present study indicated that the use of deception training in the form of

corrective feedback can improve subjects' detection accuracy. The improvement in accuracy was fairly small, and might not be considered clinically significant, but must be evaluated in light of the

short duration of training. It would be of clinical interest to determine if greater exposure to genuine and deceptive facial expressions using a corrective feedback approach would lead to

further gains in judgement accuracy.

In general, the current study determined that there is information in facial expressions

which may be useful to observers when assessing the credibility of pain experiences, and that one

can improve the detection skills of naive judges by increasing their awareness of available facial

cues. However, this research did not provide an explanation for the significant individual

differences in either people's ability to pose deceptive facial expressions and their ability to detect

deception. These questions are of considerable clinical interest and importance, and point to a

potentially productive area of research.

Limits to Generalizability

Past research has shown that clinicians use nonverbal information to form clinical judgements during pain assessments. The current study helped to clarify the decision-making

process that judges use when assessing pain experience through facial expression, and provided

support for the position that training would improve the detection of deception. When given the

opportunity to compare genuine and deceptive pain expressions, subjects became aware of the

decision-making cues which have or have not been effective for them and were able to benefit 110 from this information. However, several factors impede the generalizability of these results to a clinical setting.

First, judgement accuracy in the current study was generally low, suggesting that one

cannot confidently make decisions regarding the veracity of pain reports based solely on facial expression. In addition, the significant individual differences in pain expressions suggest that training judges to use consistent "rules" for detecting deception through facial expression would not be appropriate

Second, the information on genuine and deceptive facial expressions obtained in this study was collected in a clinical setting, but were subject to artificial conditions, such as videotaping, which may have influenced the patients' pain presentations. Similarly, the judgement task used in the current study focused solely on facial expression, and differed significantly from the judgement task of a clinician. Clinicians have many sources of information available to them when

conducting pain assessments, including medical records, self-report and nonverbal cues. As a result, deception cues based on facial expressions would be weighed against the remaining

available information. The purpose of the current study was to determine how one could improve

a judge's use of one type of deception cue available. Additional research is needed to determine the process by which judges combine information from the various modalities during clinical

decision-making. Furthermore, the need to construct validated protocols for enhancing the

accuracy of detection precludes the direct application of these findings to clinical settings at this time. Ill References

Arntz, A., Dreessen, L. & Merckelbach, H. (1991). Attention, not anxiety, influences pain. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 29. 41-50.

Benton, A.L. (1980). The neuropsychology of facial recognition. American Psychologist. 35. 176-186.

Bernstein, W.M. & Davis, M.H. (1982). Perspective-taking, self-consciousness and accuracy in person perception. Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 3. 1-19.

Boucher, J.D. (1969). Facial displays of fear, sadness and pain. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 28. 239-242

Briton, N.J. & Hall, J.A. (1995). Beliefs about female and male nonverbal communication. Sex Roles. 32. 79-90.

Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. New York: Guildford Press.

Cacioppo, J.T. & Petty, R.E. (1981). Lateral asymmetry in the expression of cognition and emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 7. 333-341.

Chlopan, BE, McCain, ML., Carbonell, JL. & Hagen, R.L. (1985). Empathy: Review of available measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 48. 635-653.

Clark, K.B. (1980). Empathy: A neglected topic in psychological research. American Psychologist. 35. 187-190.

Costanzo, M. & Archer, D. (1989). Interpreting the expressive behaviour of others: The Interpersonal Perception Task (IPT). Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour. 13. 225-245.

Craig, K.D. (1986). Social modeling influences: Pain in context. In R.A. Sternbach (Ed.), The psychology of pain. New York: Raven Press.

Craig, K.D. (1992). The facial expression of pain: Better than a thousand words? American Pain Society Journal. 1. 153-162.

Craig, K.D. (1994). Emotional aspects of pain. In P.D. Wall & R. Melzack (Eds.) Textbook of pain. 3rd edition. Edinburgh: Churchill-Livingstone.

Craig, K.D., Hyde, S. & Patrick, C.J. (1991). Genuine, supressed, and faked facial behaviour during exacerbation of chronic low back pain. Pain, 46, 161-172.

Craig, K.D. & Patrick, C.J. (1985). Facial expression during induced pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 48. 1080-1091. 112 Craig, KD. & Prkachin, K.M. (1980). Social influences on public and private components of pain. In I.G. Sarason and C. Spielberger (Eds.), Stress and anxiety. ("Vol. 7). New York: Hemisphere.

Craig, K.D. & Prkachin, K.M. (1983). Nonverbal measures of pain. In R. Melzack (Ed.), Pain measurement and assessment. New York: Raven Press.

Craig, K.D., Prkachin, K.M. & Grunau, R.V.E. (1992). The facial expression of pain. In D.C. Turk & R. Melzack (Eds.). Handbook of pain assessment. New York. Guilford Press.

Crook, J., Rideout, E. & Brown, G. (1984). The prevalence of pain complaints in a general pain population. Pain. 18.299-314.

Davis, M. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology. 10. 85.

Davis, M.H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44. 113- 126.

DePaulo, BM. & Pfeifer, R.L. (1986). On the job experience and skill at detecting deception. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 16. 249-267.

DePaulo, B.M., Rosenthal, R., Eisenstat, R.A., Rogers, P.C. & Finkelstein, S. (1978). Decoding discrepant nonverbal cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 36. 313-323.

Eich, E., Rachman, S. & Lopatka, C. (1990). Affect, pain and autobiographical memory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 99. 174-178.

Ekman, P. (1980). Facial asymmetry. Science. 209. 833-834.

Ekman, P. (1985). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in the marketplace, and politics. New York: Norton.

Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Nonverbal leakage and clues to deception. Psychiatry. 32. 88-105.

Ekman, P. & Friesen, W.V. (1974). Detecting deception from the body or face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 29. 288-298.

Ekman, P. & Friesen, W.V. (1978a). Investigator's guide to the Facial Action Coding System. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Ekman, P. & Friesen, W.V. (1978b). Manual for the Facial Action Coding System. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. 113 Ekman, P & Friesen, W.V. (1982). Felt, false and miserable smiles. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 6. 238-249.

Ekman, P. & Friesen, W.V. (1983). EMFACS: Coders instructions. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Human Interaction Laboratory, San Francisco, CA.

Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & O'Sullivan, M. (1988). Smiles when lying. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 414-420.

Ekman, P., Friesen, W.V., O'Sullivan, M. & Scherer, K. (1991). Invited article: Face, voice and body in detecting deceit. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 15. 125-135.

Ekman, P., Hager, J.C. & Friesen, W.V. (1981). The symmetry of emotional and deliberate facial actions. Psychophysiology. 18. 101-106.

Ekman, P., & O'Sullivan, M. (1991). Who can catch a liar? American Psychologist, 46. 913- 920.

Faust, D., Hart, K.J., Guilmette, T.J. & Arkes, HR. (1988). Neuropsychologists' capacity to detect adolescent malingerers. Professional Psychology: Research & Practice. 19. 508- 515.

Fridlund, A.J. (1991). Evolution and facial action in reflex, social motive, and paralanguage. Biological Psychology. 32. 3-100.

Friesen, W.V. (1982). Program for integrating FACS scoring ('CLUSTER''). Unpublished manuscript, University of California, San Francisco, CA.

Galin, K.E. & Thorn, B.E. (1993). Unmasking pain: Detection of deception in facial expressions. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 12. 182-197.

Gracely, R., Dubner, R. & McGrath, P. (1979). Narcotic analgesia: Fentanyl reduces the intensity but not the unpleasantness of painful pulp sensations. Science. 203, 1261-1263.

Gracely, R., McGrath, P. & Dubner, R. (1978a). Ratio scales of sensory and affective verbal pain descriptors. Pain. 5. 5-18.

Gracely, R., McGrath, P. & Dubner, R. (1978b). Validity and sensitivity of ratio scales of sensory and affective verbal pain descriptors: Manipulation of affect by diazepam. Pain. 5, 19-29.

Hadjistavropoulos, H.D. & Craig, K.D. (1994). Acute and chronic low back pain: Cognitive, affective and behavioral dimensions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 62. 341-349.

Hadjistavropoulos, H.D., Craig, K.D., Hadjistavropoulos, T. & Poole, G.D. (1996). Subjective judgments of deception in pain expression: Accuracy and errors. Pain. 65. 247-254. 114 Hager, J.C. & Ekman, P. (1985). The asymmetry of facial actions is inconsistent with models of hemispheric specialization. Psychophysiology, 22. 307-318.

Hall, J.A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin. 85, 845- 857.

Hall, J.A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore, MD: John's Hopkins University Press.

Hall, J.A. & Briton, N.J. (1993). Gender, nonverbal behavior and expectations. In P.D. Blanck (Ed) Interpersonal expectations: Theory, research and applications.

Heller, W. & Levy, J. (1981). Perception and expression of emotion in right-handers and left• handers. Neuropsychologia. 19, 263-272.

Hogan, R. (1969). Development of an empathy scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 33. 307-316.

Holland, M.K. & Tarlow, G. (1972). Blinking and mental load. Psychological Reports. 323. 119-127.

Holland, M.K. & Tarlow, G. (1975). Blinking and thinking. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 41. 403-406.

Hurd, K., & Noller, P. (1988). Decoding deception: A look at the process. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 12. 217-233.

Izard, C.E. (1990). Facial expression and the regulation of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 58. 487-498.

Jacox, A.K. (1980). The assessment of pain. In L. Smith, H. Merskey & S. Grass (Eds.). Pain, meaning and management. New York: Spectrum.

Johnson, M. (1977). Assessment of clinical pain. In A.K. Jakox (Ed.). Pain: A sourcebook for nurses and other health professionals. Boston: Little, Brown.

Keefe, F.J. & Block, A.R. (1982). Development of an observation method for assessing pain behavior in chronic low back pain patients. Behavior Therapy. 13. 363-375.

Keefe, F.J., Brantley, A., Manuel, G. & Crisson, J.E. (1985). Behavioral assessment of head and neck cancer pain. Pain. 23, 327-336.

Keefe, F.J. & Gil, K.M. (1986). Behavioral concepts in the analysis of chronic pain syndromes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 54. 776-783.

Kleck, R.E., Vaughan, R.C., Cartwright-Smith, J., Vaughan, K.B., Colby, C.Z. & Lanzetta, J.T. (1976). Effects of being observed on expressive, subjective, and physiological 115 responses to painful stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 1211- 1218.

Kohnken, G. (1987). Training police officers to detect deceptive eyewitness statements: Does it work? Social Behaviour. 2, 1-17.

Kotarba, J.A. (1983). Chronic pain: Its social dimensions. Beverly Hills: Sage.

Kraut, R.E. & Poe, D. (1980). On the line: The deception judgements of customs inspectors and laymen. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 39, 784-798.

Lander, J. (1990). Clinical judgements in pain management. Pain, 42, 15-22.

Lee, D.S. & Craig, K.D. (1991). Facial action determinants of observer pain judgements: Complexity and configuration. Unpublished manuscript, University of British Columbia, Vancouver.

LeResche, L. (1982). Facial expression in pain: A study of candid photographs. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour. 7. 46-56.

LeResche, L. & Dworkin, S.F. (1988). Facial expression of pain and emotions in chronic TMD patients, Pain. 35. 71-78.

LeResche, L., Ehrlick, K.J. & Dworkin, S. (1990). Facial expressions of pain and masking smiles: Is "Grin and it" a pain behavior? Pain. Suppl. 5. S286.

Ley, R.G. & Bryden, M.F. (1981). Consciousness, emotion, and the right hemisphere. In R. Stevens & G. Underwood (Eds.) Aspects of consciousness: Structural issues. New York: Academic Press.

Mayer, T.G., Gatchel, R.J., Kishino, N., Keeley, J., Mayer, H, Capra, P. & Mooney, V. 1986). A prospective short-term study of low back pain patients utilizing novel objective functional measurement. Pain. 25. 53-68.

Mehrabian, A. & Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 40,525-543.

Moore, B.S. (1990). The origins and development of empathy. Motivation and Emotion. 14, 75- 80.

Patrick, C.J., Craig, K.D. & Prkachin, KM. (1986). Observer judgements of acute pain: Facial action determinants. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1291-1298.

Poole, G. D., & Craig, K. D. (1992). Judgements of Genuine, Suppressed, and Faked Facial Expressions of Pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 797-805.

Prkachin, K.M. (1992a). Dissociating spontaneous and deliberate expressions of pain: Signal detection analyses. Pain. 51. 57-65. 116

Prkachin, K.M. (1992b). The consistency of facial expressions of pain: A comparison across modalities. Pain. 51. 297-306.

Prkachin, K.M. & Craig, K.D. (1985). Influencing non-verbal expressions of pain: Signal detection analysis. Pain. 21. 399-409.

Prkachin, K.M., Currie, N.A. & Craig, K.D. (1983). Judging nonverbal expressions of pain. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science. 15. 43-73.

Prkachin, K.M., Solomon, P.S., Hwang, T. & Mercer, S.R. (1995). Does experience affect judgements of pain behaviour? Evidence from relatives of pain patients and health-care providers. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Prkachin, K.M. & Mercer, S.R. (1989). Pain expression in patients with shoulder pathology: Validity, properties and relationship to sickness impact. Pain. 39. 257-265.

Reuter-Lorenz, P. & Davidson, RJ. (1981). Differential contributions of the two cerebral hemispheres to the perception of happy and sad faces. Neuropsychologia, 19. 609-613.

Rinn, WE. (1984). The neuropsyhology of facial expression: A review of the neurological and psychological mechanisms for producing facial expression. Psychological Bulletin. 95. 52-77.

Russell, J.A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin. 115, 102-141.

Sackeim, H.A. & Gur, R.C. (1978). Lateral asymmetry in intensity of emotional expression. Neuropsychologia. 16. 473-481.

Schwartz, G.E., Ahern, G.L. & Brown, S. (1979). Lateralized facial muscle response to positive and negative emotional stimuli. Psychophysiology, 16, 561-571.

Swalm, D. & Craig, K.D. (1991). Differential impact of placebo on verbal and nonverbal measures of pain in men and women. Unpublished manuscript, University of British Columbia, Vancouver.

Tomkins, S.S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness (Vol. 1). The positive affects. New York: Springer.

Turk, DC, Meichenbaum, D.H. & Genest, M. (1983). Pain and behavioural medicine: Theory, research and clinical guide. New< York : Guilford.

Turk, DC. & Melzack, R. (1993). The measurement of pain and the assessment of people experiencing pain. In D.C. Turk & R. Melzack (Eds.) Handbook of pain assessment. New York: Guilford Press. 117 von Baeyer, C.L., Johnson, M.E. & McMillan, M.J. (1984). Consequences of nonverbal expressions of pain: Patient distress and observer concern. Social Sciences & Medicine. 19, 1319-1324.

Wilkie, D.J. (1995). Facial expressions of pain in lung cancer. Analgesia. 1. 91-99.

Zuckerman, M., Koestner, R., & Alton, A. O. (1984). Learning to Detect Deception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 46. 519-528.

Zuckerman, M., Koestner, R., & Colella, M. J. (1985). Learning to communicate deception from three communication channels. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 9. 188-194. Appendix A Description of FACS Action Units

AU# Action AU# Action 1 inner brow raise 34 puff 2 outer brow raise 35 cheek suck 4 brow lower 36 tongue bulge 5 upper lid raise 37 lip wipe 6 cheek raise 38 nostril dilate 7 lids tight 39 nostril compress 8 lips toward 41 lids droop 9 nose wrinkle 42 slit 10 upper lip raise 43 closed 11 nasolabial deepen 44 squint 12 lip corner pull 45 blink 13 cheek puff 46 wink 14 dimpler 15 lip corner depress 16 lower lip depress 17 chin raise 18 , lip pucker 19 tongue show 20 lip stretch 21 neck tighten 22 lip funnel 23 lip tight 24 lip press 25 lips part 26 jaw drop 27 mouth stretch 28 lip suck 29 jaw thrust 30 jaw to sideways 31 jaw clench 32 bite 33 blow 119

Appendix B Research Identifiying Pain-Related Action Units

Facial Action Units Studies Identifying as Pain related facial actions brow lowering Craig & Patrick, 1985 Craig, Hyde & Patrick, 1991 Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994 LeResche, 1982 LeResche & Dworkin, 1988 LeResche, Ehrlick & Dworkin, 1990 Patrick, Craig & Prkachin, 1986 Prkachin, 1992b Prkachin, Currie & Craig, 1983 Prkachin & Mercer, 1989 Swalm& Craig, 1991 tightened eyelids same as above raised cheeks Craig & Patrick, 1985 Craig, Hyde & Patrick, 1991 Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994 LeResche, 1982; Le Resche & Dworkin, 1988 LeResche, Ehrlick & Dworkin, 1990 Patrick, Craig & Prkachin, 1986 Prkachin, Currie & Craig, 1983

eyes closed / blinking Craig & Patrick, 1985 LeResche, 1982 LeResche & Dworkin, 1988 LeResche, Ehrlick & Dworkin, 1990 Patrick, Craig & Prkachin, 1986 Prkachin, Currie & Craig, 1983 Prkachin & Mercer, 1989 Swalm& Craig, 1991 120

upper lip raised Craig & Patrick, 1985 Craig, Hyde & Patrick, 1991 Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994 LeResche & Dworkin, 1988 LeResche, Ehflick & Dworkin, 1990 Patrick, Craig & Prkachin, 1986 Prkachin 1992b lips parted Craig & Patrick, 1985 Craig, Hyde & Patrick, 1991 Galin & Thorn, 1993 Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994 LeResche, 1982 LeResche & Dworkin, 1988 Prkachin & Mercer, 1989 Swalm & Craig, 1991 jaw drop Craig & Patrick, 1985 Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994 LeResche, 1982 LeResche & Dworkin, 1988 LeResche & Dworkin, 1988 Prkachin & Mercer, 1989 horizontal lip stretch Galin & Thorn, 1993 LeResche, 1982 LeResche & Dworkin, 1988 LeResche, Ehrlick & Dworkin, 1990 Prkachin & Mercer, 1989 lip corner pull Craig & Patrick, 1985 Prkachin & Mercer, 1989 Swalm & Craig, 1991 vertical lip stretch Craig & Patrick, 1985 Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994 LeResche, 1982 Prkachin & Mercer, 1989 nose wrinkled Hadjistavropoulos & Craig, 1994 LeResche & Dworkin, 1988 Prkachin & Mercer, 1989 deepened nasolabial fold LeResche, 1982 121

Appendix C Study One Questionnaires Personal Data

Sex: Male Female Age:

Marital Status: Profession:

Number of persons Living in Your Home:

Are you presently employed? Yes_ No

If yes: fulltime parttime

In which job?

If no: when did you stop work?

Did you stop because of pain?

How has your pain been diagnosed?

Where is your pain located?

Do you have other pains?

When did your pain begin? Year Occasion

When did you first seek professional help?

How many hospitalizations or surgical procedures have you had since then?

What kind of medication do you take for pain: Daily

Occasionally

What kind of medication do you take for other problems?

Do you smoke? How much per day?

Do you drink alcohol? How much per day?

Is your pain: Constant? Intermittent? Descriptor Differential Scales (Gracely, 1980)

Please circle one word or word-pair on the scale below to describe: the pain you experience on an average day.

Pain Sensation

A. Extremely Intense B. Very Intense C. Intense D. Strong E. Slightly Intense F. Barely Strong G. Moderate H. Mild I. Very Mild J. Weak K. Very Weak L. Faint M. No Sensation of Pain 123

Descriptor Differential Scales (Gracely, 1980)

Please circle one word or word-pair on the scale below to describe: the pain you experience on an average day.

Unpleasantness A. Very Intolerable B. Intolerable C. Very Distressing D. Slightly Intolerable E. Very Annoying F. Distressing G. Very Unpleasant H. Slightly Distressing I. Annoying J. Unpleasant K. Slightly Annoying L. Slightly Unpleasant M. No Discomfort 124

Appendix D Consent Form for Pain Patients Appearance-based stereotypes of professionals: Biased or valid?

Investigators: Dr. Thomas Hadjistavropoulos (822-9231) and Dr. Ken Craig (822-3948) Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia

Health professionals' judgements about patients are based in part on facial cues. Factors such as nonverbal pain expressions and patient physical attractiveness have been found to affect professionals' impressions of health and pain intensity. We are interested in determining whether or not such impressions carry any validity. For our study we are requesting your permission to videotape a short segment of the physiotherapy session you are about to undergo. During this segment of the physiotherapy session we will be asking you to carry out certain movements under different instructions. These movements are not harmful but may lead you to experience some discomfort. In order to assess your reactions to the procedures, the videotapes will be coded and viewed by independent observers. Several observers (10-15) will rate the physical attractiveness of your face (all ratings are strictly confidential). The physical attractiveness ratings will be used to determine the extent to which they are related to health professionals' judgements. You will aslo be requested to fill out several brief questionnaires related to the discomfort you may experience during these movements and more generally on a day-to-day basis. We are also interested in understanding how back pain affects your life and how you have come to cope with the discomfort you may be experiencing. Therefore, we will be asking you to fill out some questionnaires concerning your pain experience and your coping strategies. All your responses will be kept strictly confidential. Photographs of yourself and/or a short video may be shown to health professionals (including individuals training to become health professionals) who will be asked to estimate different dimensions of your pain experience including coping strategies, pain intensity, anxiety, likelihood that you are receiving compensation etc. The health professionals will have no access to your responses and their responses to your videotape will remain strictly confidential. Our goal is to determine how well, on average, the professionals' appearance-based judgements correspond to reality. In a related health experiment, health professionals and college students may eb shown your video, along with those of other patients. They will also be shown a brief summary of information that will be collected from you through an interview and some questionnaires. The professionals and the college students will be asked to match each image with the corresponding description. We will compare their ability to do this correctly. We would also like to point out that we may wish to use the information collected from you for the purposes of additional research. We will not do so however, without writing you first and requesting your written consent. If you have any questions about this study, feel free to ask the experimenter or to contact the investigators. You have the right to refuse participation in this research and even if you agree to participate, you have the right to withdraw at any time. Refusal to participate in this research will in no way affect your treatment.

I agree to participate in this study and give permission for the Point Grey Physiotherapy Clinic to release medical information solely for the purposes of this investigation and subject to the condition that this information is kept in confidence. I am aware that I can stop participation at any time wihtout penalty. I further acknowledge the receipt of a copy of this consent form. Signature Date 125

Appendix E Physiotherapy Protocol

Neutral Sitting Up & Neutral Lying Down: "Please maintain a neutral facial expression while looking at the camera. Neck straight, lips closed, and facial muscles relaxed."

The following four sets of instructions should be presented in random order:

A. Genuine "Lift both your legs up tp about here (10 - 12 inches) and hold it for 10 seconds. Please genuinely express the pain or discomfort you are feeling."

B. Exaggerated "This time I will ask you to lift both your legs up to about here (10 - 12 inches) for about 10 seconds, but I would like you to pretend that it hurts more than it really does."

C. Masked "Please lift both your legs up to about here (10 -12 inches) for about 10 seconds, but I'd like you to pretend that it doesn't bother you at all, no matter how much it does."

D. Faked "I'm not going to ask you to make any movement this time, but I'd like you to pretend that you're experienceing pain. Let me know by your facial expression. Begin now." Appendix F

Descriptor Differential Scales (Gracely, 1980)

Please circle one word or word-pair on the scale below to describe: the most severe pain you experienced in response to the leg raises you just completed.

Pain Sensation

A. Extremely Intense B. Very Intense C. Intense D. Strong E. Slightly Intense F. Barely Strong G. Moderate H. Mild I. Very Mild J. Weak K. Very Weak L. Faint M. No Sensation of Pain 127

Descriptor Differential Scales (Gracely, 1980)

Please circle one word or word-pair on the scale below to describe: the most severe pain you experienced in response to the leg raises you just completed.

Unpleasantness A. Very Intolerable B. Intolerable C. Very Distressing D. Slightly Intolerable E. Very Annoying F. Distressing G. Very Unpleasant F£. Slightly Distressing I. Annoying J. Unpleasant K. Slightly Annoying L. Slightly Unpleasant M. No Discomfort 128

Appendix G

Training Manual

It is very important to know how much pain a patient is feeling in order to provide a correct diagnosis and effective pain control. Unfortunately, physicians cannot always rely on patients to provide accurate information about their pain experience. Patients can either minimize or exaggerate their pain levels, and both misrepresentations have medical, social and financial consequences.

When do patients suffering from severe pain minimize their pain experience? • the patient may believe that one should be strong and "endure" pain rather than giving in to it • the patient may be afraid of becoming addicted to pain medications • the patient may be afraid of needles used for morphine injections • the patient worries that pain medications will make them "fuzzy headed" or unable to function Consequences: • quality of life goes down • pain disrupts sleep, decreases appetite and drains patients energy resources • patients take longer to recover because they don't have the energy required to heal • pain is a symptom of illness and misreporting it can hamper proper diagnosis • ties up hospital resources due to longer hospital stays etc.

When do pain patients exaggerate their pain experience? • the patient may feel he/she is not receiving adequate medication for the pain • the patient may feel that his/her condition is not being taken seriously enough by medical staff • the patient may be undergoing an evaluation for Worker's Compensation, insurance or litigation purposes (in all cases, exaggeration may be an attempt to emphasize the seriousness of their condition) • the patient may be attempting Worker's Compensation or insurance fraud Consequences: • increases in unnecesary health care costs . • tying up limited medical resources (personnel, diagnostic equipment, hospital beds) • increased insurance premiums • increased suspicion towards, and possible mistreatment of,- valid pain patients • unnecessary treatment, including overmedication, increased risk of dependence and addiction 129

THE PROBLEM: • medical professionals need to improve their ability to detect deception in pain patients • there needs to be a focus on pain information other than patient self-report. • how do we distinguish between patients who are suppressing or exaggerating their pain and patients who are quite insensitive or highly sensitive to pain?

A POSSIBLE SOLUTION: • training medical professionals to improve their pain judgement skills with pain patients • increasing their focus on, and their ability to detect, cues to deception in facial expressions

EVIDENCE AGAINST THIS APPROACH: • Most people are very poor at detecting deception from a person's facial expression • accuracy doesn't improve even when people are told to expect deception • "lie detection" is difficult because there isn't a facial cue highly specific to lying • lying must be inferred from very subtle cues, such as inconsistent or inappropriate facial movements.

EVIDENCE FOR THIS APPROACH: • there is a facial expression associated with genuine pain • genuine pain expressions do differ slightly from faked pain and masked pain expressions • research suggests that observers don't pay attention to the facial cues which differentiate among genuine, faked and masked pain expressions • with intensive training, people can see very subtle movements of the facial muscles

PURPOSE OF THIS STUDY: • to see if we can improve people's ability to detect faked or masked pain by: - providing information on the typical genuine facial expression of pain - providing information on the facial cues which may occur when someone is lying. Genuine Pain Expressions

The most common facial actions seen during genuine pain are:

• a lowered brow • tightened eye lids • raised cheeks • eyes narrowed, closed or blinking

This drawing of a genuine pain face shows you what these facial actions look like.

lowered brow

tightened eye lids

eyes closed

raised cheeks

NOTE:* These actions are often very brief (a second, or even a fraction of a second long) • You might not see all of these facial ations when a person is in genuine pain • There are substantial individual differences in how people respond

The following pictures will show each of the most common facial actions 131 Most Common Pain-Related Facial Actions

Lowered Brow - Subtle Movement Lowered Brow - Stronger Movement

Tightened Eyelids - Subtle Movement Tightened Eyelids - Intense Movement 132

Raised Cheeks - Subtle Movement Raised Cheeks - Intense Movement

Eyes Narrowed Eyes Closed 133

IMPORTANT: • these facial actions are common in people experiencing pain BUT everyone's "pain face" looks different. • several other facial actions have been frequently seen during people's reactions to pain.

Other facial actions seen during genuine pain are: • a raised upper lip • an opened mouth • a mouth stretched wide open, either horizontally or vertically • a wrinkled nose • a deepened nasolabial fold

This drawing of a genuine pain face shows you what these facial actions look like:

wrinkled nose

deepened nasolabial fold

raised upper lip

an opened mouth

Neutral Expression Genuine Pain Expression

IMPORTANT:

• a pain face may include only one or two of these facial actions • these facial actions may be very subtle (e.g. not very intense) • these actions are often very brief ( a second, or even a fraction of a second long) • there are substantial individual differences in people's responses to pain, therfore some people may display facial actions other than those described here.

NOTE: The following pictures show each of these facial actions individually. Other Common Pain-Related Facial Actions

Raised Upper Lip - Subtle Movement Raised Upper Lip - Intense Movement

Lips Parted Jaw Drop 135

Vertical Lip Stretch

Horizontal Lip Stretch - Subtle Movement Horizontal Lip Stretch - Intense Movement 136

Wrinkled Nose - Subtle Movement Wrinkled Nose - Intense Movement

Deepened Nasolabial Fold 137 Faked Pain FACTS: • people are very good at faking a pain expression • people asked to fake their pain exhibit the same facial actions as genuine pain patients • observers have considerable difficulty distinguishing between genuine and faked pain expressions • BUT there are a few differences between faked and genuine pain expressions.

DIFFERENCES BETWEEN FAKED AND GENUINE PAIN EXPRESSIONS: • the individual facial actions are usually more intense when faked than when genuine Note: it is difficult to discriminate between a faked expression and a severe genuine pain expression as pain severity is communicated through increasingly intense facial expressions. • genuine pain expressions are usually only made up of a few pain-related facial actions BUT faked pain expression usually contain several pain-related facial actions THEREFORE: • faked pain faces are often stronger or more exaggerated than genuine pain expressions • faked pain faces are often more "prototypical" or "perfect" than a genuine pain expression

EXAMPLE:

GENUINE PAIN FAKED PAIN

• brow lowered • brow lowered • eyes shut • eyes squeezed shut • raised upper lip • raised upper lip • deepened nasolabial fold • lips slightly opened Note: increased intensity of all facial actions in the faked pain face 138 Masked Pain.

FACTS: • people are not as successful at masking their genuine pain expressions • very little of the genuine pain expression remains on their faces BUT observers seem to pick it up • subtle facial cues of pain often "leak out" despite the patients efforts to conceal them. • forehead and brow muscles are difficult to control, so "leakage" may occur most around the eyes

DIFFERENCES BETWEEN MASKED PAIN EXPRESSIONS AND A NEUTRAL FACE: • slightly raised cheeks • slightly narrowed eyes • a neutral face that is held unnaturally still • a neutral face which looks unusually tense, like they are trying to "hold in" a facial expression • reduced frequency of blinking • a very brief "flash" of a pain expression, followed by a prolonged neutral face.

EXAMPLE:

NEUTRAL MASKED PAIN

residual activity around the eyes 139 Cues to General Deception

HOW TO FAKE A FACIAL EXPRESSION: • cover your genuine emotion / expression with the desired facial expression

Cues: • look for an underlying expression or a mix of expressions • look for small facial actions which are incompatible with the faked emotion

HOW TO HIDE OR MASK A GENUINE EXPRESSION • cover the genuine expression with a different facial expression (e.g. a smile)

Cues: • look for incompatible facial actions (e.g. sad eyes with a smile) • look for signs of an underlying expression (e.g. 1 or 2 pain-related facial actions) • face seems unnatural

• hide a genuine expression by tightening antagonistic muscles in the face to "hold it in"

Cues: • muscle tightening may make the face appear stiff, unnatural or controlled. • "leakage" of the genuine pain expression, a brief "micro-expression" which is very brief (as short as 1/4 of a second)

OTHER GENERAL CUES TO DECEPTION:

Emotions: • shame or fear of being caught lying • signs of guilt, such as blushing, blanching or facial sweating.

Timing: • faked expressions seem to last longer than genuine ones • during faked expressions, individual facial actions actions may appear serially versus all at once • faked expressions may have a delayed onset.

Blinking: • changes in blinking rates are also thought to occur during deception. • blinking may increase because of increased emotion OR conscious efforts to control emotions will lead to an unconscious decrease in blinking. 140

Asymmetry: • faked expressions sometimes appear lopsided of asymmetrical • muscle action may be stronger on one side of tfie face than the other.

EXAMPLES: - in each example, there is asymmetry - muscle movement on the right is stronger

IMPORTANT: • the signs of deception described above have not been completely proven through research. • taken individually, they are not necessarily a sure sign that someone is lying. • these are not the only signs of deception - there may be others we are unaware of. • none of these "signs" have been looked for in pain patients. 141 Training Test

1. When people are told that there may be deception occuring, their deception T F accuracy increases considerably.

2. It should be fairly simple to train lie detectors because there are facial cues highly specific to lying. T F

3. One of the most common facial actions during genuine pain is tightened eyelids T F

4. One of the most common facial actions during genuine pain is a wrinkled nose. T F

5. One of the most common facial actions during genuine pain is an opened mouth. T F

6. One of the most common facial actions during genuine pain is a lowered brow. T F

7. Facial reactions to pain are usually pretty intense when compared to other emotional expressions like sadness, which are pretty brief and subtle. T F

8. Pain expressions always include more than one facial action. T F

9. People aren't very good at faking a pain expression. T F

10. Genuine pain expressions are usually stronger than faked pain expressions. T F

11. When compared to genuine pain expressions, faked pian expressions look

more like a "perfect" pain reaction. T F

12. People are very good at masking their genuine pain expressions. T F

13. When someone is trying to hide their pain, facial activity is most likely to "leak out" in the area around the eyes. T F 14. Faked expressions last longer than genuine ones. T F 142

Appendix H Study Two Questionnaire Package

Judgement Study Consent Form

June 29, 1992.

Dear Students^

We are conducting a study on the facial expression of pain, and the ability of observers to distinguish between genuine, faked, and masked pain. Our objective is to improve the ability of health care professionals to make appropriate decisions about diagnosis and treatment.

This study asks you to view videotapes of low back pain patients undergoing a physical examination which involves painful movements. During these examinations, patients were given certain instructions regarding their facial expressions. You would view a series of different patients' reactions and provide brief ratings of how much discomfort, if any, they were experiencing.

This study will require 60 to 90 minutes of your time. Your participation in this study will remain completely confidential; any information you provide will be identified by a numbered code and will be accessible only to researchers involved in this study. We hope that you will be able to participate and aid us in our research. However, please feel free to decline now or at any time during the course of this study. If you do wish to participate, you will receive course participation credit. A copy of this consent form will be provided to you for your own records.

For further information, please contact:

Marilyn Hill, MSc, U.B.C. 822-5280 Ken Craig, Ph.D., Psychologist, Professor, U.B.C. 822-3948

Thank-you in advance for your assistance.

I CONSENT I DO NOT CONSENT

SIGNATURE PERSONAL DATA

Age_

Sex: Male Female

Marital Status:

What country were you born in?

If you were not born in Canada, when did you move here?

What country were your parents born in?

Mother?

Father?

What language do you use at home?

What is your ethnic descent? (e.g. Taiwanese, German / Irish etc.) ? 144

PERSONAL PAIN EXPERIENCE

Have you ever experienced: 1. acute pain, (e.g. an injury or surgery)? Yes No If yes: -what was the cause of the pain? -on a scale from 0 (no pain) to 10 (severe pain) how intense was the pain? - how long did the acute pain last?

2. a recurrent painful condition (e.g. migraine headaches, ulcers)? Yes No If yes: - what was the cause of the pain? . - on a scale from 0 (no pain) to 10 (severe pain) how intense was the pain? - how long have you suffered from recurrent pain?

3. chronic pain (e.g. chronic low back pain)? Yes No If yes: - what was the cause of the pain? . . - on a scale from 0 (no pain) to 10 (severe pain) how intense was the pain?

- how long have you suffered from chronic pain?

FAMILY HISTORY OF PAIN

Has anyone in your immediate family ever experienced:

1. acute pain, (e.g. an injury or surgery)? Yes No If yes: -which family member (s)? -what was the cause of the pain? -on a scale from 0 (no pain) to 10 (severe pain) how intense was the pain? - how long did the acute pain last? 2. a recurrent painful condition (e.g. migraine headaches, ulcers)? Yes No If yes: -which family member (s)? - what was the cause of the pain? - on a scale from 0 (no pain) to 10 (severe pain) how intense was the pain? - how long have they suffered from recurrent pain?

3. chronic pain (e.g. chronic low back pain)? Yes No If yes: -which family member (s)? - what is the cause of the pain? - on a scale from 0 (no pain) to 10 (severe pain) how intense is the pain? - how long have they suffered from chronic pain? 145 Interpersonal Reactivity Index

1. I daydream and fantasize, with some regularity, about things that 0 12 3 4 might happen to me.

2. 1 often have tender, concerned feelings for people less fortunate 0 12 3 4 than me.

3. I sometimes find it difficult to see things from the "other guy's" 0 1 2 3 4 point of view.

4. Sometimes I don't feel very sorry for other people when they are 0 12 3 4 having problems.

5. I really get involved with the feelings of the characters in a novel. 0 12 3 4

6. In emergency situations, I feel apprehensive and ill-at-ease. 0 12 3 4

7. I am usually objective when when I watch a movie or play, and I 0 12 3 4 don't often get completely caught up in it.

8. I try to look at everybody's side of a disagreement before I make 0 1 2 3 4 a decision.

9. When I see someone being taken advantage of, I feel kind of 0 12 3 4 protective towards them.

10. I sometimes feel helpless when I am in the middle of a very 0 12 3 4 emotional situation. .

11. I sometimes try to understand my friends better by imagining how 0 12 3 4 things look from their perspective.

12. Becoming extremely involved in a good book or movie is 0 12 3 4 somewhat rare for me.

13. When I see someone get hurt, I tend to remain calm. 0 12 3 4

14. Other people's misfortunes do not usually disturb me a great deal. 0 12 3 4

15. If I'm sure I'm right about something, I don't waste much time 0 12 3 4 listening to other people's arguments.

16. After seeing a play or movie, I have felt as though I were one of 0 12 3 4 the characters.

17. Being in a tense emotional situation scares me. 0 12 3 4 146 18. When I see someone being treated unfairly, I sometimes don't 0 12 3 4 feel very much pity for them.

19. I am usually pretty effective in dealing with emergencies. 0 1 2 3 4

20. I am often quite touched by things that I see happen. 0 1 2 3 4

21. I believe that there are two sides to every question and try to look at 0 1 2 3 4 them both.

22. I would describe myself as a pretty soft-hearted person. 0 1 2 3 4

23. When watching a good movie, I can very easily put myself in the 0 1 2 3 4 place of a leading character.

24. I tend to lose control during emergencies. 0 1 2 3 4

25. When I'm upset at someone, I usually try to "put myself in his 0 12 3 4 shoes" for a while.

26. When I am reading an interesting story or novel, I imagine how I 0 12 3 4 would feel if the events in the story were happening to me.

27. When I see someone who badly needs help in an emergency, I 0 12 3 4 go to pieces.

28. Before criticizing somebody, I try to imagine how I would feel if I 0 12 3 4 were in their place. 147

Appendix I

Description of Videotapes & Rating Forms

Description of Videotapes

The videotapes you are about to watch are of patients with back pain who are undergoing a a routine physical examination by their physiotherapist. During this examination, patients were asked to raise their legs 10 inches off the examination table while keeping them straight. Patients with low back pain experience this movement as painful.

Patients performed the leg raise twice. During one of these movements they were asked to genuinely express any pain that they felt. During the other movement, they were asked to pretend that it didn't hurt. They were also asked to give a faked expression, by pretending that they were in a lot of pain while not actually moving their legs. The patients' facial expressions were videotaped in each of these conditions.

For each patient on the videotape you will see 4 kinds of faces: a neutral face videotaped before the examination began, a genuine pain expression, a faked pain expression, and a masked pain expression. These 4 clips will not be in any particular order. After each clip, you will be asked to estimate the pain experienced based on the facial expression you see. You will also be asked to decide which category the facial expression represents: a neutral expression, a genuine pain face, a faked pain expression or a masked pain expression. You will also be asked how confident you are about your decisions and how you came to your decision. 148

Description of Videotapes (Feedback Group)

The videotapes you are about to watch are of patients with back pain who are undergoing a a routine physical examination by their physiotherapist. During this examination, patients were asked to raise their legs 10 inches off the examination table while keeping them straight. Patients with low back pain experience this movement as painful.

Patients performed the leg raise twice. During one of these movements they were asked to genuinely express any pain that they felt. During the other movement, they were asked to pretend that it didn't hurt. They were also asked to give a faked expression, by pretending that they were in a lot of pain while not actually moving their legs. The patients' facial expressions were videotaped in each of these conditions.

For each patient on the videotape you will see 4 kinds of faces: a neutral face videotaped before the examination began, a genuine pain expression, a faked pain expression, and a masked pain expression. These 4 clips will not be in any particular order. After each clip, you will be asked to estimate the pain experienced based on the facial expression you see. You will also be asked to decide which category the facial expression represents: a neutral expression, a genuine pain face, a faked pain expression or a masked pain expression. You will also be asked how confident you are about your decisions and how you came to your decision. You will be given feedback on the accuracy of your pain judgements. Once you have completed all of your ratings on a patient, you check your accuracy by reading the appropriate feedback card. It will tell you which category each of the 4 facial expressions represents.

When you begin your pain judgement task, you will be given a stack of feedback cards labelled on one side with the patients ED# (which will match the ones on the tv screen). The other side of the card contains the correct categorizations of the facial expressions. You may only turn the cards over one at a time and ONLY when you have already completed the ratings on that patient. DO NOT change your ratings once you have seen the feedback card. If you accidentally see a card before you have rated a patient, please make a note of it so that we can discard the rating you made on that patient.

This is how you complete your judgement task: 1. Watch the video clip for your first patient.

2. Pause the tape; 3. Make your judgements (pain intensity, categorize the expressions, rate your accuracy) 4. Check your accuracy 5. Continue on to the next subject. 149

00 oo < m re 2* "T* < m re z 3 2 re re e? ?? *"^ ""i _<• = 3 re x co o B 3 o P CO •3 „ S' re X P oo 3 . re o' 3 3 ^ * cr g« 3 CO 00 P § re* re 3 o § re* = 1 00 5" re* EL 3 o 8 re re EL 3 3 5* re 8 3 re

re Q. re o o cnr n T3 •5" n >

a oo a >"_2< n <" G >_EJ0Q C C W I—I CO 3 3 3 S- 3 3 <-* S 3 3 3 Ef 3 S»T3 3 3 P O OO cr o"2 r-* r-* r~. CO P O re CO sr o JQ 0"° 3 re* o o 8 re" 2 re re S2 _ re re re S2. 00 •l »-t 3 5* 3 O . S0Q re" re* 3 OQ OQ 3 3 = 3 1 K^* 2 2 re" co 5 re" re" £2 3 OQ

•_*r^^~„a;r-cnonw> T3 B 5' 00 oo 2;TI<<:<:__DOOOoo_

reo. o. o re n • o Q

o "0 •5' E? 2r^*^r„o**flwonw> 5' 03 p' G *Zc_c_G> 2<0 00 00 < 3 < 3 O •-• ~ 3 3 M* £5 £2 < =r < _0Q 0Q >_) 5 00 OQ ^ 5 " re 5? re OQ -3 i3s .3 * 3" re* 2«5 P 3*^ 5 -•-=•03 g co co 3_ 5* D*SL 5 o T3 re* CO 3 3 3 =* co' 0Q » J» rere 3 3 3 g_ _ Co n 5 co re*^ re £2. re re s ^ 3 3 CO CO to SJ- 3 . 0Q J_ CO CO £2 OQ 3 2" 2 re* 00 re* re O 00 OQ o re. p re* Vide o Cli p w > (N/ G Typ e ( Exp r

io n acia l IM)

o o _ o _ © _ *! | # | * 1 Confidenc e i n Decisio

1 u< L/l 1 <=> 1 © 1 <=> |~ © 1 # 1 # 1 # 1 * 1 1UU % 100 %

§i 1 1 §! 5$ - Cue s Use d 15

Appendix J

Frequency of Facial Cue Use per Subject

Facial Cue Mean SD Facial Cue Mean SD smile 4.29 3.45 large expression changes 0.08 0.33 neutral / no reaction 10.18 8.34 long duration 0.89 1.88 frown 1.32 3.63 decreased movements 0.01 0.09 repressed expression 0.48 1.14 drawn eyes 0.03 0.37 lowered eyebrows 1.87 3.42 exaggerated movements 2.20 2.64 eye narrows 0.57 1.26 rapid eye movements 0.32 0.73 eyebrow movements 2.'22 3.12 too much expression 0.34 0.90 mouth movements 2.52 4.56 head movements 1.78 2.39 eyebrow twitch 0.28' 0.64 looked fresh 0.01 0.09 relaxed eyes 0.15 0.66' eyes squinting / tightened 2.12 3.48 chewing (gum) 0.14 0.35 tight jaw 0.33 1.10 eyes move upward 0.90 1.61 rapid tongue movements 0.01 0.09 steady breathing pattern 1.62 4.08 rapid breathing 0.70 1.45 eyelid movement 0.30 0.98 shakiness 0.70 1.53 relaxed 4.09 6.45 too much movement 0.48 0.99 smile with pain expression 0.24 0.53 tight hps 1.82 3.17 no emotion 0.09 0.43 too much effort / energy 0.03 0.29 eyes moved 7.47" lti09 grimace 0.92 3.19 throat moved 0.19 0.96 tighdy closed eyes 0.34 1.27 hps moved 1.28. 2.54 change in eyes 0.01 0.09 eyebrows pinched 0.77 1.74 gritting teeth 0.15 0.57 corner mouth twitch 0.08 0.31 tight neck muscles 0.45 1.32 hps curled 0.06 0.47 no change in eyes 1.08 1.91 watery eyes 0.18 0.71 too much cheek movement 0.01 0.09 eyes widened 0.28 0.66 swallowed 0.80 1.17 eyelids hooded 0.01 0.09 sudden movements 0.17 0.75 eyes open 0.15 0.53 exaggerated blinking 0.20 0.56 laugh 0.38 1.01 blinking 2.7.3 3.43 fast blinking 0.69 1.05 eyelid tightening 0.36 1.25

lips drawn 0.13 0.90 too quick to respond 0.01 0:09 short duration 0.60 1.87 deep breathing 1.79 2.75 eyes closed 2.86 3.86 too much neck movement 0.04 0.20 152

Facial Cue Mean SD Facial Cue Mean SD tense 2.37 4.75 chin 0.10 0.42 looking into camera 0.35 1.16 hps closed 0.33 0.97 asymmetry 0.86 1.47 no pain expression 0.59 2.37 cheek movement 0.32 1.12 body movement 1.50 2.82 licking lips 0.21 0.63 real discomfort 0.62 1.41 sad face 0.18 0.67 no discomfort 0.23 0.73 nose movement 0.13 0.58 fake cheeks 0.01 0.09 forehead movement 0.08 0.29 raised cheeks 0.75 1.94 no blinking 0.43 1.24 fast eye shut 0.04 0.46 nostril movement 0.47 1.09 normal expression 1.02 3.26 tongue movement 0.11 0.38 unpleasant / cool / irritated 0.27 0.79 fake smile 0.32 0.82 very slight movement 0.07 0.28 symmetry 0.08 0.57 nervous / worried 0.72 1.50 exaggerated nose move 0.01 0.09 widened mouth 0.20 0.72 sad eyes 0.11 0.46 slight pain expression 0.22 1.11 nose wrinkle 0.23 0.88 repressed pain expression 0.38 0.93 no change in expression 2.01 5.17 perfect expression 0.14 0.87 abrupt head movement 0.09 0.41 rubbing shoulder 0.12 0.32 raised head 0.04 0.27 fidgety 0.14 0.55 raised eyebrows 0.69 1.61 contradictory expressions 0.18 1.03 sudden expression change 0.14 0.47 fleeting expression' 0.01 0.09 eye bulge 0.06 0,55 "leakage @ eyes 0.06 0.35 sour look 0.01 0.09 timing off 0.03 0.27 raised upper lip 0.49 2.16 tried not to shout 0.01 0.09 sudden jump / jolt 0.93 2.14 looked down 0.22 0.97 stiff face / controlled face 1.36 2.72 too little movement 0.24 0:74 lip shiver / wobble 0.11 0.36 too painful 0.09 0.48 biting lips 0.52 1.09 looked relieved 0.16 0.47 open mouth 1.65 2.36 jaw movement 0.13 0.48 strange movements 0.15 0.44 intense look 0.30 0.91 silly / comical 0.06 0.27 red face 0.04 0.24 fake / forced / unnatural 1.38 1.83 face movements 1.31 7.78 eyes in pain 0.17 0.51 glazed eyes 0.05 0.25 stronger movements 0.04 0.20 lips pursed 0.32 0.99 missing data 369.5 25.47 no smile 0.03 0.26 153

Facial Cue Mean SD Facial Cue Mean SD no body movement 0.63 2.33 wince 0.01 0.09 vocalization 0.21 0.63 muscle tension 0.76 2.08 holding breath 0.80 1.99 fake muscle tension 0.03 0.37 wrinkled face 0.04 0.24 head tilted 0.04 0.30 looks sedated 0.09 0.34 "pain, what pain?" 0.01 0.09 nasolabial fold 0.08 0.36 eyelid relaxation 0.13 0.63 happy 0.07 0.28 tense then relaxed 0.15 0.60 weird look in eyes 0.04 0.30 satisfied expression 0.01 0.09 alert eyes / inquisitive 0.05 0.25 controlled breathing 0.13 0.59 strange.expression 0.03 0.20 posture 0.01 0.09 no head movement 0.07 0.34 not distracted by pain 0.06 0.55 trying to hide 0.11 0.31 distracted by pain 0.07 0.36 involuntary movement 0.08 0.65 eyes unfocused 0,03 0.20 serious expression 0.16 0.47 no change in breathing 0.06 0.42 stoic expression 0.05 0.46 twisted facial expression 0.08 0.44 waiting 0.03 0.16 no pain in eyes 0.04 0.30 fear 0.07 0.31 relaxed squint 0.01 0.09 dry mouth 0.02 0.18 no eye squint 0.02 0.13 repressed smile 0.05 0.22 eyes fade 0.01 0.09 conspiratory smile 0.01 0.09 smirk 0.05 0.25 surprise 0.07 0.36 eyes straining 0.03 0.16

pouting 0r04 -0:38 looking at Dr. 0.05 0.39 concentration 0.35 1.32 animated 0.02 0.13 hands 0.02 0.18 horizontal mouth move 0.09 0.43 shoulders 0.01 0.09 abrupt expression 0.03 0.27 face harmony 0,03 0.37 relaxed / normal breathing 0.69 1.69 pain face leakage 0.04 0.24 lopsided mouth 0.01 0.09 looks hurried 0.03 0.20 no mouth movement 0.06 0.33 leakage 0.04 0.30 yawn 0.01 0.09 unnatural gazing 0.03 0.27 boredom 0.16 0.73 distant 0.03 0.29 sudden intake of breath 0.21 0.62 pain face 0.24 0.66 delayed onset 0.19 0.60 pleasant face 0.06 0.40 about to cry 0.03 0.18 looking around 2.05 3.10 prep for pain 0.02 0.18 inconsistant 0.20 0.74 not neutral 0.02 0.18 1

Facial Cue Mean SD Facial Cue Mean SD lip stretch 0.03 0.16 looks guilty 0.03 0.22 shifty/furtive eyes 0.60 1.99 not exaggerated 0.05 0.25 loose facial actions 0.01 0.09 mean eyes 0.02 0.13 eye twitch 0.34 0.87 upper lip raise 0.03 0.20 no pain in mouth 0.01 0.09 slow movements 0.07 0.32 hp twitch 0.30 1.04 not shaking 0.01 0.09 hp twist 0.04 0.38 showing teeth 0.03 0.27 looks weak 0.01 0.09 no intensity 0.07 0.31 smile to hide 0.03 0.16 not worried 0.03 0.16 tired eyes 0.03 0.16 repeated pain expressions 0.02 0.13 shudder 0.02 0.18 repeated movements 0.06 0.24 trying to remain calm 0.20 0.72 decreased blinking 0.07 0.31 fast movements 0.27 0.81 no leakage 0.02 0.13 no grimace 0.03 0.29 expectant face 0.01 0.09 arms crossed 0.01 0.09 slight body movement 0.02 0.18 amused 0.02 0.13 breathing through mouth 0.02 0.13 unpleasant / strange smile 0.03 0.22 breathing through nose 0.01 0.09 less swallowing . 0.01 0.09 barely breathing 0.03 0.16 change in expression 0.01 0.09 one eye open, one closed 0.01 0.09 smile disappears 0.05 0.41 extreme pain to neutral 0.03 0.16 expecting pain 0.03 0.16 slow eye movement 0.03 0.22 no relief 0.01 0.09 extreme eye lowering 0.04 0.46 wobbly chin 0.01 0.09 hp lowered 0.02 0.18 irregular breathing 0.31 0.91 mouth wrenched 0.01 0.09 forced smile 0.17 0.63 collected strength 0.01 0.09 nervous smile 0.02 0.13 diverted attention 0.03 0.18 quick glance 0.01 0.09 not constant 0.04 0.24 vertical hp movement 0.01 0.09 no jolt or jerk 0.03 0.29 partial expression 0.01 0.09 not simultaneous actions 0.13 1.05 full reaction 0.06 0.64 unclear 0.01 0.09 more reaction 0.01 0.09 no eyebrow movements 0.05 0.2-2 simultaneous facial actions 0.03 0.29 pain in mouth 0.01 0.09 few abnormal actions 0.01 0.09 relaxed mouth 0.13 0.79 genuine eyes 0.02 0.13 relaxed cheeks 0.06 0.42 genuine pain 0.04 0.33 cheek twitch 0.03 0.20 Facial Cue Mean SD fake eye movement 0 03 0 16 early onset 0 05 0 31 no tension 0 10 0 70 sigh 0 02 0 13 puzzled 0 01 0 09 chewing interrupted 0 01 0 09 normal blinking 0 15 0 75 drooled 0 03 0 20 very slight smile 0 03 0 20 sad 0 01 0 09 resistant 0 01 0 09 laughing eyes 0 01 0 09 fake mouth 0 01 0 09 cough 0 01 0 09 consistant 0 03 0 20 more realistic 0 01 0 09 decreased intensity 0 02 0 18 swooned 0 01 0 09 decreased tension 0 01 0 09 not neutral 0 01 0 09 relaxed jaw 0 01 0 09 couldn't hold expression 0 01 0 09 eyes crossed 0 01 0 09 abrupt end of expression 0 01 0 09 too consistant 0 01 0 09 forehead tension 0 02 0 18 constant expression 0 01 0 09 156

Appendix K

Categorization of Facial Cues Eye Movements eye narrows relaxed eyes eyes rolling eyelid movements eyes moved watery eyes eyes widened eyes open eyes closed eyes squinted eyes squeezed shut rapid eye movements staring eyelid tightening looking into camera sad eyes eye bulge eyes in pain glazed eyes leakage around eyes weird look in eyes alert eyes looking around eyes relaxed furtive eyes eye twitch

Eyebrow Movements lowered eyebrows eyebrow twitch eyebrows pinched eyebrow movements raised eyebrows

Cheek / Nose Movements cheek movement raised cheeks relaxed cheeks nose movement nostril movement nose wrinkle

Mouth Movements smile frown mouth movement lips moved lips curled lips drawn tight lips corner mouth twitch licking lips raised upper lip lip wobble biting lips open mouth lips closed widened mouth lips pursed lip twitch lip twist relaxed mouth horizontal mouth movement

Smile repressed smile fake smile smile smile with pain expression smirk smile disappears forced smile

"Held In" Facial Actions staring no blinking fixed expression repressed expression hiding emotions repressed smile stiff / controlled face repressed pain expression trying to remain neutral

Breathing Patterns steady breathing rapid breathing deep breathing holding breath 157 controlled breathing relaxed breathing irregular breathing sudden intake of breath

Incongruent Facial Actions inconsistant smile with paincontradictory actions too many expressions actions not simultaneous

Pain Expression grimace real discomfort too painful slight pain expression pain face twisted face repressed pain expression perfect pain expression smile with pain expression

Blinking Patterns fast blinking strange blinking staring blinking no blinking decreased blinking

Timing short duration long duration sudden movements delayed onset slow movements early onset not simultaneous

Exaggeration silly fake smile too painful too many expressions fake intense look strange movements exaggerated movements too much movement

Tense / Tight tight lips tight jaw eye squint eyebrows pinched eyes squeezed shut gritting teeth tight neck muscles tense muscle tension tense then relaxed

Sudden Movements sudden jolt / twitch sudden movements early onset sudden change in expressio nfast movement involuntary movement

Relaxed looks relieved looks sedated relaxed eyelid relaxation relaxed breathing relaxed mouth relaxed cheeks no tension relaxed eyes Appendix L Type and frequency of Subjects' Pain Experiences

Self Family Cause Acute Recurent Chronic Acute Recurrent Chrc surgery 7.5 0 0 13.3 0 0.8 sports injury 5.8 0.8 3.3 1.7 1.7 eating disorder 0 0 0 0.8 0 0.8 knee pain 2.5 2.5 0.8 0 0.8 0.8 bacterial infection 1.7 0 0 0.8 0 0 compressed nerve 0 0.8 0 0 0 0.8 ruptured spleen 0.8 0 0 0 0.8 0 headaches 0.8 11.7 0 0.8 20.8 0.8 broken bone 7.5 0 1.7 13.3 0.8 1.7 Osgood schlater's 0 0 0.8 0 0.8 0 back pain 5.0 3.3 6.7 3.3 2.5 18.3 cut 3.3 0 0 1.7 0 0 burn 1.7 0 0 1.7 0 0 blood clot 0 0 0 0.8 0 0 neck pain 0 0 0 0 0 0.8 torn ligaments 2.5 0.8 1.7 0 0 0.8 cataracts 0 0 0 1.7 0 0 ulcer 0 4.2 0 0 5.8 0 cramps 0 3.3 0 0 1.7 0 wisdom teeth 5.8 0 0 0.8 0 0 sore throat 0 0.8 0 P 0 0 gallstone 0 0 0 4.2 0 0 dehydration 0.8 0 0 0 0 0 work injury 0 0 0 0.8 0 0.8 car accident 4.2 0 0.8 3.3 0 2.5 head injury 2.5 0 0 0.8 0 0 leg injury 0 0 0 0.8 0.8 0.8 hemerroids 0.8 0 0 0 0 0 dislocation 0.8 0 0 2.5 0 0 stomachaches 0 5.8 0 0 2.5 0.8 stress 0 1.7 0 0 1.7 0.8 unknown 0.8 0.8 0.8 0 0 0.8 PMS 0 0.8 0.8 0 0 0 allergies 0.8 0 0 0 0 0 appendicitis 0 0 0 1.7 0 0 cut leg with saw 0 0 0 0.8 0 0 worm }n stomach 0 0 0 0 0.8 0 arthritis 0 0 0 0.8 1.7 1.7 rash 0.8 0 0 0.8 0 0 sprain 4.2 0 0 0 0 0 throat infection 0 0 0 0 0.8 0 poor posture 0 0 0.8 0 0 0 a fall 5.0 0 0 0.8 0 0 ingrown toenail 0 0 0.8 0 0 0 gallbladder 0 0 0 0.8 0 0 seizure 0.8 0 0 0 0 0 heart attack 0 0 0 1.7 0.8 0 a growth 0 0 0 0.8 0 0 toothache 0.8 0.8 0 0.8 0 0 poor nutrition 0 0 0 0 0.8 0 gout 0.8 0 0 0 0.8 0 labor 1.7 0 0 0.8 0 0 cancer 0 0 0 1.7 0.8 0 liver 0 0 0 0 0.8 0 water in legs 0 0 0 0 0 0.8 kidney stones 0 0 0 0 0.8 0 lack of sleep 0 0.8 0 0 0 0 too busy 0 0 0 0 0.8 1.7 foot pain 0.8 0 0 0 0 0.8 inflamed ganglion 0.8 0 0 0 0 0 exercise 0 0.8 0.8 0 0 0 finger injury 1.7 0 0 0 0 0 dysmennorhea 0 0.8 0 0 0 0 gum pain 0 0 0 0.8 0 0 ovarian cysts 0 0 0.8 0 0 0 160

Appendix M

Correlation Matrix: Empathy and Experience with Judgement Variables

IRI score Perspectiv Concern Distress Fantasy Experience

e

Pain Judgements

Accuracy -.02 -.06 -.01 -.04 .04 .08

Confidence -.08 .02 .01 -.17 -.10 -.10

Unpleasantness .16 .05 .15 .17 .12 -.04

Intensity .10 .14 .00 .07 .08 .16

Experience .15 .22* .03 7.01 .20* 1.00

Females

Accuracy -:12 -.16 -.08 .04 -.11 .01

Confidence .02 .10 .06 -.04 -.06 -.11

Unpleasantness .06 -.03 .19 -.05 .07 -.02

Intensity .07 .18 -.06 .16 -.07 .09

Experience .19 ,23 .14 -.02 .18 1.00

Males

Accuracy -.05 -.05 -.05 -.11 04 .05

Confidence -.17 -.07 -.05 -.27* -.16 -.12

Unpleasantness .29* .16 .18 .34** .25 -.01

Intensity .08 .06 .02 -.03 .22 .24

Experience .08 .17 -.10 .00 .17 1.00

* significance less than .05 ** significance less than .01 (two tailed)

IRI score = Interpersonal Reactivity Index Distress = Personal Distress IRI subscale

Perspective = Perspective-taking IRI subscale Fantasy = Fantasy IRI subscale

Concern = Empathic Concern IRI subscale Experience = Sum of pain experiences