Beyond HERA: Contributions of Specific Prefrontal Brain Areas to Long-Term Memory Retrieval

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Beyond HERA: Contributions of Specific Prefrontal Brain Areas to Long-Term Memory Retrieval Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1996,3 (2),149-158 Beyond HERA: Contributions of specific prefrontal brain areas to long-term memory retrieval RANDY L, BUCKNER Washington University, si: Louis, Missouri Recent neuroimaging studies have provided a wealth of information about areas within prefrontal cortex involved in long-term memory, These studies prompted a proposal by Tulving and colleagues (Tulving, Kapur, Craik, Habib, & Houle, 1994)that prefrontal contributions to memory function are related to laterality differences (the hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry model). This review goes beyond a general characterization of prefrontal lobes to a more specific analysis of distinct areas within the prefrontal cortex. Separate prefrontal areas, sometimes within the same hemi­ sphere, are discussed in terms of selective contributions that they might make to memory retrieval, In the end, it is concluded that a framework which tries to understand prefrontal function in terms of specific areas is a useful complement to models, like HERA,which attempt to find unifying prin­ ciples across multiple areas. The past decade has seen an outpouring of informa­ multiple neuroimaging studies conducted across several tion about the neurobiological basis ofhuman cognition, different laboratories. The main assertions of the HERA owing largely to the refinement ofmethods for imaging model were that (1) right prefrontal cortical areas are more active brain areas in healthy, awake subjects as they per­ involved in episodic retrieval than are left prefrontal areas, form cognitive tasks (Posner & Raichle, 1994). These and (2) left prefrontal areas are more involved in semantic methods, often referred to as neuroimaging techniques, retrieval and episodic encoding than are right prefrontal provide a window, previously unavailable, into the func­ areas. The idea that the prefrontal cortex may be involved tion ofthe brain. Memory research has likewise benefit­ in long-term memory function had been previously advo­ ted tremendously from these technologies, with over 50 cated on the basis oflesion work (Milner, Petrides, & Smith, imaging studies ofmemory reported to date. Importantly, 1985; Moscovitch, 1982; Schacter, 1987; Shimamura, these studies have allowed researchers to propose and Janowsky, & Squire, 1991), but hemispheric asymmetry in test ideas about brain areas and pathways that might un­ relation to memory function was a completely novel pro­ derlie memory function (for examples see Buckner & Tul­ posal which helped draw attention to the commonalities ving, 1995; Petrides, Alivisatos, Evans, & Meyer, 1993; observed across a wide range ofneuroimaging studies. Raichle et al., 1994; Schacter, Alpert, Savage, Rauch, & Recently, the HERA model has been revisited in order Albert, 1996). Many ofthe results are in good agreement to determine how well it has predicted new data (Nyberg, with predictions based on earlier methodologies; other Cabeza, & Tulving, 1996). The current scorecard, which results have been entirely unanticipated. includes data collected after HERA's initial report (Tul­ On the basis ofunexpected findings about the lateral­ ving, Kapur, Craik, et aI., 1994), is well in favor of HERA, ization of prefrontal activations during memory tasks, suggesting that the model reliably captures a general pat­ Tulving, Kapur, Craik, Moscovitch, and Houle (1994) pro­ tern observed across many neuroimaging studies. posed a model ofhemispheric encoding/retrieval asym­ However, in spite ofthe HERA model's ability to pre­ metry (HERA). The HERA model drew on data from dict data on the average, several ofits shortcomings need to be addressed. The HERA model's main limitation is that it is minimally constrained in terms of prefrontal I thank Steve Petersen, Julie Fiez, Adina Roskies, and Susan Court­ anatomy and thus provides only an initial heuristic from ney for helpful comments on various versions ofthis manuscript and which to work. A second limitation is that the model fo­ Dan Schacter, Roger Ratcliff, John Gabrieli, Henry Roediger, and an cuses on prefrontal brain areas that differ across stages anonymous reviewer for thoughtful comments during the review process. Support was provided by NIH Grant NS32979 and grants and types of memory, while it underemphasizes com­ from the Charles A. Dana Foundation and the McDonnell Center for monalities-which may also be quite important. Higher Brain Function. The present research was conducted at Wash­ In this review, attention will be focused on a finer level ington University School ofMedicine, Department ofNeurology and of prefrontal analysis in line with our current under­ Neurological Surgery. Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to R. L. Buckner, MGH-NMR Center, 13th St., Building 149, standing ofprefrontal anatomy. This should not be taken Room 2301, Charlestown, MA 02129 (e-mail: [email protected]. as a step backward. The goal is not to show that the HERA harvard.edu). model is inaccurate, but rather to provide a more detailed 149 Copyright 1996 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 150 BUCKNER look at which prefrontal areas are involved in memory with regard to the human prefrontal cortex, it is essential function, and in what capacities. This is meant as a step to begin a functional characterization with the assump­ forward and as a complement to the HERA model. tion that multiple areas exist which may perform separa­ Evidence shows that neuroimaging techniques are ca­ ble operations (Buckner & Petersen, 1996). pable of resolving distinct subdivisions (areas) of the Given this orientation, the natural question becomes, prefrontal cortex. Examination of the prefrontal cortex Are we technically capable ofstudying the multiple func­ in terms of specific areas should yield a more complete tional subdivisions of human prefrontal cortex? Using description ofprefrontal contributions to memory. Sev­ traditional methodologies, such as lesion analysis, it is eral examples will be given which show how multiple, often difficult to divide the prefrontal cortex into func­ functionally distinct prefrontal areas contribute to long­ tional areas because of the limited availability of focal term memory retrieval processes; some ofthese areas are brain injuries. However, such characterization is begin­ located in the right prefrontal cortex, while others are lo­ ning and is likely to make important contributions (e.g., cated in the left. Neuroimaging studies have revealed Fiez, Damasio, & Tranel; 1995; Swick & Knight, in press). distinct prefrontal areas which have selective roles in Neuroimaging methods, however, serve as a ready memory retrieval that are not captured by descriptions tool for characterizing distinct prefrontal areas. These based solely on their laterality. methods permit the observation oflocal changes in me­ tabolism that are correlated with focused changes in CONCEPTUAL CONSIDERATIONS neural activity (DeYoe, Bandettini, Neitz, Miller, & Multiple Prefrontal Areas and the Ability of Winans, 1994; Raichle, 1987). Moreover, there are data Neuroimaging to Detect Them that can be used to determine the current ability of neu­ roimaging to localize and resolve activations within the Before a description of prefrontal contributions to prefrontal cortex-locali::ation referring to the accuracy memory can be made, it is important to discuss what, ex­ with which an activation can be placed within the pre­ actly, the prefrontal cortex is. The prefrontal cortex is a frontal cortex, and resolution referring to the distance by subdivision of the frontal cortex, the most anterior por­ which two areas of activation must be separated before tion of the human brain. Unlike the primary motor cor­ they can be identified as more than one area. Localiza­ tex and premotor cortex, which are also subdivisions of tion and resolution are similar in that they both enable the frontal cortex, the prefrontal cortex does not have the accurate description ofan activation. However, mul­ connections to brain areas that directly control move­ tiple areas of activation can be indistinguishable (poor ment and thus must act indirectly (through the premotor resolution) but nonetheless consistently localized to the and motor cortex) to participate in the production of same large area (reliable localization). Similarly, multi­ overt behaviors. Anatomically, the prefrontal cortex is ple activated areas can consistently appear as separate defined by the organization ofits cortical layers at a mi­ activations (good resolution), but their locations can croscopic level, its connections to other brain areas such vary from one subject group to the next (unreliable lo­ as the thalamus, and physiological properties (Goldman­ calization). It is thus important to address localization Rakic, 1987). and resolution separately. It has long been accepted that the prefrontal cortex is The ability of neuroimaging to reliably localize pre­ made up of multiple, functionally distinct areas. Brod­ frontal brain areas is demonstrated in studies ofthe same mann (1909/1994), using purely anatomical techniques, behavioral paradigm across independent groups of sub­ characterized the human prefrontal cortex and described jects imaged on separate occasions. Figure IA shows such (with some uncertainty) eight distinct prefrontal areas. an example, in which a left prefrontal activation was de­ The map that he produced, which included these areas and tected across three independent groups of subjects per­ a few more added later, is now widely used as a common forming
Recommended publications
  • Assessment of Confabulation in Patients RENSON Publishedonline11sepetember2015 GREEN
    King’s Research Portal DOI: 10.1080/13854046.2015.1084377 Document Version Peer reviewed version Link to publication record in King's Research Portal Citation for published version (APA): Renson, Y. C. M., Oosterman, J. M., van Damme, J. E., Griekspoor, S. I. A., Wester, A. J., Kopelman, M. D., & Kessels, R. P. C. (2015). Assessment of Confabulation in Patients with Alcohol-Related Cognitive Disorders: The Nijmegen–Venray Confabulation List (NVCL-20). The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 29, 804-823. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2015.1084377 Citing this paper Please note that where the full-text provided on King's Research Portal is the Author Accepted Manuscript or Post-Print version this may differ from the final Published version. If citing, it is advised that you check and use the publisher's definitive version for pagination, volume/issue, and date of publication details. And where the final published version is provided on the Research Portal, if citing you are again advised to check the publisher's website for any subsequent corrections. General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the Research Portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognize and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. •Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the Research Portal for the purpose of private study or research. •You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain •You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the Research Portal Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is It Like to Be Confabulating?
    What is it like to be Confabulating? Sahba Besharati, Aikaterini Fotopoulou and Michael D. Kopelman Kings College London, Institute of Psychiatry, London UK Different kinds of confabulations may arise in neurological and psychiatric disorders. This chapter first offers conceptual distinctions between spontaneous and momentary (“provoked”) confabulations, as well as between these types of confabulation and other kinds of false memories. The chapter then reviews current explanatory theories, emphasizing that both neurocognitive and motivational factors account for the content of confabulations. We place particular emphasis on a general model of confabulation that considers cognitive dysfunctions in memory and executive functioning in parallel with social and emotional factors. It is argued that all these dimensions need to be taken into account for a phenomenologically rich description of confabulation. The role of the motivated content of confabulation and the subjective experience of the patient are particularly relevant in effective management and rehabilitation strategies. Finally, we discuss a case example in order to illustrate how seemingly meaningless false memories are actually meaningful if placed in the context of the patient’s own perspective and autobiographical memory. Key words: Confabulation; False memory; Motivation; Self; Rehabilitation. 1 Memory is often subject to errors of omission and commission such that recollection includes instances of forgetting, or distorting past experience. The study of pathological forms of exaggerated memory distortion has provided useful insights into the mechanisms of normal reconstructive remembering (Johnson, 1991; Kopelman, 1999; Schacter, Norman & Kotstall, 1998). An extreme form of pathological memory distortion is confabulation. Different variants of confabulation are found to arise in neurological and psychiatric disorders.
    [Show full text]
  • Functional–Anatomic Correlates of Control Processes in Memory
    The Journal of Neuroscience, May 15, 2003 • 23(10):3999–4004 • 3999 Functional–Anatomic Correlates of Control Processes in Memory Randy L. Buckner Department of Psychology, Howard Hughes Medical Institute at Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri 63130 Introduction when memory retrieval requires flexible use of control processes A central role for control processes in long-term memory is ap- (Milner et al., 1985; Schacter, 1987; Shimmamura et al., 1991). In parent when one considers that there is far more information many cases, patients will fail to recall, or sometimes even recog- available in memory than can be accessed at any single moment nize, information (for review, see Wheeler et al., 1995) or inap- (Tulving and Pearlstone, 1966; Koriat, 2000). Much as selective propriately estimate the source or timing, or both, of a past epi- attention operates to focus processing on objects in the environ- sode (for review, see Schacter, 1987). In rare cases, a patient may ment, related control processes are required to constrain and make up episodes from the past while believing they really hap- select from information stored in memory. pened, a phenomenon called “confabulation” (Moscovitch, Neuroimaging studies have expanded our understanding of 1989; Burgess and Shallice, 1996). Unlike purer forms of amnesia, control processes in long-term memory in three important ways. which result from medial temporal and diencephalic lesions First, networks of regions, prominently including specific regions (Scoville and Milner, 1957; Squire, 1992; Cohen and Eichen- in prefrontal cortex, contribute to control processes during baum, 1993), the deficits after frontal damage align more to im- memory retrieval.
    [Show full text]
  • Disordered Memory Awareness: Recollective Confabulation in Two Cases of Persistent Déj`A Vecu
    Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 1362–1378 Disordered memory awareness: recollective confabulation in two cases of persistent dej´ a` vecu Christopher J.A. Moulin a,b,∗, Martin A. Conway b, Rebecca G. Thompson a, Niamh James a, Roy W. Jones a a The Research Institute for the Care of the Elderly, St. Martin’s Hospital, UK b Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, UK Received 7 April 2004; received in revised form 10 December 2004; accepted 16 December 2004 Available online 11 March 2005 Abstract We describe two cases of false recognition in patients with dementia and diffuse temporal lobe pathology who report their memory difficulty as being one of persistent dej´ a` vecu—the sensation that they have lived through the present moment before. On a number of recognition tasks, the patients were found to have high levels of false positives. They also made a large number of guess responses but otherwise appeared metacognitively intact. Informal reports suggested that the episodes of dej´ a` vecu were characterised by sensations similar to those present when the past is recollectively experienced in normal remembering. Two further experiments found that both patients had high levels of recollective experience for items they falsely recognized. Most strikingly, they were likely to recollectively experience incorrectly recognised low frequency words, suggesting that their false recognition was not driven by familiarity processes or vague sensations of having encountered events and stimuli before. Importantly, both patients made reasonable justifications for their false recognitions both in the experiments and in their everyday lives and these we term ‘recollective confabulation’.
    [Show full text]
  • Alcohol and Brain Damage in Adults with Reference to High-Risk Groups
    CR185 Alcohol and brain damage in adults With reference to high-risk groups © 2014 Royal College of Psychiatrists For full details of reports available and how to obtain them, contact the Book Sales Assistant at the Royal College of Psychiatrists, 21 Prescot Street, COLLEGE REPORT London E1 8BB (tel. 020 7235 2351; fax 020 3701 2761) or visit the College website at http://www.rcpsych.ac.uk/publications/collegereports.aspx Alcohol and brain damage in adults With reference to high-risk groups College report CR185 The Royal College of Psychiatrists, the Royal College of Physicians (London), the Royal College of General Practitioners and the Association of British Neurologists May 2014 Approved by the Policy Committee of the Royal College of Psychiatrists: September 2013 Due for review: 2018 © 2014 Royal College of Psychiatrists College Reports constitute College policy. They have been sanctioned by the College via the Policy Committee. For full details of reports available and how to obtain them, contact the Book Sales Assistant at the Royal College of Psychiatrists, 21 Prescot Street, London E1 8BB (tel. 020 7235 2351; fax 020 7245 1231) or visit the College website at http://www.rcpsych. ac.uk/publications/collegereports.aspx The Royal College of Psychiatrists is a charity registered in England and Wales (228636) and in Scotland (SC038369). | Contents List of abbreviations iv Working group v Executive summary and recommendations 1 Lay summary 6 Introduction 12 Clinical definition and diagnosis of alcohol-related brain damage and related
    [Show full text]
  • The Three Amnesias
    The Three Amnesias Russell M. Bauer, Ph.D. Department of Clinical and Health Psychology College of Public Health and Health Professions Evelyn F. and William L. McKnight Brain Institute University of Florida PO Box 100165 HSC Gainesville, FL 32610-0165 USA Bauer, R.M. (in press). The Three Amnesias. In J. Morgan and J.E. Ricker (Eds.), Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis/Psychology Press. The Three Amnesias - 2 During the past five decades, our understanding of memory and its disorders has increased dramatically. In 1950, very little was known about the localization of brain lesions causing amnesia. Despite a few clues in earlier literature, it came as a complete surprise in the early 1950’s that bilateral medial temporal resection caused amnesia. The importance of the thalamus in memory was hardly suspected until the 1970’s and the basal forebrain was an area virtually unknown to clinicians before the 1980’s. An animal model of the amnesic syndrome was not developed until the 1970’s. The famous case of Henry M. (H.M.), published by Scoville and Milner (1957), marked the beginning of what has been called the “golden age of memory”. Since that time, experimental analyses of amnesic patients, coupled with meticulous clinical description, pathological analysis, and, more recently, structural and functional imaging, has led to a clearer understanding of the nature and characteristics of the human amnesic syndrome. The amnesic syndrome does not affect all kinds of memory, and, conversely, memory disordered patients without full-blown amnesia (e.g., patients with frontal lesions) may have impairment in those cognitive processes that normally support remembering.
    [Show full text]
  • Disordered Recognition Memory: Recollective Confabulation
    cortex xxx (2013) 1e12 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Special issue: Research report Disordered recognition memory: Recollective confabulation Chris J.A. Moulin* Laboratoire d’Etude de l’Apprentissage et du De´veloppement, CNRS UMR 5022, Universite´ de Bourgogne, Dijon, France article info abstract Article history: Recollective confabulation (RC) is encountered as a conviction that a present moment is a Received 31 January 2012 repetition of one experienced previously, combined with the retrieval of confabulated Reviewed 12 April 2012 specifics to support that assertion. It is often described as persistent de´ja` vu by family Revised 24 September 2012 members and caregivers. On formal testing, patients with RC tend to produce a very high Accepted 24 January 2013 level of false positive errors. In this paper, a new case series of 11 people with dementia or Published online xxx mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and with de´ja` vu-like experiences is presented. In two experiments the nature of the recognition memory deficit is explored. The results from Keywords: these two experiments suggest e contrary to our hypothesis in earlier published case re- Dementia ports e that recollection mechanisms are relatively spared in this group, and that patients De´ja` vu experience familiarity for non-presented items. The RC patients tended to be overconfident Reduplicative paramnesia in their assessment of recognition memory, and produce inaccurate assessments of their Familiarity performance. These findings are discussed with reference to delusions more generally, and Metacognition point to a combined memory and metacognitive deficit, possibly arising from damage to temporal and right frontal regions.
    [Show full text]
  • Confabulation: a Guide for Mental Health Professionals Jerrod Brown1,2,3*, Deb Huntley1, Stephen Morgan1, Kimberly D Dodson4, and Janina Cich1,3
    ISSN: 2378-3001 Brown et al. Int J Neurol Neurother 2017, 4:070 International Journal of DOI: 10.23937/2378-3001/1410070 Volume 4 | Issue 2 Neurology and Neurotherapy Open Access REVIEW ARTICLE Confabulation: A Guide for Mental Health Professionals Jerrod Brown1,2,3*, Deb Huntley1, Stephen Morgan1, Kimberly D Dodson4, and Janina Cich1,3 1Concordia University, St. Paul, MN, USA 2Pathways Counseling Center, St. Paul, MN, USA 3 The American Institute for the Advancement of Forensic Studies, St. Paul, MN, USA Check for 4University of Houston - Clear Lake, TX, USA updates *Corresponding author: Jerrod Brown, Ph.D., MA, MS, MS, MS, Pathways Counseling Center, 1919 University Ave. W. Suite 6 St. Paul MN, 55104, USA, E-mail: [email protected] truthful memories [2-4]. These false memories may con- Abstract sist of exaggerations of actual events, inserting memo- Confabulation is the creation of false memories in the ab- ries of one event into another time or place, recalling an sence of intentions of deception. Individuals who confabu- late have no recognition that the information being relayed older memory but believing it took place more recently, to others is fabricated. Confabulating individuals are not filling in gaps in memory, or the creation of a new mem- intentionally being deceptive and sincerely believe the in- ory of an event that never occurred [5-7]. While some formation they are communicating to be genuine and accu- confabulated memories are easier to identify as false, rate. Confabulation ranges from small distortions of actual in other cases, the confabulated memory may be so memories to creation of bizarre and unusual memories, of- ten with elaborate detail.
    [Show full text]
  • Nutrition and Nutritional Supplements to Promote Brain Health
    Chapter 16 Nutrition and Nutritional Supplements to Promote Brain Health Abhilash K. Desai, Joy Rush, Lakshmi Naveen, and Papan Thaipisuttikul Abstract New scientific evidence strongly suggests that a variety of nutritional strategies can promote brain health and slow the course of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias (ADRD). Improving nutrition is, thus, a critical component of any comprehensive strategy to slow the course of ADRD. This chapter presents recom- mendations designed to meet this objective. Specific goals are to consume a balanced diet with particular focus on increasing consumption of brain power foods. Examples of brain power foods include oily fish (to be consumed at least twice a week), berries (espe- cially blueberries), green leafy vegetables, legumes, and turmeric. In addition, some nutritional supplements (especially vitamin D and vitamin B12) are also necessary to promote brain health. Multinutritional intervention, targeting multiple aspects of pro- cesses causing ADRD, instituted as early as possible is likely to have the greatest benefi- cial effect. Staying physically and mentally active and maintaining a healthy weight can greatly enhance the beneficial effects of nutritional interventions on brain health. Introduction Cognitive impairment associated with Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias (such as Vascular dementia, Dementia with Lewy Bodies, Parkinson’s Disease Dementia, Fronto-Temporal Dementia) (ADRD) causes impairment in daily function- ing and significant emotional distress. New scientific evidence has indicated that appropriate changes in a person’s diet can enhance their cognitive abilities, protect brain from damage, and may counteract the effects of dementia (Morley, 2010). A.K. Desai (*) Medical Director, Geriatric Psychiatry Director, Memory Clinic, Sheppard Pratt Health Systems, Associate Professor, Department of Neurology and Psychiatry, Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Saint Louis University School of Medicine, 605 N.
    [Show full text]
  • Objective Metamemory Testing Captures Awareness of Deficit in Alzheimer's Disease
    SPECIAL ISSUE: ORIGINAL ARTICLE OBJECTIVE METAMEMORY TESTING CAPTURES AWARENESS OF DEFICIT IN ALZHEIMER’S DISEASE Stephanie Cosentino1, Janet Metcalfe2, Brady Butterfield1 and Yaakov Stern1,2 (1Cognitive Neuroscience Division of the Taub Institute, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA; 2Department of Psychology, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA) ABSTRACT For reasons that remain unknown, there is marked inter-person variability in awareness of episodic memory loss in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Existing research designs, primarily subjective in nature, have been at a relative disadvantage for evaluating disordered metamemory and its relation to the clinical and neuropathological heterogeneity of AD, as well as its prognosis for various disease outcomes. The current study sought to establish an objective means of evaluating metamemory in AD by modifying traditional metacognitive paradigms in which participants are asked to make predictions regarding their own memory performance. Variables derived from this measure were analyzed in relation to clinically rated awareness for memory loss. As predicted, a range of awareness levels existed across patients with mild to moderate AD (n = 24) and clinical ratings of awareness (CRA) were significantly associated with verbal episodic memory monitoring (r = .46, p = .03). Further, patients who were rated as aware of their memory loss remained well calibrated over the course of the task whereas those rated as relatively unaware grew over-confident in their predictions [F (1, 33) = 4.19, p = .02]. Findings suggest that over-confidence may be related to impaired online error recognition and compromised use of metamemory strategies such as the Memory for Past Test (MPT) heuristic.
    [Show full text]
  • A Case Study of Confabulatory Hypermnesia
    cortex 45 (2009) 566–574 available at www.sciencedirect.com journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Research report ‘‘Do you remember what you did on March 13, 1985?’’ A case study of confabulatory hypermnesia Gianfranco Dalla Barbaa,b,c,d,* and Caroline Decaixe aInserm U. 610, Paris, France bUniversite´ Pierre et Marie Curie-Paris 6, Paris, France cAP-HP, Hoˆpital Henri Mondor, Service de Neurologie, Cre´teil, France dDipartimento di Psicologia, Universita` di Trieste, Trieste, Italy eAP-HP, Hoˆpital Saint Antoine, Service de Neurologie, Paris, France article info abstract Article history: We report on a patient, LM, with a Korsakoff’s syndrome who showed the unusual ten- Received 8 March 2007 dency to consistently provide a confabulatory answer to episodic memory questions for Reviewed 25 May 2007 which the predicted and most frequently observed response in normal subjects and in con- Revised 7 January 2008 fabulators is ‘‘I don’t know’’. LM’s pattern of confabulation, which we refer to as confabula- Accepted 11 March 2008 tory hypermnesia, cannot be traced back to any more basic and specific cognitive deficit and Action editor Michael Kopelman is not associated with any particularly unusual pattern of brain damage. Making reference Published online 5 June 2008 to the Memory, Consciousness and Temporality Theory – MCTT (Dalla Barba, 2002), we pro- pose that LM shows an expanded Temporal Consciousness – TC, which overflows the limits Keywords: of time (‘‘Do you remember what you did on March 13, 1985?’’) and of details (‘‘Do you re- Confabulation member what you were wearing on the first day of summer in 1979?’’) that are usually re- Episodic memory spected in normal subjects and in confabulating patients.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Disorders Anita Amelia Thompson Heisterman
    CHAPTER 31 Cognitive Disorders Anita Amelia Thompson Heisterman key terms learning objectives agnosia On completion of this chapter, you should be able to accomplish aphasia the following: apraxia 1. Define the term cognitive mental disorder. cognitive mental disorders confabulation 2. Discuss the incidence and significance of cognitive disorders. delirium 3. Identify clinical features or behaviors associated with cognitive dementia disorders. sundowning 4. Compare possible etiologies of various cognitive disorders, especially Alzheimer’s disease. 5. Explain the continuum of care and interdisciplinary treatment/ management for clients and families dealing with cognitive disorders. 6. Discuss common interventions for cognitive disorders. 7. Apply the steps of the nursing process to care for clients with cognitive disorders. UNIT 652 six Psychiatric Disorders ognitive mental disorders are characterized by a in a few days to weeks. In contrast, dementia results from C disruption of or deficit in cognitive function, which primary brain pathology that usually is irreversible, chronic, encompasses orientation, attention, memory, vocabulary, cal- and progressive. Prognosis with dementia depends on whether culation ability, and abstract thinking (see Chap. 10). Specific a cause can be identified and reversed. For example, prompt categories delineated by the American Psychiatric Associa- oxygen treatment for dementia stemming from hypoxia can tion (APA, 2000) include the following: prevent further damage. A comparison of the characteristics of delirium versus dementia is shown in TABLE 31.1. 1. Delirium, dementia, and amnestic and other cognitive With most cognitive disorders, the brain is temporarily disorders or permanently compromised. Usual consequences include 2. Mental disorders resulting from a general medical disturbed perceptions, delusions, paranoia, and aggressive condition (see Chap.
    [Show full text]