The Use of Cues for Attention in Ancient Greek Art: Aspects That Influence Concentration in the Work of Art and Its Elements Arte, Individuo Y Sociedad, Vol
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Arte, Individuo y Sociedad ISSN: 1131-5598 [email protected] Universidad Complutense de Madrid España Duarte, Antonio Manuel; Stefanakis, Manolis I. The use of cues for attention in ancient Greek art: aspects that influence concentration in the work of art and its elements Arte, Individuo y Sociedad, vol. 27, núm. 3, 2015, pp. 517-535 Universidad Complutense de Madrid Madrid, España Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=513551298010 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative The use of cues for attention in ancient Greek art: aspects that influence concentration in the work of art and its elements El uso de señales para la atención en el arte Griego antiguo: aspectos que influencian la concentración en la obra de arte y sus elementos Antonio Manuel Duarte Universidade de Lisboa, Faculdade de Psicologia [email protected] Manolis I. Stefanakis University of the Aegean [email protected] Recibido: 26 de septiembre de 2014 Aprobado: 8 de junio de 2015 Abstract The goal of this study is to identify cues for the cognitive process of attention in ancient Greek art, aiming to find confirmation of its possible use by ancient Greek audiences and artists. Evidence of cues that trigger attention’s psychological dispositions was searched through content analysis of image reproductions of ancient Greek sculpture and fine vase painting from the archaic to the Hellenistic period - ca. 7th -1st cent. BC. Through this analysis, it was possible to observe the presence of cues that trigger orientation to the work of art (i.e. amplification, contrast, emotional salience, simplification, symmetry), of a cue that triggers a disseminate attention to the parts of the work (i.e. distribution of elements) and of cues that activate selective attention to specific elements in the work of art (i.e. contrast of elements, salient color, central positioning of elements, composition regarding the flow of elements and significant objects). Results support the universality of those dispositions, probably connected with basic competencies that are hard-wired in the nervous system and in the cognitive processes. Keywords: attention, ancient Greek art, selective attention, psychology of art. Duarte, A.M., Stefanakis, M. I. (2015): The use of cues for attention in ancient Greek art: aspects that influence concentration in the work of art and its elements. Arte, Individuo y Sociedad, 27(3) 517-535 Arte, Individuo y Sociedad 517 ISSN: 1131-5598 2015, 27(3) 517-535 http://dx.doi.org/10.5209/rev_ARIS.2015.v27.n3.46654 Antonio Manuel Duarte, Manolis I. Stefanakis The use of cues for attention in ancient Greek... Resumen El objetivo de este estudio es identificar las señales para el proceso cognitivo de la atención en el arte Griego antiguo, con la intención de detectar su posible uso por los antiguos griegos (público y artistas). La evidencia de señales que desencadenan las disposiciones psicológicas de la atención se buscó mediante el análisis de contenido de reproducciones de imágenes de arte Griego antiguo (escultura y pintura de vasos desde el período arcaico hasta el período Helenístico – ca. 7º-1º siglo AC). Por medio de este análisis fue posible observar la presencia de señales que condicionan la orientación para la obra de arte (i.e. amplificación, contraste, énfasis emocional, simplificación, simetría), de una señal que provoca una atención diseminada a las partes de la obra (i.e. distribución de los elementos) y de señales que activan una atención selectiva a las partes de la obra (i.e. contraste de los elementos, color saliente, posicionamiento central de los elementos, composición con respecto al fluir de los elementos y objetos significativos). Los resultados apoyan la universalidad de esas disposiciones, probablemente relacionado con competencias básicas del sistema nervioso y de los procesos cognitivos. Palabras clave: atención, atención selectiva, arte Griego antiguo, psicología del arte. Summary: 1. Introduction, 2. Method, 3. Results, 3.1. Amplification, 3.2. Contrast, 3.3. Emotional Salience, 3.4.Simplification, 3.5. Symmetry, 3.6. Distribution and Contrast of Elements, Salient Color, 3.7. Central Positioning of Elements, 3.8. Composition Regarding the Flow of Elements, 3.9. Significant Objects, 4. Discussion. References. 1. Introduction Archaeology became interested in how and why art has been possibly created and appreciated by the groups that produced and used it. This interest is mainly oriented to pre-history (Dissanayake, 1992; Lewis-Williams, 2002; Lewis-Williams & Pearce, 2005; Mithen, 1990, 1996) with few attempts that consider the ancient world period (Gonçalves, Moura, Magalhães & Chalmers, 2013; Lewis-Williams & Pearce, 2005). In parallel, Psychology has an old interest on how and why people create and appreciate works of art (Arnheim, 1974, 1986, 1988; Gombrich, 1977; Solso, 1996). Our approach for this study follows the perspective that art functions in a context that demands psychological “active” creators and spectators (Arnheim, 1986; Gombrich, 1977). To make sense out of a work of art, the spectator needs to actively react to it through mental and behavioral processes. These consist of emotional reactions, behaviors and cognitive operations, which use several “cues” of the work of art in order to “construct” an internal coherent representation, on the basis of the stimulus that constitutes it. Correlatively, the artists’ effort is to produce a work of art that contains (or not) the necessary “cues” in order to allow (or not) the spectator to process it for a coherent desirable experience. Therefore, the artist must have an intuitive knowledge on how his or her audience naturally reacts to stimuli and must arrange the work’s stimulus in a way that, being cognitively processed, produces an intended experience (Gombrich, 1977). A propos we should note that the term “aesthetics” derives from the ancient Greek αἰσθάνομαι, which means “to perceive”, emphasizing the general sensory experience and reactions that stimuli produce (Feagin, 1999). The thesis here presented is illustrated by a story reported by Ioannes Tzetzes of Byzantium (1110 - 1180 AD), and remembered by Gombrich in his “Art 518 Arte, Individuo y Sociedad 2015, 27(3) 517-535 Antonio Manuel Duarte, Manolis I. Stefanakis The use of cues for attention in ancient Greek... and Illusion” (1977), regarding the competition for a high pillar statue of Athena between Phidias and Alcamenes, in the Athens of ancient Greece. While Alcamenes presented a realist figure for the goddess, Phidias exaggerated the features of her face and body, considering the distance from which it would be seen. “(…) when these two images were afterwards brought to light and compared, Phidias was in great danger to have been stoned by the whole multitude, until the statues where at length set on high. For Alcamenes his sweet and diligent strokes being drowned, and Phidias his disfigured and distorted hardness being vanished by the height of the place, made Alcamenes to be laughed at, and Phidias to be much more esteemed.” (Junius 1638, p. 232 - 233 cit. Gombrich, 1977, p. 153-154). Specifically, artists’ control of spectators’ reactions involves arranging “cues” directed to their emotions (Vessel, Starr & Rubin, 2012) and their cognitive processes (Leder, 2013; Leder, Belke, Oeberst & Augustin, 2004): attention; sensorial processing; perception; comprehension; interpretation. Nevertheless, this separation is more descriptive than concrete since, assuming a “continuity of the mind” (Spivey, 2006), art appreciation would correspond more to a continuum than to a sequence of stages. Each of the mentioned cognitive processes involves mental activity describable in terms of cognitive “dispositions”. These are specific ways of reacting to art stimulus, not necessarily consciously for the spectator (or for the artist) and frequently automatic. In this sense, the perspectives of “Extended Mind Theory” and “Material Engagement Theory” also have relevance for the angle of this article. “Extended Mind Theory” emphasizes interaction between mental states and the physical-social environment therefore conceptualizing cognitive processes as interactive networks (that connect brain, body and world) and cultural artifacts as extensions of the cognitive system (Clarck, 2008; Clarck & Chalmers, 1998). In parallel, “Material Engagement Theory” points that human beings think through the objects they create, that gain then a “cognitive life” (Malafouris & Renfrew, 2010). The perspective of art as a “(…) kind of cognitive engineering. (…) an activivity intendend to influence the minds of an audience.” considers that art expresses a human propensity for joint and reciprocal control of attention, which is seen as the basis of communication (Donald, 2006, p. 4). It seems then that the control and involvement of attention is an essential basic aspect of the aesthetic encounter, even if besides attention to visual form this encounter presupposes a conception of the work of art “(…) as a thing of a particular kind (…)” (Levinson, 2011, p. 205). Now regarding the cognitive process of attention to the work of art – precisely the focus of this study – psychology of art has been identifying a range of correspondent “cues”, revised in the next sections: amplification; contrast; emotional