Whole Language and the Great Plummet of 1987-92
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Focus on Reading Whole Language and the Great Plummet of 1987-92 AN URBAN LEGEND FROM CALIFORNIA There is compelling evidence that California’s low reading scores are related to California’s impoverished print environment, not to the introduction of the whole-language approach to literacy, Mr. Krashen points out. BY STEPHEN KRASHEN HERE ARE a number of ways to define an urban legend. Here’s one from the Urban Legends Research Centre: “An Urban Legend is usually a (good/captivating/titillating/en- grossing/incredible/worrying) story that has had a wide au- dience, is circulated spontaneously, has been told in sever- al forms, and which many have chosen to believe (whether actively or passively) despite the lack of actual evidence to substantiate the story.”1 T I wish to add another urban legend to those that already exist, a legend that I believe ranks with the legend of the alligators living in the sewers of New York City.2 I will refer to it as the “Plummet Legend.” It goes like this. After whole language was introduced in California in 1987, test scores “plummeted” to the point where California’s fourth-graders were last in the country in 1992. It makes a good story, if we can judge by the number of times it has been repeated. But this sudden plummet never hap- pened. It is an urban legend, a captivating and worrisome story that has been told in several forms and that many people have chosen the legend is inconsistent with the results of studies of lit- to believe despite the lack of actual evidence. eracy development. The Plummet Legend has had serious consequences. It has led to the discrediting of the whole-language approach DID TEST SCORES PLUMMET IN CALIFORNIA? to literacy and has nurtured a strong movement promoting a “skill-building” approach.3 I will try to show here both Here is a more complete version of the Plummet Leg- that the evidence does not support this legend and that end. In 1987 a group of whole-language advocates took STEPHEN KRASHEN is professor emeritus of education at the over the California Language Arts Framework Committee University of Southern California, Los Angeles. and brought in whole language. Phonics instruction and 748 PHI DELTA KAPPAN Photo: Eyewire Images other forms of direct teaching were banned, and language mentary schools had only 12 books per child. Although scores plummeted to the point where California’s fourth- some of this decline was due to the pruning of old books, graders scored last in the country in reading in 1992. Cali- there is no sign of improvement.7 fornia is now recovering from this damage, thanks to a ra- • The same 1990 study reported 4,595 students per tional, sensible phonics-based approach to reading. school librarian in California. The national ratio was 900 This is not what happened. I served on the California to 1.8 In 1998 California had 4,673 students per school li- Language Arts Framework Committee in 1987. Phonics brarian.9 teaching was not banned. We simply proposed that lan- • California’s public libraries are not impressive. Ac- guage arts should be “literature-based.” This is hardly con- cording to data published in 1997, California’s public li- troversial. In fact, I regarded it as part of the definition of braries had 1.9 volumes per capita. The national average language arts. was 2.8. Only three states were worse. California’s public Did teachers change their ways in California? Nobody libraries circulate 4.9 books per capita, per year. The na- really knows. There have been no empirical studies com- tional average is 6.6. Only 10 states are lower.10 paring methodology in language arts teaching before and • California now ranks in the bottom eight among states after the 1987 committee met. in terms of percentage of children between the ages of 5 Did test scores decline? It is certainly true that Califor- and 17 who live in poverty.11 nia fourth-graders scored last in the country in the fourth- What’s more, print-access variables are strongly corre- grade National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) lated with NAEP reading scores. McQuillan reported a cor- in reading in 1992. But this was the first time NAEP scores relation of .85 between measures of print access (books had been presented by state. It was assumed that there had and other forms of print available in the home, school, and been a decline, but there was no evidence that this was so, community) and 1994 NAEP scores.12 Controlling for pov- for no comparison with earlier test scores was made. Jeff erty, the correlation remained high (r=.63).13 California’s McQuillan examined CAP (California Achievement Pro- problem is not whole language but a lack of reading ma- gram) reading comprehension scores from 1984 to 1990, terial. which I present in Table 1. There is no clear pattern of in- Independent research supports McQuillan’s analysis. creases or decreases during these years, which leads to There is excellent evidence that children with more ac- the conclusion that California’s read- ing problem existed well before “whole TABLE 1. language” was introduced in 1987. There CAP Scores in California, 1984-90 was no Great Plummet of 1987-92.4 McQuillan also provides a convinc- Grade 1984 1985 1986 1987 1988 1989 1990 ing explanation for the low scores. There 3 268 274 280 282 282 277 275 is strong evidence that California’s poor 6 249 253 260 260 265 262 262 performance is related to its print-poor 8 250 240 243 247 252 256 257 environment. California ranks last in 12 236 241 240 246 250 248 251 the country in the quality of its school Source:Jeff McQuillan, The Literacy Crisis:False Claims and Real Solutions (Portsmouth, N.H.:Heine- libraries and ranks near the bottom in mann, 1998). the quality of its public libraries. In ad- dition, many of its children have very little reading materi- cess to books read more and that children who read more al at home. California ranked ninth in the country in the make superior gains in literacy development. percentage of children between the ages of 5 and 17 who lived in poverty in 1995, and it ranked near the bottom in ACCESS LEADS TO READING the percentage of homes with more than 25 books.5 McQuillan’s analysis was based on school library data There is a great deal of evidence showing that children published in 1990 and public library data published in with more access to books read more. Children with more 1995. There has been little change in California since that books in the home read more.14 Barbara Heyns reported time. that children who live close to public libraries read more • A study published in 1990 reported that California’s than those who live far away.15 Leslie Morrow and Carol school libraries had 13 books per child; the national av- Weinstein found that installing well-designed library cor- erage was 18 books per child.6 In 2001, California ele- ners in kindergartens resulted in more use of books by the JUNE 2002 749 children during intervals of free play.16 In a study of high was defined correctly — that is, as including a great deal school libraries, Rachel Houle and Claude Montmarquette of real reading — students in these classes performed as reported that students take more books out of school li- well as or better than children in skills classes on tests of braries that have more books and that stay open longer.17 reading comprehension, were equivalent to children in McQuillan and Julie Au reported that high school students skills-based classes on tests of “skills” (e.g., reading non- did more reading when their teachers took them to the sense words), had more positive attitudes toward reading, school library more often on planned library visits.18 and read more on their own.24 Once again, the federal government thinks otherwise. The NRP concluded that skills-based methods were supe- READING LEADS TO LITERACY DEVELOPMENT rior to whole-language methods (d=.31).25 In my reanaly- Until recently, it was considered obvious that actual sis, I considered performance on tests of reading compre- reading helps readers get better and helps them improve hension, and I also considered the amount of reading their vocabulary, grammar, spelling, and writing. The U.S. done. I found an overall advantage for whole language government disagrees. The National Reading Panel (NRP) (d=.17). For the four studies in which it was clear that one concluded that there is insufficient research to support the group did more real reading, the advantage for the read- hypothesis that reading itself is beneficial and concluded ers was substantial (d=.70).26 that we should concentrate our efforts on phonemic aware- ness training, intensive phonics, and having children read BETTER LIBRARIES LEAD TO BETTER READING aloud so that their errors can be corrected.19 The NRP overlooked a tremendous amount of research. If more access to books results in more reading and The case for recreational reading is overwhelming. It con- more reading results in more literacy development, it fol- sists of many case histories in which it is clear that read- lows that more access to books will result in more litera- ing was the causative factor in helping individuals — such cy development, and research confirms that this is the case. as Richard Wright, Malcolm X, and Ben Carson — increase A particularly important aspect of this research deals with their level of literacy development.20 It includes studies in the impact of libraries.