Administration & Society

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Administration & Society ADMINISTRATION & SOCIETY Editor: GARY L. WAMSLEY, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University Associate Editors: Applied Behavioral Science: Management and Business Administration: ANDRE DELBECQ, University of Santa Clara Constitutionalism, Ethics, and Administrative History: JOHN A. ROHR, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University Organizational Sociology: DAVID A. WHETTEN, Brigham Young University Contact Editor for European Government & Administration: THEO A. J. TOONEN, University of Leiden Editorial Board: Carolyn Ban, University of Pittsburgh Barry Bozeman, Georgia Institute of Technology Louise K. Comfort, University of Pittsburgh Phillip J. Cooper, University of Vermont Robert B. Denhardt, Arizona State University Michael A. Diamond, University of Missouri–Columbia Robert F.Durant, University of Baltimore Frank Fischer, Rutgers University Louis C. Gawthrop, University of Baltimore Robert Golembiewski, University of Georgia Charles T. Goodsell, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University Michael M. Harmon, George Washington University David K. Hart, Brigham Young University Christopher Hood, Department of Government, London School of Economics and Political Science Patricia W. Ingraham, Syracuse University Manfred F.R. Kets de Vries, European Institute of Business Administration Donald F.Kettl, University of Wisconsin–Madison Alexander Kouzmin, University of Western Sydney–Nepean Martin Landau, University of California–Berkeley Theodore J. Lowi, Cornell University Thomas D. Lynch, Louisiana State University Laurence E. Lynn, Jr., University of Chicago Cynthia J. McSwain, George Washington University Kenneth J. Meier, Texas A&M University Yves Mény, Robert Schuman Centre, European University Institute–Florence H. Brinton Milward, University of Arizona Terry Moe, Stanford University Gareth Morgan, York University, Toronto Dorothy Olshfski, Rutgers University Laurence J. O’Toole, Jr., University of Georgia James L. Perry, Indiana University Keith G. Provan, University of Arizona Hal G. Rainey, University of Georgia Bert A. Rockman, Ohio State University Barbara Romzek, University of Kansas David H. Rosenbloom, American University Uriel Rosenthal, University of Leiden Lester M. Salamon, Johns Hopkins University Fritz W. Scharpf, Max Planck Institute for Social Research Saundra K. Schneider, University of South Carolina Camilla Stivers, Cleveland State University–Ohio Michael E. Urban, University of California–Santa Cruz Orion F.White, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University Oliver E. Williamson, University of California–Berkeley James Q. Wilson, University of California–Los Angeles Editorial Assistant: Brandy S. Faulkner, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University For Sage Publications: Scott Springer, Michelle Fisher, Joyce Kuhn, and Katinka Baltazar ADMINISTRATION & SOCIETY volume 33, number 5 / November 2001 contents A Reflection on Herbert Simon: A Satisficing Search for Significance HAL G. RAINEY 491 Culture, Commerce, and Civil Society: Rethinking Support for the Arts STEFAN TOEPLER 508 Welfare Reform in Wisconsin: The Local Role LAWRENCE M. MEAD 523 Institutions of Private Authority in Global Governance: Linking Territorial Forms of Self-Regulation KARSTEN RONIT 555 Sage Publications Thousand Oaks • London • New Delhi POLICY: ADMINISTRATION & SOCIETY seeks to further the understanding of public and human service organizations, their administrative processes, and their effect on society. The journal publishes empirically oriented research reports and theoretically specific articles that synthesize or contribute to the advance- ment of understanding and explanation in these fields. Of particular interest are (1) studies that analyze the effects of the introduction of administrative strategies, programs, change interventions, and training; and (2) studies of intergroup, interorganizational, and organization-environment relationships and policy processes. MANUSCRIPTS should be submitted to Gary L. Wamsley, Editor, ADMINISTRATION & SOCIETY, Center for Public Administration and Policy, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, VA 24061. Four (4) copies are required. A copy of the final revised manuscript saved on an IBM-compatible disk should be included with the final revised hard copy.Submission of a manuscript implies commitment to pub- lish in the journal. Authors submitting manuscripts to the journal should not simultaneously submit them to another journal, nor should manuscripts have been published elsewhere in substantially similar form or with substantially similar content. Manuscripts must be prepared in accordance with the guidelines of the Publi- cation Manual of the American Psychological Association (APA) (5th ed.). Articles should not exceed 40 typewritten, double-spaced pages. Tables, charts, notes, and references must all be on separate pages, also completely double-spaced. Research notes should not exceed 15 typewritten double-spaced pages. A double-spaced abstract of approximately 100 words as well as a double-spaced brief biographical para- graph describing each author’s affiliation, research interest, and recent publications should accompany the manuscript. Because the manuscripts are sent out anonymously for editorial evaluation, the author’s name, affiliation, and biographical paragraph should appear on a separate cover page. In addition, to help ensure anonymity in the review process, three (3) of the four (4) copies should have all references to the author(s) left blank. This includes any references in the manuscript, the notes, the title, and reference sections. ADMINISTRATION & SOCIETY (ISSN 0095-3997) is published six times annually—in March, May, July, September, November, and January—by Sage Publications, 2455 Teller Road, Thousand Oaks, CA 91320; telephone: (800) 818-SAGE (7243) and (805) 499-9774; fax/order line: (805) 375-1700; e-mail: [email protected]; http://www. sagepub.com. Copyright © 2002 by Sage Publications. All rights reserved. No portion of the contents may be repro- duced in any form without written permission of the publisher. Subscriptions: Annual subscription rates for institutions and individuals are based on the current frequency.Prices quoted are in U.S. dollars and are subject to change without notice. Canadian subscribers add 7% GST (and HST as appropriate). Outside U.S. subscription rates include shipping via air-speeded delivery. Institutions: $470 (within the U.S.) / $494 (outside the U.S.) / single issue: $91 (worldwide). Individuals: $88 (within the U.S.) / $112 (outside the U.S.) / single issue:$24 (worldwide).Noninstitutional orders must be paid by personal check, VISA, or MasterCard. Periodicals postage paid at Thousand Oaks, California, and at additional mailing offices. This journal is abstracted or indexed in Academic Search, America: History and Life, Business Source, Commu- nication Abstracts, Corporate ResourceNET, CSA Political Science and Government: A Guide to Periodical Literature, Current Citations Express, Current Contents: Social & Behavioral Sciences, Expanded Academic Index, General Periodicals Index/ASAP, Historical Abstracts, International Bibliography of the Social Sci- ences, LEXIS-NEXIS, MasterFILE FullTEXT, PAIS International, Peace Research Abstracts Journal, Periodi- cal Abstracts, Political Science Abstracts, Sage Public Administration Abstracts, Social Services Abstracts, Sociological Abstracts, Sociology of Education Abstracts, Standard Periodical Directory (SPD), TOPICsearch, Urban Affairs Abstracts, and Wilson Social Sciences Index/Abstracts and is available on micro- film from University Microfilms, Ann Arbor, Michigan. Back Issues: Information about availability and prices of back issues may be obtained from the publisher’s order de- partment (address below). Single-issue orders for 5 or more copies will receive a special adoption discount. Contact the order department for details. Write to the London office for sterling prices. Inquiries: All subscription inquiries, orders, and renewals with ship-to addresses in North America, South America, Australia, China, Indonesia, Japan, Korea, New Zealand, and the Philippines must be addressed to Sage Publica- tions, 2455 TellerRoad, Thousand Oaks, CA 91320, U.S.A.; telephone: (800) 818-SAGE (7243) and (805) 499-9774; fax: (805) 375-1700; e-mail: [email protected]; http://www.sagepub.com. All subscription inquiries, orders, and renewals with ship-to addresses in the U.K., Europe, the Middle East, and Africa must be addressed to Sage Publica- tions Ltd, 6 Bonhill Street, London EC2A 4PU, England; telephone: +44 (0)20 7374-0645; fax: +44 (0)20 7374-8741. All subscription inquiries, orders, and renewals with ship-to addresses in India and South Asia must be addressed to Sage Publications Private Ltd, P.O. Box 4215, New Delhi 110 048 India; telephone: (91-11) 641-9884; fax: (91-11) 647-2426. Address all permissions requests to the Thousand Oaks office. Authorization to photocopy items for internal or personal use, or the internal or personal use of specific clients, is granted by Sage Publications for libraries and other users registered with the Copyright Clearance Center (CCC) Transactional Reporting Service, provided that the base fee of 50¢ per copy, plus 10¢ per copy page, is paid directly to CCC, 21 Congress St., Salem, MA 01970. 0095-3997/2002 $.50 + .10. Advertising: Current rates and specifications may be obtained by writing to the Advertising Manager at the Thou- sand Oaks office (address above). Claims: Claims
Recommended publications
  • Psycholinguistics
    11/09/2013 Psycholinguistics What do these activities have in common? What kind of process is involved in producing and understanding language? 1 11/09/2013 Questions • What is psycholinguistics? • What are the main topics of psycholinguistics? 9.1 Introduction • * Psycholinguistics is the study of the language processing mechanisms. Psycholinguistics deals with the mental processes a person uses in producing and understanding language. It is concerned with the relationship between language and the human mind, for example, how word, sentence, and discourse meaning are represented and computed in the mind. 2 11/09/2013 9.1 Introduction * As the name suggests, it is a subject which links psychology and linguistics. • Psycholinguistics is interdisciplinary in nature and is studied by people in a variety of fields, such as psychology, cognitive science, and linguistics. It is an area of study which draws insights from linguistics and psychology and focuses upon the comprehension and production of language. • The scope of psycholinguistics • The common aim of psycholinguists is to find out the structures and processes which underline a human’s ability to speak and understand language. • Psycholinguists are not necessarily interested in language interaction between people. They are trying above all to probe into what is happening within the individual. 3 11/09/2013 The scope of psycholinguistics • At its heart, psycholinguistic work consists of two questions. – What knowledge of language is needed for us to use language? – What processes are involved in the use of language? The “knowledge” question • Four broad areas of language knowledge: Semantics deals with the meanings of sentences and words.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology, Meaning Making and the Study of Worldviews: Beyond Religion and Non-Religion
    Psychology, Meaning Making and the Study of Worldviews: Beyond Religion and Non-Religion Ann Taves, University of California, Santa Barbara Egil Asprem, Stockholm University Elliott Ihm, University of California, Santa Barbara Abstract: To get beyond the solely negative identities signaled by atheism and agnosticism, we have to conceptualize an object of study that includes religions and non-religions. We advocate a shift from “religions” to “worldviews” and define worldviews in terms of the human ability to ask and reflect on “big questions” ([BQs], e.g., what exists? how should we live?). From a worldviews perspective, atheism, agnosticism, and theism are competing claims about one feature of reality and can be combined with various answers to the BQs to generate a wide range of worldviews. To lay a foundation for the multidisciplinary study of worldviews that includes psychology and other sciences, we ground them in humans’ evolved world-making capacities. Conceptualizing worldviews in this way allows us to identify, refine, and connect concepts that are appropriate to different levels of analysis. We argue that the language of enacted and articulated worldviews (for humans) and worldmaking and ways of life (for humans and other animals) is appropriate at the level of persons or organisms and the language of sense making, schemas, and meaning frameworks is appropriate at the cognitive level (for humans and other animals). Viewing the meaning making processes that enable humans to generate worldviews from an evolutionary perspective allows us to raise news questions for psychology with particular relevance for the study of nonreligious worldviews. Keywords: worldviews, meaning making, religion, nonreligion Acknowledgments: The authors would like to thank Raymond F.
    [Show full text]
  • The Three Meanings of Meaning in Life: Distinguishing Coherence, Purpose, and Significance
    The Journal of Positive Psychology Dedicated to furthering research and promoting good practice ISSN: 1743-9760 (Print) 1743-9779 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rpos20 The three meanings of meaning in life: Distinguishing coherence, purpose, and significance Frank Martela & Michael F. Steger To cite this article: Frank Martela & Michael F. Steger (2016) The three meanings of meaning in life: Distinguishing coherence, purpose, and significance, The Journal of Positive Psychology, 11:5, 531-545, DOI: 10.1080/17439760.2015.1137623 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2015.1137623 Published online: 27 Jan 2016. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 425 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rpos20 Download by: [Colorado State University] Date: 06 July 2016, At: 11:55 The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2016 Vol. 11, No. 5, 531–545, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2015.1137623 The three meanings of meaning in life: Distinguishing coherence, purpose, and significance Frank Martelaa* and Michael F. Stegerb,c aFaculty of Theology, University of Helsinki, P.O. Box 4, Helsinki 00014, Finland; bDepartment of Psychology, Colorado State University, 1876 Campus Delivery, Fort Collins, CO 80523-1876, USA; cSchool of Behavioural Sciences, North-West University, Vanderbijlpark, South Africa (Received 25 June 2015; accepted 3 December 2015) Despite growing interest in meaning in life, many have voiced their concern over the conceptual refinement of the con- struct itself. Researchers seem to have two main ways to understand what meaning in life means: coherence and pur- pose, with a third way, significance, gaining increasing attention.
    [Show full text]
  • Between Psychology and Philosophy East-West Themes and Beyond
    PALGRAVE STUDIES IN COMPARATIVE EAST-WEST PHILOSOPHY Between Psychology and Philosophy East-West Themes and Beyond Michael Slote Palgrave Studies in Comparative East-West Philosophy Series Editors Chienkuo Mi Philosophy Soochow University Taipei City, Taiwan Michael Slote Philosophy Department University of Miami Coral Gables, FL, USA The purpose of Palgrave Studies in Comparative East-West Philosophy is to generate mutual understanding between Western and Chinese philoso- phers in a world of increased communication. It has now been clear for some time that the philosophers of East and West need to learn from each other and this series seeks to expand on that collaboration, publishing books by philosophers from different parts of the globe, independently and in partnership, on themes of mutual interest and currency. The series also publishs monographs of the Soochow University Lectures and the Nankai Lectures. Both lectures series host world-renowned philosophers offering new and innovative research and thought. More information about this series at http://www.palgrave.com/gp/series/16356 Michael Slote Between Psychology and Philosophy East-West Themes and Beyond Michael Slote Philosophy Department University of Miami Coral Gables, FL, USA ISSN 2662-2378 ISSN 2662-2386 (electronic) Palgrave Studies in Comparative East-West Philosophy ISBN 978-3-030-22502-5 ISBN 978-3-030-22503-2 (eBook) https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-22503-2 © The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s) 2020. This book is an open access publication. Open Access This book is licensed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence and indicate if changes were made.
    [Show full text]
  • Meaning in Life in Psychotherapy: the Perspective of Experienced Psychotherapists
    Marquette University e-Publications@Marquette College of Education Faculty Research and Publications Education, College of 11-2015 Meaning in life in psychotherapy: The perspective of experienced psychotherapists Clara E. Hill University of Maryland - College Park Yoshi Kanazawa Meigi Gakuin University - Tokyo, Japan Sarah Knox Marquette University, [email protected] Iris Schauerman University of Maryland - College Park Darren Loureiro University of Maryland - College Park See next page for additional authors Follow this and additional works at: https://epublications.marquette.edu/edu_fac Part of the Education Commons Recommended Citation Hill, Clara E.; Kanazawa, Yoshi; Knox, Sarah; Schauerman, Iris; Loureiro, Darren; James, Danielle; Carter, Imani; King, Shakeena; Razzak, Suad; Scarff, Melanie; and Moore, Jasmine, "Meaning in life in psychotherapy: The perspective of experienced psychotherapists" (2015). College of Education Faculty Research and Publications. 385. https://epublications.marquette.edu/edu_fac/385 Authors Clara E. Hill, Yoshi Kanazawa, Sarah Knox, Iris Schauerman, Darren Loureiro, Danielle James, Imani Carter, Shakeena King, Suad Razzak, Melanie Scarff, and Jasmine Moore This article is available at e-Publications@Marquette: https://epublications.marquette.edu/edu_fac/385 Marquette University e-Publications@Marquette Education Faculty Research and Publications/College of Education This paper is NOT THE PUBLISHED VERSION. Access the published version at the link in the citation below. Psychotherapy Research, Vol. 27, No. 4 (July 2017): 381–396. DOI. This article is © Routledge Taylor & Francis and permission has been granted for this version to appear in e-Publications@Marquette. Routledge Taylor & Francis does not grant permission for this article to be further copied/distributed or hosted elsewhere without the express permission from Routledge Taylor & Francis.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Psychology
    SN-05(153-196) 3/4/02 2:12 PM Page 153 CHAPTER 5 Evolutionary psychology esearchers committed to an evolutionary perspective Ron humanity were initially united in the face of wide- spread hostility to human sociobiology. However, in the 1980s, as the number of investigators using evolution to study human behaviour increased, subgroups began to emerge with different opinions on how best to proceed. One such subgroup was dominated by academic psycho- logists searching for the evolved psychological mechanisms that they envisaged underpinned any universal mental and behavioural characteristics of humanity. While the intellec- tual roots of some of these practitioners could be traced to human sociobiology, or to the study of animal behaviour, the majority were fresh recruits who sought to differentiate themselves from human sociobiology, and restyled them- selves as Darwinian or evolutionary psychologists. For Leda Cosmides and John Tooby, two of the pioneers of this new discipline, evolutionary psychology owed little intellectual debt to Edward Wilson but did draw inspiration from the writings of Bill Hamilton, Robert Trivers, and George Williams. Tooby, a Harvard-trained anthropologist who had worked closely with Irven DeVore, and Cosmides, a psychologist also from Harvard, were brought by Donald Symons to Santa Barbara where they founded the first Center for Research in Evolutionary Psychology. SN-05(153-196) 3/4/02 2:12 PM Page 154 154 SENSE AND NONSENSE The ‘Santa Barbara school’1 were concerned that human sociobiologists and behavioural ecologists had neglected psychological adaptations: In the rush to apply evolutionary insights to a science of human behavior, many researchers have made a conceptual ‘wrong turn’, leaving a gap in the evolutionary approach that has limited its effectiveness.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology Defined ᮢ
    Psychology Defined ᮢ Gregg R. Henriques James Madison University A new form of knowledge technology is used to diagnose psychology’s epistemological woes and provide a solution to the difficulties. The argu- ment presented is that psychology has traditionally spanned two separate but intimately related problems: (a) the problem of animal behavior and (b) the problem of human behavior. Accordingly, the solution offered divides the field into two broad, logically consistent domains. The first domain is psychological formalism, which is defined as the science of mind, corre- sponds to animal behavior, and consists of the basic psychological sci- ences. The second domain is human psychology, which is defined as the science of human behavior at the individual level and is proposed as a hybrid that exists between psychological formalism and the social sciences. © 2004 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Clin Psychol 60: 1207–1221, 2004. Keywords: Tree of Knowledge (ToK) System; psychological formalism; unified theory; mental behaviorism; Justification Hypothesis We persevere in looking at small questions instead of large ones and our view of the forest is forever obscured by the trees. (Bevan, 1991, p. 475) What is psychology? Is it a single, coherent scientific discipline awaiting transfor- mation from the current preparadigmatic state into a more mature unified one? Or, is it a heterogeneous federation of subdisciplines that will ultimately fragment into a multitude of smaller, more specialized fields? This is, in essence, the “to be or not to be” question of the field. Currently, psychology exists as an uneasy compromise between unification and fragmentation. On the one hand, the existence of numerous societal institutions sug- gests that psychology is a singular entity at some level.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 1 • What Is Psychology?
    You were, perhaps, asked by your teacher in the first class why you opted for psychology over other subjects. What do you hope to learn? If you were asked this question, what was your response? Generally, the range of responses which surface in class to this question are truly bewildering. Most students give inane responses, like they want to know what others are thinking. But then one also comes across such responses as knowing oneself, knowing others or more specific responses like knowing why people dream, why people go out of their way to help others or beat each other up. All ancient traditions have engaged themselves with questions about human nature. The Indian philosophical traditions, in particular, deal with questions relating to why people behave in the manner in which they do. Why are people generally unhappy? What changes should they bring about in themselves if they desire happiness in their lives? Like all knowledge, psychological knowledge too is intended to contribute to human well-being. If the world is full of misery, it is largely due to humans themselves. Perhaps, you have asked why a 9/11 or war in Iraq happened. Why innocent people in Delhi, Mumbai, Srinagar or in the North- East have to face bombs and bullets? Psychologists ask what is in the experiences of young men which turn them into terrorists seeking revenge. But there is another side to human nature. You may have heard the name of Major HPS Ahluwalia, paralysed waist down because of an injury he suffered in a war with Pakistan, who climbed the Mt.
    [Show full text]
  • Methodological Behaviorism As a Radical Behaviorist Views It
    Behavior and Philosophy, 39/40, 145-202 (2011/2012). © 2011 Cambridge Center for Behavioral Studies METHODOLOGICAL BEHAVIORISM AS A RADICAL BEHAVIORIST VIEWS IT J. Moore University of Wisconsin – Milwaukee ABSTRACT: Methodological behaviorism is a stance on verbal processes and the meaning of “psychological” terms and concepts that are deployed in theories and explanations of behavior. According to this stance, all such terms and concepts should be based on observable stimuli and behavior. Over the years, psychologists have interpreted the phrase “based on” in at least three different ways. One interpretation was that psychologists should remain formally silent on causal mental terms, and not speak at all about unobservables. A second interpretation allowed psychologists to appeal indirectly to mediating mental terms, provided the psychologists could logically connect the terms to observables through operational definitions, where those definitions were exhaustive. A third interpretation again allowed psychologists to appeal indirectly to mediating mental terms, provided the psychologists could logically connect the terms to observables through operational definitions. This time, however, the definitions need be only partial instead of exhaustive. We argue the interpretations lead to an incomplete psychology, if not also an institutionalized mentalism, because they fail to recognize private behavioral events. None of the interpretations are consistent with the radical behaviorism of B. F. Skinner. Key words: methodological behaviorism, radical behaviorism, operationism, logical positivism, theoretical terms, psychological terms Methodological Behaviorism as a Radical Behaviorist Views It Skinner (1964) opened one of his canonical articles with the following paragraph: Behaviorism, with an accent on the last syllable, is not the scientific study of behavior but a philosophy of science concerned with the subject matter and methods of psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Psycholinguistics
    To appear in In K. Malmkjaer (Ed.) The Linguistics Encyclopedia. Second Edition. London, Routledge. Psycholinguistics John N. Williams University of Cambridge, UK Psycholinguistics is a discipline in which the insights of linguistics and psychology are brought to bear on the study of the cognitive aspects of language understanding and production. One of the earliest psychological accounts of language was Wundt's Die Sprache (1900), which is essentially a psychological interpretation of the linguistic work of the Junggrammatiker (see HISTORICAL LINGUISTICS, p.194). However, the strongly empiricist and anti-mentalist attitude to science which dominated both linguistics and psychology during the first half of the twentieth century (see BEHAVIOURIST LINGUISTICS) inhibited theorizing about mental processes involved in linguistic behaviour, and it was not until the late 1950s and early 1960s that the work of Noam Chomsky (see RATIONALIST LINGUISTICS and TRANSFORMATIONAL-GENERATIVE GRAMMAR) provided a climate of thought in which the discipline could flourish. The main impetus for psycholinguistic research in the 1960s was the wish to explore the psychological reality of grammars produced by linguists, that is, to try to show that these in some way mirrored what went on in speakers' and hearers' minds. The two most famous contro- versies within this framework were produced by the derivational theory of complexity (DTC), according to which a sentence would be more difficult to process the further removed its surface structure was from its deep structure, and the theory of the autonomy of syntactic processing, according to which the syntactic analysis of sentences constitutes an independent stage in their perception. There is now general agreement that DTC is false (Garnham, 1985, pp.71-74) and the grammars which produced it have, in any case, been superseded (see TRANSFORMATIONAL-GENERATIVE GRAMMAR).
    [Show full text]
  • Meaning” in Psychology a Lay Theories Approach to Self-Regulation, Social Perception, and Social Development
    Finding “Meaning” in Psychology A Lay Theories Approach to Self-Regulation, Social Perception, and Social Development Daniel C. Molden Northwestern University Carol S. Dweck Stanford University Much of psychology focuses on universal principles of sal principles with a careful consideration of personal thought and action. Although an extremely productive pur- meaning. Consider, for example, recent work in cultural suit, this approach, by describing only the “average per- psychology. An increasing number of findings are showing son,” risks describing no one in particular. This article that many psychological phenomena common in Western discusses an alternate approach that complements interests cultures either do not hold or operate differently in other in universal principles with analyses of the unique psycho- cultures (e.g., Heine et al., 2001; Markus & Kitayama, logical meaning that individuals find in their experiences 1991; Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan, 2001). Yet and interactions. Rooted in research on social cognition, rather than conclude that the principles typically thought to this approach examines how people’s lay theories about be behind these phenomena are flawed, researchers in the stability or malleability of human attributes alter the cultural psychology have focused on unraveling how and meaning they give to basic psychological processes such as why such principles are altered by the personal meaning self-regulation and social perception. Following a review that members of distinct cultures bring to their experiences of research on this lay theories perspective in the field of (see Gardner, Gabriel, & Lee, 1999; Hong & Chiu, 2001; social psychology, the implications of analyzing psycho- Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martinez, 2000; Medin & logical meaning for other fields such as developmental, Atran, 2004).
    [Show full text]
  • Stages of Psychometric Measure Development: the Example of the Generalized Expertise Measure (GEM)
    893 Stages of Psychometric Measure Development: The example of the Generalized Expertise Measure (GEM) Marie-Line Germain Barry University & City College This paper chronicles the steps, methods, and presents hypothetical results of quantitative and qualitative studies being conducted to develop a Generalized Expertise Measure (GEM). Per Hinkin (1995), the stages of scale development are domain and item generation, content expert validation, and pilot test. Content/face validity and internal consistency of the scores of the GEM are discussed, as well as directions to ensure that the psychometric properties of the scale are theoretically and empirically sound. Keywords: Measure, Research Methods, Expertise Since its humble beginnings in the late 1950s, expertise has slowly permeated the human resource development literature. This seemingly obscure concept became an intriguing research topic as artificial intelligence and cognitive psychology further developed in the mid- to late sixties. Swanson & Holton (2001) define expertise as a “displayed behavior within a specialized domain and / or related domain in the form of consistently demonstrated actions of an individual that are both optimally efficient in their execution and effective in their results.” (p. 241). The absence of empirical evidence may well be the main reason of the gradual development of understanding of expertise in the last three decades (cf. Bédard & Chi, 1992). The meticulous standards of scientific methodologies invalidate the pervasive existence of anecdotal evidence in the popular press of expertise in organizational settings. The past 15 years, however, have seen an upsurge in the pace of expertise research, as indicated in the growing number of peer-reviewed publications in the area (Holton & Swanson, 2001).
    [Show full text]