Tone-Driven Vowel Epenthesis in Wamey

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Tone-Driven Vowel Epenthesis in Wamey Tone-driven Vowel Epenthesis in Wamey Jack Merrill & Nik Rolle 5/12/20 Wamey tone-driven ǝ-epenthesis We argue (following Santos 1996) that in Wamey, [ǝ] is inserted to host a floating H tone after a linked L tone CVC root Tone pattern Surface -guv H -gúv ‘braid’ -guk L -gùk ‘be soft’ -gox HL -gôx ‘stay away’ -gwaf LH -gwàfǝ́ ‘sell’ 2 Tone-driven vowel epenthesis Intonation: pragmatic/discourse-based use of pitch Tone: grammatical/lexical use of pitch • Both analyzed with H, L, etc. • Both analytical traditions make frequent use of floating tones o I.e. a tone not underlyingly linked to a Tone-Bearing Unit (TBU) • In a number of languages, intonational floating tones are described as conditioning the insertion of a vowel to serve as its host o Roettger (2017), Roettger & Grice (2019); case studies in Sardinian (Torres-Tamarit et al. 2015, 2017), Galician (Martínez-Gil 1997), Moroccan Arabic (Dell & Elmedlaoui 2002), and Tunisian Arabic (Hellmuth in press), Bari Italian (Grice et al. 2015, 2018) 3 Intonational vowel epenthesis Example: Tashlhiyt Berber (Roettger 2017: 98) • In /tfsχt/ ‘you cancelled,’ there is no TBU to host an intonational H • Possible responses include epenthesis of [ǝ] to host the H: • Note that these cases seem to always involve optionality 4 Tone-driven vowel epenthesis However, in the realm of (grammatical/lexical) tone, it has been claimed that a floating tone does not condition the insertion of a TBU • de Lacy 2003, Blumenfeld 2006, Gleim 2019 The existing claim is that segmental epenthesis is always motivated by the segmental context We argue that Wamey is a strong counterexample to this claim • Wamey exhibits a phenomenon of ǝ-epenthesis that is entirely motivated by tone 5 The Wamey language Wamey/Konyagi (endonym wæ-mèỹ): • ~18,000 speakers in Senegal and Guinea • Part of the Tenda subgroup within Niger-Congo Bassari Tenda Bedik Wamey Documentation: • Some very limited documentation before Santos • Santos (1996): grammar + ~4000 entry lexicon 6 Fula Sereer Wolof Wamey Bassari Bedik Pajade Biafada Kobiana (S), Kasanga (N) Bainunk languages Cangin languages Nalu, Mbulungish Joola languages Manjak, Mankanya, Pepel Balanta Bijogo Southern Atlantic 7 Wamey consonant inventory labial coronal palatal velar labiovel. vl. stop p t c k kw (vl.) cont. f r s x xw vd. stop b d j g gw w l vd. cont. y v ry implosive ɓ ɗ ƴ nasal m n ñ ŋ ŋw nas. cont. w̃ l̰ ỹ vl. prenas. mp nt nc nk nkw vd. prenas. mb nd nj 8 Wamey vowel inventory high i u mid e ǝ o low æ a • No long vowels (assimilated V-V possible across morpheme boundary) • All vowels can appear in a root, including /ǝ/ e.g. æ̀ -mpǝ̀nk ‘wooden sword,’ lǝ̀n ‘snake’ But there are restrictions on /ǝ/, e.g.: • Cannot appear word-initially • No /Cǝ/ roots. 9 Wamey tone • Two underlying tones: H, L • No tone-bearing consonants • Surface falling (HL) and rising (LH) contours are found on a single TBU, but subject to restrictions— notably, only on stem-final syllable o Note: rising (LH) tone most often realized as a mid tone • All four surface tones are possible on a final vowel (other than ǝ, æ) wæ̀ -ɓùyá ‘puffy pants’ wæ̀ -ɓúmbà ‘earthen roundhouses’ wæ̀ -ɓùñâ ‘gifts’ wæ̀ -cémǎ ‘molars’ 10 Wamey tone • Furthermore, a LH contour must be preceded by a H tone — no L.LH o No similar restriction on HL L.HL kòmbô ‘sorghum’ H.HL ì-támpûl ‘navel’ H.LH æ̀ -ŋómpě ‘falcon’ *L.LH 11 *CC-driven ǝ-epenthesis in Wamey • CC clusters are not attested tautomorphemically • When they arise across a morpheme boundary, 3 possible responses: a) Do nothing — used in e.g. reduplication, compounding væ̀ -njæ̀ g-ƴæ̀ g ‘jaw joints,’ æ̀ -nkə́l̰-fǽc ‘first rooster crow’ b) Delete a consonant /-mǝ́t-ryǽry/ ‘make war + ASSOC’ → -mǝ́tǽry ‘fight’ c) Insert ǝ /-ƴív-má/ ‘be well + DURATIVE’ → -ƴívǝ́má ‘be always well’ 12 *CC-driven ǝ-epenthesis Whether C-deletion or ǝ-epenthesis is employed depends on the identity of the morphemes, as well as grammatical factors (e.g. negation) • For example, with “adverbial” verbal suffixes, ǝ is inserted, but with more ‘core’ verbal suffixes (mostly valence-changing), the suffix-initial C is deleted Our analysis of *CC resolution differs quite a bit from Santos’ analysis, which notably requires a few suffixes to be ǝ-final underlyingly Lots more to say about this *CC ǝ-epenthesis phenomenon, but as it’s completely separate from tone-driven ǝ-epenthesis, we’ll leave it for now 13 Tone patterns on Wamey roots On surface CVC roots: L ì-cæ̀ w̃ ‘hiding’ H ì-cǽw̃ ‘urinating’ HL ì-cæ̂ w̃ ‘domestic animal’ *LH *ì-cæ̌ w̃ 14 Tone patterns on Wamey roots 4 common surface patterns: • 1 - monosyllabic roots CVC • 2- roots ending in schwa CVCə • 3- bisyllabic roots ending in a vowel other than schwa CVCV • 4- those ending in a consonant CVCVC (with any vowel in either position) 1 - cvc 2 - cvcə 3 - cvcv 4 - cvcvc cv́c cv̂c *cv́cə́ *cv́cə̀ cv́cv́ cv́cv̀ cv́cv́c cv́cv̀c cv̀c *cv̌c *cv̀cə̀ cv̀cə́ cv̀cv̀ cv̀cv́ cv̀cv̀c cv̀cv́c 15 Tone patterns on Wamey roots • Roots of the shape [cv̀cə́] are very common [æ̀ -mbə̀ỹə́] ‘leper’ [à-l̰ə̀nkwə́] ‘imbecile’ [dòlə́] ‘today’ [ì-còkə́] ‘to weld’ • Types 3 CVCV and 4 CVCVC (with vowels other than final [ə]) demonstrate that there is no general ban on all H- or all L-toned words, nor a ban on transitions from H to L 1 - cvc 2 - cvcə 3 - cvcv 4 - cvcvc cv́c cv̂c *cv́cə́ *cv́cə̀ cv́cv́ cv́cv̀ cv́cv́c cv́cv̀c cv̀c *cv̌c *cv̀cə̀ cv̀cə́ cv̀cv̀ cv̀cv́ cv̀cv̀c cv̀cv́c 16 Tone patterns on Wamey roots • Just as cvc and CVCə roots are in complementary tonal distribution, CVCVC and CVCVCə roots are as well • Several mono-morphemic nominal roots of the shape [cv́cv̀cə́] exist [ì-pófòlə́] ‘ankle’ [ì-pǽl̰æ̀ cə́] ‘horse’ [ì-búl̰ùnə́] ‘wasp’ [ryénkànə́] ‘antelope species’ 5 - cvcv 6 - cvcvc 7 - cvcvcə cv́cv̂ cv́cv̌ cv́cv̂c (?cv́cv̌c) *cv́cv́cə́ *cv́cv̀cə̀ *cv́cv́cə̀ cv́cv̀cə́ cv̀cv̂ *cv̀cv̌ cv̀cv̂c *cv̀cv̌c *cv̀cv́cə́ *cv̀cv̀cə̀ *cv̀cv́cə̀ cv̀cv̀cə́ 17 Type frequency of tone patterns on roots/stems CVC roots (n=1625) CVCV stems (n=589) CVCVC stems (n=724) H cv́c 610 H(H) cv́cv́ 209 H(H) cv́cv́c 231 L cv̀c 795 L(L) cv̀cv̀ 27 L(L) cv̀cv̀c 83 HL cv̂c 47 HL cv́cv̀ 38 HL cv́cv̀c 18 LH cv̀cǝ́ 173 LH cv̀cv́ 293 LH cv̀cv́c 233 HHL cv́cv̂ 0 HHL cv́cv̂c 25 LHL cv̀cv̂ 16 LHL cv̀cv̂c 81 HLH cv́cv̌ 6 HLH cv́cv̀cǝ́ 18 LLH cv̀cv̌ 0 LLH cv̀cv̀cǝ́ 35 Most disyllabic stems contain a suffix (e.g. -á ‘anticausative’), so the counts are skewed in favor of certain patterns 18 Tone patterns on Wamey roots Derivations: i. H H ii. L L iii. H L H L iv. L H L H → | → | → \/ → | | cvc cv́c cvc cv̀c cvc cv̂c cvc cv̀cə́ 19 Tone patterns on Wamey roots • All consonants allowed in coda position (at all prosodic constituent levels, e.g. p-word, p-phrase, i-phrase) • ǝ-epenthesis in these roots cannot be attributed to any segmental factor […v̀cə́] […v̀c] […v̀cv́c] […v́c] […v̂c] i. [ì-pófòlə́] [ì-ɓòl] [ì-tàmból] [xóról] [l̰ə̀m və́pôl] ‘ankle’ ‘to refuse’ ‘to climb to the top’ ‘whistling viper’ ‘paternal aunt’ (mère du pénis) ii. [ì-pǽl̰æ̀ cə́] [ì-kæ̀ c] [ì-ŋàbə́c] [ì-mǽc] [kùmpôc] ‘horse’ ‘skin’ ‘to take a big share’ ‘to flood’ ‘pumpkin and peanut dish’ 20 Definite determiners Further evidence that [cv̀cǝ́] roots are underlyingly C-final: • The enclitic definite determiner =ŋǎ~ỹǎ~w̃ ǎ (form dependent on noun class) has a “short variant” =ǎ after C-final roots. /æ-tǝ́x=ŋǎ/ → æ̀ -tǝ́xǎ ‘the tree’ vs. /i-ɓú=ŋǎ/ → ì-ɓúŋǎ ‘the baobab fruit’ /i-líyá=ỹǎ/ → ì-líyáỹǎ ‘the tool’ • This “short variant” is used after [cv̀cǝ́] roots— interestingly with a surface long vowel after the root-final C Ⓗ /æ-mbǝ̀l =ŋǎ/ → æ̀ -mbǝ̀láǎ ‘the milk’ Ⓗ /i-bìn =ŋǎ/ → ì-bìnáǎ ‘the ant’ 21 [ə]-alternating suffixes in Wamey • Thus far, the simpler analysis is one which posits less structure rather than more, i.e. underlyingly CVC • However, roots show no active alternations which could provide direct evidence to the learner for V-epenthesis or V-retention • Fortunately, we can turn to affixal morphemes for evidence, which do show alternations between [C]-final and [Cə]-final variants • These pattern underlyingly as [C]-final based on how they condition following suffixes, and therefore support V-epenthesis 22 [ə]-alternating suffixes in Wamey 3SG.PFTV [-k]~[-kə́] ì-cǽs ‘to suffer’ cǽsə́-k ‘he suffers’ (il souffre - [S96:43]) ì-tòk ‘to eat’ tókə̀-kə́ ‘he ate’ (il a mangé – [S96:43]) 2PL.IMP [-n]~[-nə́] ƴə̀ɗ-ə́ry-æ̀ kə́-n ‘give (it)!’ (donnez donc! - [S96:554]) tòk-ə́ryì-nə́ ‘eat!’ (mangez! - [S96:220]) ADJ1 [-ǽx]~[-æ̀ xə́] ì-ƴǽk ‘be hot’ wə̀-nkà wæ̀ -ƴǽk-ǽx ‘hot water’ (de l'eau chaude - [S96:183]) ì-gùk ‘be sliced’ wæ̀ -wə́d wæ̀ -wùk-æ̀ xə́ ‘sl. mangos’ (des mangues talées - [S96:107]) ADJ2 [-ák]~[-àkə́] (after stem final [a]) ì-dáñá ‘to sit down’ dáñá-ák ‘seated’ (assis - [S96:200]) ì-ɓávà ‘to (still) be green’ ɓávà-àkə́ ‘still green’ (encore vert - [S96:115]) 23 [ə]-alternating suffixes in Wamey …C# ~ …Cə# Gloss S96 term i. [-k] [-kə́] ≈3SG.PERFECTIVE indice personnel minimal (ps. min.) [-ɗ] [-ɗə́] IMPERFECTIVE1 imperfectif [-nd] [-ndə́] IMPERFECTIVE2 imperfectif à l’énonciatif antérieur, l’impératif, i.a. [-n] [-nə́] 2PL.IMPERATIVE impératif (deuxième personne du pluriel) ii. [-ǽx] [-æ̀ xə́] ADJECTIVAL1 adjective [-ák] [-àkə́] ADJECTIVAL2 adjective [-ǽk] [-æ̀ kə́] ANTERIOR modalité verbale d’antériorité [-(ry)ǽry] [-(ry)æ̀ ryə́] ASSOCIATIVE associative [-(x)ǽl̰] [-(x)æ̀ l̰ə́] LOCATIVE locative 24 [ə]-alternating suffixes in Wamey Ⓗ Root /-k / CVC H /də́c/ ‘fart’ [lə́cə́-k] Ⓗ LH /ƴòmp / ‘slack (not taut)’ [yòmpə́-k] L /tòk/ ‘eat’ [tòkə̀-kə́] HL /yôj/ ‘bring’ [yójə̀-kə́] CVC+ H /ƴáry-í/ ‘baptize-PASS’ [yáryí-k] LH /tòk-í/
Recommended publications
  • T3 Sandhi Rules of Different Prosodic Hierarchies
    T3 Sandhi rules of different prosodic hierarchies Abstract This study conducts acoustic analysis on T3 sandhi of two characters across different boundaries of prosodic hierarchies. The experimental data partly support Chen (2000)’s view about sandhi domain that “T3 sandhi takes place obligatorily within MRU”, and “sandhi rule can not apply cross intonation phrase boundary”. Nonetheless, the results do not support his claim that “there are no intermediate sandhi hierarchies between MRU and intonation phrase”. On the contrary, it is found that, although T3 sandhi could occur across all kinds of hierarchical boundaries between MRU and intonation phrase, T3 sandhi rules within a foot (prosodic word), or between foots without pause, or between pauses without intonation (phonology phrase) are very different in terms of acoustic properties. Furthermore, based on the facts that (1) T3 sandhi could occur cross boundary of pauses and (2) phrase-final sandhi could be significantly lengthening, it is argued that T3 sandhi is due to dissimilation of low tones, rather than duration reduction within MRU domain. It is also demonstrated that tone sandhi and prosodic hierarchies may not be equivalently evaluated in Chinese phonology. Prosodic hierarchies are determined by pause and lengthening, not by tone sandhi. Key words: tone sandhi; prosodic hierarchies; boundary; Mandarin Chinese 1 Introduction So-called T3 sandhi in mandarin Chinese has been widely discussed in Chinese Phonology. Mandarin Chinese has four tones: T1 (level 55), T2 (rising 35), T3 (low-rise 214), T4 (falling 51).Generally, when a T3 is followed by another T3, it turns into T2. The rule could be simply stated as: 214->35/ ___214.
    [Show full text]
  • The Cambridge Handbook of Phonology
    This page intentionally left blank The Cambridge Handbook of Phonology Phonology – the study of how the sounds of speech are represented in our minds – is one of the core areas of linguistic theory, and is central to the study of human language. This state-of-the-art handbook brings together the world’s leading experts in phonology to present the most comprehensive and detailed overview of the field to date. Focusing on the most recent research and the most influential theories, the authors discuss each of the central issues in phonological theory, explore a variety of empirical phenomena, and show how phonology interacts with other aspects of language such as syntax, morph- ology, phonetics, and language acquisition. Providing a one-stop guide to every aspect of this important field, The Cambridge Handbook of Phonology will serve as an invaluable source of readings for advanced undergraduate and graduate students, an informative overview for linguists, and a useful starting point for anyone beginning phonological research. PAUL DE LACY is Assistant Professor in the Department of Linguistics, Rutgers University. His publications include Markedness: Reduction and Preservation in Phonology (Cambridge University Press, 2006). The Cambridge Handbook of Phonology Edited by Paul de Lacy CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town, Singapore, São Paulo Cambridge University Press The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 8RU, UK Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York www.cambridge.org Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9780521848794 © Cambridge University Press 2007 This publication is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provision of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press.
    [Show full text]
  • 5 Phonology Florian Lionnet and Larry M
    5 Phonology Florian Lionnet and Larry M. Hyman 5.1. Introduction The historical relation between African and general phonology has been a mutu- ally beneficial one: the languages of the African continent provide some of the most interesting and, at times, unusual phonological phenomena, which have con- tributed to the development of phonology in quite central ways. This has been made possible by the careful descriptive work that has been done on African lan- guages, by linguists and non-linguists, and by Africanists and non-Africanists who have peeked in from time to time. Except for the click consonants of the Khoisan languages (which spill over onto some neighboring Bantu languages that have “borrowed” them), the phonological phenomena found in African languages are usually duplicated elsewhere on the globe, though not always in as concen- trated a fashion. The vast majority of African languages are tonal, and many also have vowel harmony (especially vowel height harmony and advanced tongue root [ATR] harmony). Not surprisingly, then, African languages have figured dispro- portionately in theoretical treatments of these two phenomena. On the other hand, if there is a phonological property where African languages are underrepresented, it would have to be stress systems – which rarely, if ever, achieve the complexity found in other (mostly non-tonal) languages. However, it should be noted that the languages of Africa have contributed significantly to virtually every other aspect of general phonology, and that the various developments of phonological theory have in turn often greatly contributed to a better understanding of the phonologies of African languages. Given the considerable diversity of the properties found in different parts of the continent, as well as in different genetic groups or areas, it will not be possible to provide a complete account of the phonological phenomena typically found in African languages, overviews of which are available in such works as Creissels (1994) and Clements (2000).
    [Show full text]
  • ELISION in ESAHIE Victoria Owusu Ansah Abstract One of the Syllable Structure Changes That Occur in Rapid Speech Because of Sounds Influencing Each Other Is Elision
    Ghana Journal of Linguistics 9.2: 22-43 (2020) ________________________________________________________________________ http://dx.doi.org/10.4314/gjl.v9i2.2 ELISION IN ESAHIE Victoria Owusu Ansah Abstract One of the syllable structure changes that occur in rapid speech because of sounds influencing each other is elision. This paper provides an account of elision in Esahie, also known as Sehwi, a Kwa language spoken in the Western North region of Ghana. The paper discusses the processes involved in elision, and the context within which elision occurs in the language. The paper shows that sound segments, syllables and tones are affected by the elision process. It demonstrates that elision, though purely a phonological process, is influenced by morphological factors such as vowel juxtapositioning during compounding, and at word boundary. The evidence in this paper show that there is an interface between phonology and morphology when accounting for elision in Esahie. Data for this study were gathered from primary sources using ethnographic and stimuli methods. Keywords: Elision, Esahie, Sehwi, Tone, Deletion, Phonology 1.0 Introduction This paper provides an account of elision in Esahie, a Kwa language spoken in the Western North Region of Ghana1. It discusses the processes involved in elision in Esahie, and the context within which elision occurs in the language. The paper demonstrates that elision is employed in Esahie as a syllable structure repair mechanism. Elision is purely a phonological process but can sometimes be triggered by morphological factors. Indeed, the works of (Abakah 2004a, 2004b), Abdul-Rahman (2013), Abukari (2018), Becker and Gouskova (2016) writing on Akan, Dagbani and Russia respectively, confirm that elision 1 Speakers of Esahie in Ghana number about 580,000 and they live mostly in the Western North Region of the country (Ghana Statistical Service Report 2012, 2010 National Population Census).
    [Show full text]
  • How to Study a Tone Language, with Exemplification from Oku (Grassfields Bantu, Cameroon)
    UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2010) How To Study a Tone Language, with exemplification from Oku (Grassfields Bantu, Cameroon) Larry M. Hyman University of California, Berkeley DRAFT • August 2010 • Comments Welcome 1. Introduction On numerous occasions I have been asked, “How does one study a tone language?” Or: “How can I tell if my language is tonal?” Even seasoned field researchers, upon confronting their first tone system, have asked me: “How do I figure out the number of tones I have?” When it comes to tone, colleagues and students alike often forget everything they’ve learned about discovering the phonology of a language and assume that tone is somehow different, that it requires different techniques or expertise. Some of this may derive from an incomplete understanding of what it means to be a tone system. Prior knowledge of possible tonal inventories, tone rules, and tone- grammar interfaces would definitely be helpful to a field researcher who has to decipher a tone system. However, despite such recurrent encounters, general works on tone seem not to answer these questions—specifically, they rarely tell you how to start and how to discover. I sometimes respond to the last question, “How do I figure out the number of tones I have?”, by asking in return, “Well, how would you figure out the number of vowels you have?” Hopefully the answer would be something like: “I would get a word list, starting with nouns, listen carefully, transcribe as much detail as I can, and then organize the materials to see if I have been consistent.” (I will put off until §5 commentary concerning the use of speech software as an aid in linguistic discovery.) Although I don’t think the elicitation techniques that one applies in studying segmental vs.
    [Show full text]
  • Multiple Feature Affixation in Seenku Plural Formation
    Pre-publication version, please use caution in citing, to appear in Morphology (journal) Multiple feature affixation in Seenku plural formation Laura McPherson (Dartmouth College) Abstract Nominal plural formation in Seenku involves two surface changes: fronting of the final vowel and raising of the final tone. Diachronically, the plural patterns likely derive from a suffix *-˝ı, which has been obscured through the loss of falling sonority diphthongs. By comparing plural formation to other morphophonological processes in Seenku, this paper argues that the plural suffix has been restructured as a featural suffix consisting of two features: the vocalic feature [+front], resulting in vowel fronting, and the tonal feature [+raised], resulting in tone raising. Given the atomic nature of the morphosyntactic feature plural, Seenku plural formation represents a strong case of multiple feature affixation, albeit a case that can be ac- counted for through Max constraints on feature values (Lombardi 1998, 2001, etc.) and Realize-Morpheme (van Oostendorp 2005, Trommer 2012) rather than the constraint Max-Flt argued for by Wolf (2007). A level-ordered approach retaining the vocalic suffix is also considered but is shown to suffer from a number of short- comings, particularly with respect to tone and a challenging class of nasal stems. In short, this paper explores how the synchronic grammar copes with the vestiges of affixal morphology in a language that has undergone heavy reduction. 1 Introduction Seenku (ISO 639-3 [sos], exonym Sambla or Sembla) is a Northwestern Mande language of Burkina Faso with very little apparent affixation. The majority of morphosyntactic meanings are conveyed either analytically or through what appear to be patterns of base modification.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonology of Tone and Intonation
    This page intentionally left blank The Phonology of Tone and Intonation Tone and Intonation are two types of pitch variation, which are used by speak- ers of many languages in order to give shape to utterances. More specifically, tone encodes morphemes, and intonation gives utterances a further discoursal meaning that is independent of the meanings of the words themselves. In this comprehensive survey, Carlos Gussenhoven provides an up-to-date overview of research into tone and intonation, discussing why speakers vary their pitch, what pitch variations mean, and how they are integrated into our grammars. He also explains why intonation in part appears to be universally understood, while at other times it is language-specific and can lead to misunderstandings. The first eight chapters concern general topics: phonetic aspects of pitch mod- ulation; typological notions (stress, accent, tone, and intonation); the distinction between phonetic implementation and phonological representation; the paralin- guistic meaning of pitch variation; the phonology and phonetics of downtrends; developments from the Pierrehumbert–Beckman model; and tone and intona- tion in Optimality Theory. In chapters 9–15, the book’s central arguments are illustrated with comprehensive phonological descriptions – partly in OT – of the tonal and intonational systems of six languages, including Japanese, French, and English. Accompanying sound files can be found on the author’s web site: http://www.let.kun.nl/pti Carlos Gussenhoven is Professor and Chair of General and Experimental Phonology at the University of Nijmegen. He has previously published On the Grammar and Semantics of Sentence Accents (1994), English Pronunciation for Student Teachers (co-authored with A.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Duration and Sonority on Contour Tone Distribution— Typological Survey and Formal Analysis
    The Effects of Duration and Sonority on Contour Tone Distribution— Typological Survey and Formal Analysis Jie Zhang For my family Table of Contents Acknowledgments xi 1 Background 3 1.1 Two Examples of Contour Tone Distribution 3 1.1.1 Contour Tones on Long Vowels Only 3 1.1.2 Contour Tones on Stressed Syllables Only 8 1.2 Questions Raised by the Examples 9 1.3 How This Work Evaluates The Different Predictions 11 1.3.1 A Survey of Contour Tone Distribution 11 1.3.2 Instrumental Case Studies 11 1.4 Putting Contour Tone Distribution in a Bigger Picture 13 1.4.1 Phonetically-Driven Phonology 13 1.4.2 Positional Prominence 14 1.4.3 Competing Approaches to Positional Prominence 16 1.5 Outline 20 2 The Phonetics of Contour Tones 23 2.1 Overview 23 2.2 The Importance of Sonority for Contour Tone Bearing 23 2.3 The Importance of Duration for Contour Tone Bearing 24 2.4 The Irrelevance of Onsets to Contour Tone Bearing 26 2.5 Local Conclusion 27 3 Empirical Predictions of Different Approaches 29 3.1 Overview 29 3.2 Defining CCONTOUR and Tonal Complexity 29 3.3 Phonological Factors That Influence Duration and Sonority of the Rime 32 3.4 Predictions of Contour Tone Distribution by Different Approaches 34 3.4.1 The Direct Approach 34 3.4.2 Contrast-Specific Positional Markedness 38 3.4.3 General-Purpose Positional Markedness 41 vii viii Table of Contents 3.4.4 The Moraic Approach 42 3.5 Local Conclusion 43 4 The Role of Contrast-Specific Phonetics in Contour Tone Distribution: A Survey 45 4.1 Overview of the Survey 45 4.2 Segmental Composition 48
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the Phonology of Mixteco's
    THE PHONOLOGY OF MIXTECO'S FLOATING HIGH TONES: THEORETICAL IMPLICATIONS BERNARD TRANEL University of California, Irvine <[email protected]> 1. INTRODUCTION This paper provides a cross-theoretical comparison of the treatment of floating high tones in Mixteco, a language of Southern Mexico studied by Kenneth Pike in the 1930's and 40's. Such a case study proves instructive in three major ways. First, it justifies the recent theoretical trend to move away from rule-based phonology to constraint-based phonology. Secondly, it raises interesting theoretical issues within the nascent constraint-based approach. Finally, it brings about new perspectives on our understanding of tonal phonology and the morphology of floating elements. In a nutshell, the facts of relevance in Mixteco are the following: a morpheme-final floating high tone anchors into the next morpheme (if no pause intervenes), but its landing site varies depending on the host morpheme's own tonal and segmental make-up. Thus, the floating tone does not automatically dock onto the first tone-bearing element of its host, as one might expect in such tone sandhi configurations. Instead, it prefers to attach to the first low-tone or high-tone vowel, even if it requires skipping an intervening mid-tone vowel. This preference can however be thwarted in some cases. For example, a medial glottal stop in effect prevents a floating high tone from ignoring an initial mid-tone vowel in favor of a subsequent low-tone or high-tone vowel. Another important characteristic of floating high tones in Mixteco is that they are never phonetically realized within their own lexical morphemes.
    [Show full text]
  • Tonal System in Kumam, a Double Downstep Language1)
    Journal of Asian and African Studies, No., Article Tonal System in Kumam, a Double Downstep Language1) Hieda, Osamu Research Institute for Languages and Cultures of Asia and Africa Kumam, a western Nilotic language, has two types of downstep high tones, a downstep high tone and a double downstep high tone. Double downstep is attested only in a handful of languages. More than two downstep high tones are synchronically attested in still fewer languages. Kumam has synchronically two kinds of downstep high tones, a downstep high and a double downstep high tones. This paper is the first attempt to demonstrate that the western Nilotic language has two types of downstep high tones, a downstep high and a double downstep high tones. is paper also shows that ‘fl oating tone approach’ is applicative to phonological representations of a downstep and a double downstep in Kumam. I also propose the principle that lexical tonemes are preserved within phonological units during derivation. However, more studies are needed to manifest phonological units. Kumam tonal system consists of the general tonal principles and the lan- guage specifi c tone sandhi rules. e general tonal principles are common and well-known in autosegmental theories. They are summarized as follows: 1) Tonemes are assigned to TBUs from le to right. 2) Association lines do not cross each other. 3) High tonemes are preferable for being assigned to TBUs. Kumam has only two language specific tone sandhi rules, High spread and ‘Floating high assignment.’ Tone sandhi occurs beyond boundaries of pho- nological units. I propose the principle that all lexical tonemes are preserved within a phonological unit (word) all through tonal process.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Typology OUP CORRECTED PROOF – FINAL, 24/3/2016, Spi
    OUP CORRECTED PROOF – FINAL, 24/3/2016, SPi Phonological Typology OUP CORRECTED PROOF – FINAL, 24/3/2016, SPi OXFORD SURVEYS IN PHONOLOGY AND PHONETICS GENERAL EDITOR: Bert Vaux, University of Cambridge This series aims to provide a balanced account of leading approaches to and debates in the most active and productive topics in phonology and phonetics. Each volume examines current and past treatments of a specific topic and offers a reasoned account of the theories and methods that lead to the best account for the facts. The books provide students and practitioners of phonology, phonetics, and related fields with a valuable source of instruction and reference, set within the context of wider developments in the field, and where relevant in linguistics and cognitive science more generally. PUBLISHED Phonological Typology Matthew K. Gordon IN PREPARATION The Syllable Juliette Blevins The Phonetics–Phonology Interface Abigail C. Cohn and Marie Huffman Intonation Sónia Frota and Carlos Gussenhoven OUP CORRECTED PROOF – FINAL, 24/3/2016, SPi 3 Great Clarendon Street, Oxford, OXDP, United Kingdom Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide. Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries © Matthew K. Gordon The moral rights of the author have been asserted First Edition published in Impression: All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press, or as expressly permitted by law, by licence or under terms agreed with the appropriate reprographics rights organization.
    [Show full text]
  • 27Mfm Abstracts Booklet
    The Twenty-Seventh Manchester Phonology Meeting ABSTRACTS BOOKLET Thursday 23rd - Saturday 25th May 2019 Held at UMIC, Manchester Organised by a collaboration of phonologists at the University of Edinburgh, the University of Manchester, and elsewhere. This booklet contains the abstracts for all the papers presented at the twenty-seventh Manchester Phonology Meeting, held at Hulme Hall, Manchester, in May 2019. The abstracts are arranged in alphabetical order by the surname of the (first named) presenter. The abstracts for the oral paper sessions are presented first, followed by the abstracts for the poster paper sessions, and the booklet concludes with abstracts for the special session. The final programme, included in your registration pack and available on the conference website, gives the details of which papers are in which room, and at which times. Oral papers Are phonotactically unusual words ‘exceptions’? Adam Albright, MIT ([email protected]) A growing body of work has advanced the hypothesis that grammatical restrictions may be gradient. In the domain of static phonotactic restrictions, this includes OCP place in Arabic (Frisch, Pierrehumbert and Broe 2004), transparent vowels in Hungarian (Hayes and Londe 2006), sonority sequencing in English (Daland et al. 2011), and numerous others. Support generally comes from probabilistic generalization: speakers are more likely accept or produce novel items that conform. Less attention has been paid to the status of existing words that fail to conform. Under the gradient grammatical analysis, these are exceptions. Implementations vary considerably, but most approaches to exceptions rely on listing morpheme-specific information, which blocks or exempts them from the regular grammar.
    [Show full text]