Seeing Black and White

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Seeing Black and White Seeing Black and White Alan Gilchrist OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Seeing Black and White This page intentionally left blank SEEING BLACK AND WHITE Alan Gilchrist 1 2006 1 Oxford University Press, Inc., publishes works that further Oxford University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education. Oxford New York Auckland Cape Town Dar es Salaam Hong Kong Karachi Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi New Delhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto With offices in Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam Copyright ᭧ 2006 by Oxford University Press, Inc. Published by Oxford University Press, Inc. 198 Madison Avenue, New York, New York 10016 www.oup.com Oxford is a registered trademark of Oxford University Press All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior permission of Oxford University Press. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Gilchrist, Alan. Seeing black and white / by Alan Gilchrist. p. cm. (Oxford psychology series; no. 40) Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-19-518716-4 ISBN-13 978-0-19-518716-8 1. Imagery (Psychology). I. Title. II. Series. BF241.L54 2006 153—dc22 2006006283 987654321 Printed in the United States of America on acid-free paper This book is dedicated to the memory of Irvin Rock, my wonderful mentor and warm friend. This page intentionally left blank Foreword And God said let Gilchrist be and all was light. —V. S. Ramachandran I feel honored to be asked to write this foreword for Professor Alan Gilchrist’s book, as I have followed his work on perception with keen interest over the years. The study of perception has a long and venerable history dating back to the great 19th-century German physicist, physiologist, and ophthalmologist Hermann Von Helmholtz, who more than anyone else pointed out that the visual image is inherently ambiguous. A circular image in the retina can be produced by an infinity of oval objects of different widths, each tilted by a certain amount. Black print in sunlight reflects more light than a white page seen in artificial light at night, yet it looks black. The only way the brain could solve such a problem, Helmholtz realized, was by using certain built-in knowledge or “assumptions” about the statistics of the natural world (“built in” can either mean built in by learning or by genes; Helmholtz empha- sized the former, and Hering, the latter). These assumptions are used by the visual system to eliminate an infinity of improbable solutions and home in effortlessly on the correct solution. Since these mecha- nisms are unconscious—on autopitot, as it were—Helmholtz called them “unconscious inferences,” a phrase that has never been im- proved on. The study of perception, then, is the study of these as- sumptions and of their implementation in the neural hardware of the brain. One approach to perception is to study visual illusions, which have the same effect on psychologists as the smell of burning rubber on an engineer: an irresistible urge to find the cause (as Peter Medawar once said of philosophers). Illusions are not mere curiosities; they are clev- erly contrived stimuli that reveal the hidden assumptions that drive viii Foreword perception and allow us to discover underlying rules of operation. This book is full of lovely new examples of illusions of brightness and lightness, many of which were discovered by the author. In the last century the experimental study of perception went through three distinct stages. First, in the early 20th century the Ge- stalt psychologists (including Wertheimer, Koffka, Ko¨hler, and Anstis) used astonishingly simple displays to uncover what they called the “laws” of perception, reminding us that great science is driven mainly by ingenuity and experimental cunning rather than fancy equipment. Unfortunately, some of them—although not the best among them— had the habit of calling every observation a “law,” with the air of having actually explained something. (Such explanations, as Peter Medawar might have said, are “mere analgesics that dull the ache of incomprehension without removing the cause.”) It is to Gilchrist’s credit that he doesn’t fall into this trap. But at least the Gestaltists discovered and studied perceptually compelling phenomena; what they did was interesting. The same can- not be said of the next big movement, called (ironically) “classical psychophysics,” which was championed by Stevens at Harvard. This movement, which completely eclipsed Gestalt psychology, was partly the result of the pernicious effects of behaviorism and a general sus- picion of “introspective” psychology. It became unfashionable, for sev- eral decades, to ask the subject what he or she was actually seeing in a visual display. Which is ironic, given that that’s why most of us study perception in the first place! The emphasis was placed, instead, on obtaining detailed quantitative measurements of small second- order effects, in order to plot what are pompously referred to as “psy- chometric functions” (i.e., graphs). A time will come when this whole movement will be seen as a curious anomaly in the history of psy- chology, a manifestation of “physics envy” and the accompanying be- lief that the mere act of measuring something makes it scientific (what I call “researchmanship” rather than research). When I was a graduate student in Cambridge, in the late seventies, the use of any perceptual stimuli, other than blurred stripes, was con- sidered taboo. (To be sure, sine wave gratings were useful stimuli for providing more complete and accurate descriptions of visual perfor- mance than two-point discrimination, resulting in the use of modulation-transfer functions. But it’s fair to say people got a bit car- ried away.) Giles Brindley told us there are two types of experiments: Class A (obtaining “psychometric functions”) and Class B (simply observing carefully what you see—for example, the work of Bela Ju- lesz or Edwin Land). He warned us not to trust Class B “because they are no better than dreams.” (It’s a good thing he wasn’t around when Newton passed white light through a prism, Galileo saw the moons of Jupiter, or Faraday moved a magnet to and fro within a coil. He might have tapped them on the shoulder and said, “This isn’t science Foreword ix until you have obtained a graph,” thereby aborting the birth of phys- ics.) And as for biology, David Hubel once reminded me that the most important book ever published—Darwin’s Origin of Species—doesn’t have a single graph in it! This desire to “ape” physics is especially ironic given that physics itself had to initially go through a qualitative “Faraday” stage before it reached a mature quantitative “Maxwell” stage. Psychology has to— and will—pass through the same stages, and there is simply no point in trying to jump ahead. Fortunately, this obsession with blurred stripes turned out to be a temporary aberration. In the last three decades there has been a tre- mendous resurgence of interest in those phenomena that drew us all to the study of perception in the first place. This was spearheaded by Irvin Rock, Richard Gregory, J. J. Gibson, George Sperling, Bela Julesz, Julie Hochberg, Gaetano Kanizsa, and Donald MacKay in the gener- ation previous to mine. Although their work was initially ignored by the “classical psychophysics” types, (remember Kenneth Ogle’s dis- missal of Julesz and Leo Hurvich’s contempt for Edwin Land?), it has now become part of “mainstream” research in perception. The study of perception became interesting once again. When I tell younger col- leagues about the Ogle/Julesz debate, the usual reaction I get is, “Who’s Ogle?” Exactly my point. Following this, there was a sort of “neo-gestalt” revolution—a movement that owes its existence largely to a handful of contempo- rary researchers: Ken Nakayama, Pat Cavanagh, Randy Blake, Chris Tyler, Ted Adelson, Lothar Spillman, and, now, Alan Gilchrist, Dale Purves, and Pawan Sinha. Thanks to all these researchers (and many whose names I’ve left out) there has been a renaissance of interest in such phenomena as illusory contours, occlusion, shape-from-shading, binocular rivalry, apparent motion, and perceptual “constancies”—in- trinsically fascinating phenomena that were eclipsed by “Class A” psychophysics for nearly three decades. (But to be fair to Brindley, Class A research has had its victories, especially in the study of color vision. The discovery of the detailed laws of Trichromacy is one of the great triumphs of visual science, although—as David Hubel noted— its practitioners study it with a passion that seems grossly out of pro- portion to its evolutionary importance.) But it’s the revival of interest in what used to be called “illusions” that has fired the imagination of physiologists (no mean feat!) and AI researchers. If you look at the big picture, these last two decades have been heady times for perceptual psychologists. The neo-gestalt revolution is here! One of the foremost among these revolutionaries is the author of this book. What I especially like about his work is that, unlike many of us who jump around from topic to topic, he has devoted his whole life to the single-minded pursuit of the laws that govern our percep- x Foreword tion of lightness/brightness. Anyone acquainted with Gilchrist knows that he doesn’t merely study lightness; he lives it. (I remember driving around Manhattan with him. Any advertisement or newly painted or oddly illuminated wall becomes an “experiment” for him as he jumps out of the car excitedly, oblivious to the traffic; high on science, high on life, and just high!) The result of his lifelong obsession is this book, the most comprehensive and thorough monograph on this topic that has ever been published, a tome that Helmholtz and Hering would have taken great delight reading in their bathrooms.
Recommended publications
  • Eye-Movement Studies of Visual Face Perception
    Eye-movement studies of visual face perception Joseph Arizpe A dissertation submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy of the University College London Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience University College London 2015 1 Declaration I, Joseph Arizpe, confirm that the work presented in this thesis is my own. Where information has been derived from other sources, I confirm that this has been indicated in the thesis. 2 Abstract This thesis investigates factors influencing eye-movement patterns during face perception, the relationship of eye-movement patterns to facial recognition performance, and methodological considerations impacting the detection of differences in eye-movement patterns. In particular, in the first study (chapter 2), in which the basis of the other-race effect was investigated, differences in eye- movement patterns during recognition of own- versus other-race (African, Chinese) faces were found for Caucasian participants. However, these eye- movement differences were subtle and analysis-dependent, indicating that the discrepancy in prior reports regarding the presence or absence of such differences are due to variability in statistical sensitivity of analysis methods across studies. The second and third studies (chapters 3 and 4) characterized visuomotor factors, specifically pre-stimulus start position and distance, which strongly influence subsequent eye-movement patterns during face perception. An overall bias in fixation patterns to the opposite side of the face induced by start position and an increasing undershoot of the first ordinal fixation with increasing start distance were found. These visuomotor influences were not specific to faces and did not depend on the predictability of the location of the upcoming stimulus.
    [Show full text]
  • The Nature and Timing of Tele-Pseudoscopic Experiences
    University of Wollongong Research Online Faculty of Social Sciences - Papers Faculty of Social Sciences 2016 The an ture and timing of tele-pseudoscopic experiences Stephen Palmisano University of Wollongong, [email protected] Harold C. Hill University of Wollongong, [email protected] Robert S. Allison York University, [email protected] Publication Details Palmisano, S., Hill, H. & Allison, R. S. (2016). The an ture and timing of tele-pseudoscopic experiences. i-Perception, 7 (1), 1-24. Research Online is the open access institutional repository for the University of Wollongong. For further information contact the UOW Library: [email protected] The an ture and timing of tele-pseudoscopic experiences Abstract Interchanging the left nda right eye views of a scene (pseudoscopic viewing) has been reported to produce vivid stereoscopic effects under certain conditions. In two separate field studies, we examined the experiences of 124 observers (76 in Study 1 and 48 in Study 2) while pseudoscopically viewing a distant natural outdoor scene. We found large individual differences in both the nature and the timing of their pseudoscopic experiences. While some observers failed to notice anything unusual about the pseudoscopic scene, most experienced multiple pseudoscopic phenomena, including apparent scene depth reversals, apparent object shape reversals, apparent size and flatness changes, apparent reversals of border ownership, and even complex illusory foreground surfaces. When multiple effects were experienced, patterns of cooccurrence suggested possible causal relationships between apparent scene depth reversals and several other pseudoscopic phenomena. The al tency for experiencing pseudoscopic phenomena was found to correlate significantly with observer visual acuity, but not stereoacuity, in both studies.
    [Show full text]
  • The Nature and Timing of Tele-Pseudoscopic
    Article i-Perception The Nature and Timing January-February 2016: 1–24 ! The Author(s) 2016 DOI: 10.1177/2041669515625793 of Tele-Pseudoscopic ipe.sagepub.com Experiences Stephen Palmisano Centre for Psychophysics, Psychophysiology and Psychopharmacology, School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, NSW, Australia Harold Hill School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, NSW, Australia Robert S Allison Centre for Vision Research, York University, Ontario, Canada Abstract Interchanging the left and right eye views of a scene (pseudoscopic viewing) has been reported to produce vivid stereoscopic effects under certain conditions. In two separate field studies, we examined the experiences of 124 observers (76 in Study 1 and 48 in Study 2) while pseudoscopically viewing a distant natural outdoor scene. We found large individual differences in both the nature and the timing of their pseudoscopic experiences. While some observers failed to notice anything unusual about the pseudoscopic scene, most experienced multiple pseudoscopic phenomena, including apparent scene depth reversals, apparent object shape reversals, apparent size and flatness changes, apparent reversals of border ownership, and even complex illusory foreground surfaces. When multiple effects were experienced, patterns of co- occurrence suggested possible causal relationships between apparent scene depth reversals and several other pseudoscopic phenomena. The latency for experiencing pseudoscopic phenomena was found to correlate significantly with observer visual acuity, but not stereoacuity, in both studies. Keywords Stereopsis, pseudoscopic viewing, depth perception, space perception Introduction Our laterally separated eyes receive different perspective views of the same scene. One direct consequence of this horizontal eye arrangement is that the angular subtense between Corresponding author: Stephen Palmisano, Centre for Psychophysics, Psychophysiology and Psychopharmacology, School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, NSW 2522, Australia.
    [Show full text]
  • University of Dundee the Disparate Histories of Binocular Vision And
    University of Dundee The disparate histories of binocular vision and binaural hearing Wade, Nicholas J. Published in: Journal of the History of the Neurosciences DOI: 10.1080/0964704X.2017.1347389 Publication date: 2018 Document Version Peer reviewed version Link to publication in Discovery Research Portal Citation for published version (APA): Wade, N. J. (2018). The disparate histories of binocular vision and binaural hearing. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences, 27(1), 10-35. https://doi.org/10.1080/0964704X.2017.1347389 General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in Discovery Research Portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from Discovery Research Portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain. • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal. Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 29. Sep. 2021 1 The disparate histories of binocular vision and binaural hearing Running head: Binocular vision and binaural hearing Nicholas J. Wade, Psychology, University of Dundee, Nethergate, Dundee DD1 4HN, UK Tel: +44 1382384616 E-mail: [email protected] This is the accepted manuscript version.
    [Show full text]
  • Depth Inversion Despite Stereopsis: the Appearance of Random-Dot Stereograms on Surfaces Seen in Reverse Perspective
    Perception, 1979, volume 8, pages 135-142 Depth inversion despite stereopsis: the appearance of random-dot stereograms on surfaces seen in reverse perspective John I Yellott, Jr, Jerry L Kaiwi Cognitive Science Group, School of Social Sciences, University of California at Irvine, Irvine, California 92717, USA Received 20 October 1977, in revised form 18 October 1978 Abstract. Inside-out relief masks of faces can be depth-inverted (i.e. seen in reverse perspective) during close-up binocular viewing. If a random-dot stereogram is projected onto such a mask, stereopsis can be achieved for the stereogram, and its depth planes are correctly seen while the mask itself, including the region covered by the stereogram, is simultaneously perceived as depth- inverted. This demonstration shows that binocular depth inversion cannot be explained by a complete loss of stereoscopic information (e.g. through monocular suppression), or by a process analogous to pseudoscopic viewing whereby retinal disparities are incorporated into perception, but with their signs uniformly reversed. 1 Introduction Reversible perspective is probably most often thought of in connection with ambiguous pictures like the Necker cube (figure la), but it is also well-known that depth reversals can sometimes be experienced with solid objects, and with far more dramatic perceptual consequences. [Gregory (1970) describes these in detail; Helmholtz (1925) reviews references back to 1712, and Hochberg (1972) and Robinson (1972) review the modern literature, which is surprisingly thin.] In this note we use 'depth inversion' to refer specifically to this second kind of reversible perspective, in which real objects (rather than depicted ones) are perceived, from the standpoint of depth, as inside-out, so that their concave surfaces appear convex and vice versa.
    [Show full text]
  • Hershenson (1992) Size-Distance Invariance. Kinetic Invariance Is
    Perception & Psychophysics 1992, 51 (6), 541-548 Size-distance invariance: Kinetic invariance is different from static invariance MAURICE HERSHENSON Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts The static form of the size-distance invariance hypothesisasserts that a given proximal stimu- lus size (visual angle) determines a unique and constant ratio of perceived-object size to perceived object distance. A proposed kinetic invariance hypothesis asserts that a changing proximal stim- ulus size (an expanding or contracting solid visual angle) produces a constant perceived size and a changing perceived distance such that the instantaneous ratio of perceived size to perceived distance is determined by the instantaneous value of visual angle. The kinetic invariance hy- pothesis requires a new concept, an operating constraint, to mediate between the proximal ex- pansion or contraction pattern and the perception of rigid object motion in depth. As a conse- quence of the operating constraint, expansion and contraction patterns are automatically represented in consciousness as rigid objects. In certain static situations, the operation of this constraint produces the anomalous perceived-size-perceived-distance relations called the size- distance paradox. The size-distance invariance hypothesis (SDIH) asserts STATIC INVARIANCE RELATIONSHIP that a given proximal stimulus size (visual angle) deter- mines a unique constant ratio of perceived object size to Traditional Formulation perceived object distance (Epstein, 1977; Epstein, Park, The traditional form of the SDIH is derived from the & Casey, 1961; Kilpatrick & Ittelson, 1953). Is it possible analysis of stationary objects (Epstein, 1977; Gilinsky, to explain the moon illusion and retain this form of the 1951; Ittelson, 1951a; Johansson, 1977; Kilpatrick & It- SDIH? To do so, Kaufman and Rock (1962, 1989; Rock telson, 1953; Weintraub & Gardner, 1970).
    [Show full text]
  • Vision and Mind: Selected Readings in the Philosophy of Perception
    ision and Mind Selected Readings in the Philosophy of Perception edited by Alva Noë and Evan Thompson Vision and Mind This Page Intentionally Left Blank Vision and Mind Selected Readings in the Philosophy of Perception edited by Alva Noë and Evan Thompson A Bradford Book The MIT Press Cambridge, Massachusetts London, England © 2002 Massachusetts Institute of Technology All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form by any electronic or mechanical means (including photocopying, recording, or information storage and retrieval) without permission in writing from the publisher. This book was set in Sabon by SNP Best-set Typesetter Ltd., Hong Kong. Printed and bound in the United States of America. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Vision and mind : selected readings in the philosophy of perception / edited by Alva Noë and Evan Thompson. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-262-14078-0 (alk. paper)—ISBN 0-262-64047-3 (pbk. : alk. paper) 1. Perception (Philosophy) I. Noë, Alva. II. Thompson, Evan. B828.45 .V565 2002 121¢.34—dc21 2002023533 Contents Preface vii Sources ix 1 Introduction 1 Alva Noë and Evan Thompson 2 Selections from Phenomenology of Perception 15 Maurice Merleau-Ponty 3 Some Remarks about the Senses 35 H. P. Grice 4 The Intentionality of Sensation: A Grammatical Feature 55 G. E. M. Anscombe 5 A Theory of Direct Visual Perception 77 James J. Gibson 6 Perception and Its Objects 91 P. F. Strawson 7 Perceptions as Hypotheses 111 Richard L. Gregory 8 Veridical Hallucination and Prosthetic Vision 135 David Lewis 9 Perception, Vision and Causation 151 Paul Snowdon 10 How Direct Is Visual Perception?: Some Reflections on Gibson’s “Ecological Approach” 167 Jerry A.
    [Show full text]
  • Bela Julesz in Depth
    Commentary Bela Julesz in Depth Thomas V. Papathomas 1,*, Kazunori Morikawa 2 and Nicholas Wade 3 1 Laboratory of Vision Research, Center for Cognitive Science, and Department of Biomedical Engineering, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ 08854, USA 2 School of Human Sciences, Osaka University, Suita, Osaka 565-0871 Japan; [email protected] 3 Department of Psychology, University of Dundee, Nethergate, Dundee DD1 4HN, UK; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-848-445-6533 Received: 23 March 2019; Accepted: 5 May 2019; Published: 8 May 2019 Abstract: A brief tribute to Bela Julesz (1928–2003) is made in words and images. In addition to a conventional stereophotographic portrait, his major contributions to vision research are commemorated by two ‘perceptual portraits’, which try to capture the spirit of his main accomplishments in stereopsis and the perception of texture. Keywords: depth perception; stereopsis; texture; perceptual portrait; random-dot stereograms Binocular vision has been a topic of enquiry for at least two millennia but linking retinal disparity to depth perception is much more recent. Almost two centuries ago, the invention of the stereoscope by Wheatstone [1] revolutionized the experimental investigation of binocular vision. Separating the perception of depth from object recognition (with random-dot stereograms [2]) heralded a second revolution. It is then fitting that a scientist who contributed so much to our understanding of stereoscopic vision should be presented stereoscopically, as in Figure1. Most significantly, Bela Julesz is also represented in two ‘perceptual portraits’. Their aim is to combine his portrait with graphical elements that reflect the contributions that he made to the study of mind and behavior—after the manner of those displayed in Wade’s book [3].
    [Show full text]
  • A Stereo/Photo Glossary Page 1 of 34
    A Stereo/Photo Glossary Page 1 of 34 A STEREO/PHOTO GLOSSARY (Second edition, version 93.5/97.4a) by: Craig Daniels and Dr. Dale E. Hammerschmidt This short work is intended for anyone interested in stereoscopic pursuits, but particularly for those who are trying to create stereoscopic images. We hope that the words and phrases to be found here will become useful tools rather than obstacles in your approach to "3-D" imaging. We stress that this is a compendium of terms that we have found useful, confusing and/or interesting; while we have tried to make it accurate, we do not represent that it is complete or of great scholarly depth. Some entries are long, because we found the subject interesting or we knew a lot about it; some are short for the obvious other reasons! We have tried to keep the tone conversational, rather than strive for uniform and rigid style; we've not felt ourselves above an occasional wise-crack. Additions, corrections and improvements are always welcome --- we see ourselves as editors rather than authors. (Glossary is available via diskette or e-mail.) * ANSI: American National Standards Institute. See next entry. * ASA: (1) American Standards Association. Although the expression "ASA" is still applied to U.S. film speeds, the "American Standards Association" changed its name to the "American National Standards Institute" in 1969. Their standards are referenced by ANSI numbers such as "PH3.11-1953" (which describes the 5p format used in cameras like the Stereo Realist and the Kodak Stereo 35). (2) As a film speed, it now appears in conjunction with the European DIN number (see) in the format "100/21"" which, as such, is the "ISO" speed.
    [Show full text]
  • A Functional MRI Index of Spatial Context Effects in Vision
    Psychology, 2015, 6, 2145-2154 Published Online December 2015 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/psych http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/psych.2015.616211 A Functional MRI Index of Spatial Context Effects in Vision Keith A. Feigenson1,2*, Catherine Hanson3, Thomas Papathomas4, Steven M. Silverstein1,5 1Department of Psychiatry, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School at Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Piscataway, USA 2Albright College, Reading, PA, USA 3Rutgers University Brain Imaging Center, Newark, NJ, USA 4Department of Biomedical Engineering and Center for Cognitive Science, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, USA 5University Behavioral Health Care at Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Piscataway, NJ, USA Received 25 October 2015; accepted 28 December 2015; published 31 December 2015 Copyright © 2015 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc. This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International License (CC BY). http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Abstract Coordination of brain activity, in the form of modulation of feedforward activity by stored infor- mation and expectations, occurs across domains of perception and cognition. A reliable and com- pelling example of this is size contrast in vision. This paper builds on a prior study to show that in healthy humans, the spread of activation in striate and extrastriate visual cortex during a con- text-modulated size perception task is dependent on the perceived size of the target image, not on the physical size of the retinal image. These data provide further evidence that early regions in visual cortex are modulated by top-down influences, and provide a framework for investigating visual context processing in psychiatric disorders where reduced sensitivity to visual contextual effects has been demonstrated in behavioral tasks.
    [Show full text]
  • Combining Cues to Judge Distance and Direction in an Immersive Virtual Reality Environment
    Journal of Vision (2021) 21(4):10, 1–25 1 Combining cues to judge distance and direction in an immersive virtual reality environment Peter Scarfe University of Reading, Earley Gate, Reading, UK Andrew Glennerster University of Reading, Earley Gate, Reading, UK When we move, the visual direction of objects in the most psychophysical experiments, the estimation of environment can change substantially. Compared with visual direction seems simpler and less to do with the our understanding of depth perception, the problem the representation of the 3D world around us. However, visual system faces in computing this change is relatively for a moving observer in a static world, or a static poorly understood. Here, we tested the extent to which observer viewing moving objects, the situation is quite participants’ judgments of visual direction could be different (Wexler et al., 2001). Objects change both predicted by standard cue combination rules. their depth and their visual direction and, in both cases, Participants were tested in virtual reality using a if an observer is to perceive a stable 3D world, they head-mounted display. In a simulated room, they judged must take account of changes in the visual direction of the position of an object at one location, before walking objects just as much as they do for changes in object to another location in the room and judging, in a second depth. interval, whether an object was at the expected visual There is good evidence that people are able to update direction of the first. By manipulating the scale of the room across intervals, which was subjectively invisible their estimate of the visual direction of previously to observers, we put two classes of cue into conflict, one viewed objects when they move to a new location (Foo that depends only on visual information and one that et al., 2005; Klatzky et al., 2003; Klier et al., 2008; uses proprioceptive information to scale any Loomis et al., 1998; Medendorp, 2011; Rieser & Rider, reconstruction of the scene.
    [Show full text]
  • Catherine E. Grafton Phd Thesis
    BINOCULAR VISION AND THREE-DIMENSIONAL MOTION PERCEPTION: THE USE OF CHANGING DISPARITY AND INTER-OCULAR VELOCITY DIFFERENCES Catherine E. Grafton A Thesis Submitted for the Degree of PhD at the University of St Andrews 2011 Full metadata for this item is available in Research@StAndrews:FullText at: http://research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk/ Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://hdl.handle.net/10023/1922 This item is protected by original copyright This item is licensed under a Creative Commons Licence Binocular Vision and Three-Dimensional Motion Perception: The Use of Changing Disparity and Inter-Ocular Velocity Differences Catherine E. Grafton Submitted for degree of Doctor of Philosophy Friday August 20th 2010 Thesis Declaration I, Catherine Grafton, hereby certify that this thesis, which is approximately 69,700 words in length, has been written by me, that it is the record of work carried out by me and that it has not been submitted in any previous application for a higher degree. I was admitted as a research student in October 2006 and as a candidate for the degree of PhD in November 2010; the higher study for which this is a record was carried out in the University of St Andrews between 2006 and 2010. date ..................... signature of candidate ................................ I hereby certify that the candidate has fulfilled the conditions of the Resolution and Regulations appropriate for the degree of PhD in the University of St Andrews and that the candidate is qualified to submit this thesis in application for that degree. date ..................... signature of supervisor ...............................
    [Show full text]