ijmec 14 (1) pp. 9–15 Intellect Limited 2019

International Journal of Music in Early Childhood Volume 14 Number 1 ijmec © 2019 Intellect Ltd Article. English language. doi: 10.1386/ijmec.14.1.9_1

International Journal of Music in Early Childhood

Intellect

10.1386/ijmec.14.1.9_1

14 SANDRA E. TREHUB University of Toronto 1

9

15 Nurturing infants with music

© 2019 Intellect Ltd ABSTRACT KEYWORDS 2019 Primary caregivers throughout the world provide infants with life-sustaining infants care such as nutrition and protection from harm as well as life-enhancing care caregivers such as affection, contingent responsiveness and mentoring of various kinds. They mothers ARTICLES nurture infants musically by means of one-on-one (i.e. infant-directed) singing music accompanied by movement in some cultures and by visual gestures in others. Such singing singing, which is acoustically and visually distinct from solitary (i.e. self-directed) emotional regulation singing, is effective in engaging infants and regulating their mood and arousal. The repetition and stereotypy of caregivers’ performances contribute to their memorability and dyadic significance. Caregivers’ singing also influences infants’ social engagement more generally. Once infants become singers, their songs play an important role in social interaction and emotional self-regulation. Although caregivers sing to infants with playful or soothing intentions, their performances highlight the temporal and melodic structure of the music. In sum, caregivers lay the foundation for a lifelong musical journey.

Primary caregivers nurture infants in countless ways. In addition to providing life-sustaining care such as nutrition and protection from harm, they provide life-enhancing care such as affection, contingent responsiveness and various kinds of mentoring. The focus here is on musical nurture, which commonly occurs in the context of affectionate and responsive care. Musical nurture is achieved primarily by one-on-one, or infant-directed, singing and secondarily by exposure to live or recorded music at home or else- where (Custodero and Johnson-Green 2003; Ilari 2005). Caregivers around the world sing to infants although there are notable variations in the content

www.intellectbooks.com 9 Sandra E. Trehub

and manner of singing (Trehub and Gudmundsdottir 2015; Trehub and Cirelli 2019; Trehub and Trainor 1998). In parts of the world where infants are carried for much of the day (e.g. in slings) and sleep alongside mothers at night, vocal interactions are generally soothing, with lullabies being the songs of choice. Infants in urban environments, especially in North America, experience less physical contact but more face-to-face contact with caregivers, whose predominant manner of interacting with infants is playful. Such caregivers generally sing children’s play songs (i.e. lively, playful songs such as ‘Itsy Bitsy Spider’) rather than lullabies. Those who also sing lullabies tend to reserve them for bedtime routines. Lullabies, which are characteristically simple and repetitious (Unyk et al. 1992), are readily distinguished from non-lullabies by adults who are unfa- miliar with the musical style (i.e. culture) or language (i.e. meaning of lyrics) (Mehr et al. 2018; Trehub et al. 1993a). It remains to be determined whether play songs are comparably recognizable across cultures. Nevertheless, perfor- mances directed to infants, whether lullabies or play songs, are readily distin- guished from informal, self-directed performances (i.e. no infant present) by listeners who differ in culture, gender or musical training (Trainor 1996; Trehub et al. 1993b, 1997).

The nature of singing to infants In general, caregivers’ songs to infants are higher pitched, slower, more temporally predictable, and more emotionally expressive than solitary (i.e. self-directed) performances of the same songs (Nakata and Trehub 2011; Trainor 1996; Trainor et al. 1997; Trehub et al. 1997). Moreover, playful rendi- tions of songs are typically higher pitched, faster, louder and more rhythmic than soothing renditions (Cirelli et al. forthcoming). The presence of an infant enhances the expressiveness of songs even when preschool siblings are the singers (Trehub et al. 1994). The singer’s expressiveness is influenced not only by infants’ presence but also by subtle feedback from infants. For example, mothers’ sung renditions are more emotive when the infant is fully in view rather than comparably near but obscured from view (Trehub et al. 2016). The implication is that subtle signs of infant engagement such as eye contact or eye-widening have emotional consequences on the singer and, in turn, on the unfolding performance. For reasons that are as yet clear, caregivers’ sung performances for infants are highly stereotyped in the sense of being almost identical in pitch level and tempo on different occasions (Bergeson and Trehub 2002). The consequences of such stable performances are magnified by caregivers’ inclination to sing a relatively small number of songs repeatedly rather than sampling more widely from their repertoire of children’s songs (Trehub and Gudmundsdottir 2019). For the most part, caregivers provide multimodal renditions of songs. Infants who are carried throughout the day commonly experience songs in conjunction with movement from the caregiver’s ongoing activities. Where infants are rocked to sleep in cradles, their face is often covered to minimize visual stimulation (Trehub and Prince 2010). The movement that infants expe- rience during exposure to sound patterns dominates their perception of the auditory rhythms (Phillips-Silver and Trainor 2005). North American infants experience most of their singing in face-to-face contexts, often without touch, but caregivers’ facial expressions and ener- getic movements are integral to their performance (Cirelli et al. forthcoming;

10 international Journal of Music in Early Childhood Nurturing infants with music

Longhi 2009). Face-to-face singing features almost constant smiling, in contrast to the intermittent smiling that characterizes caregivers’ face-to-face speech with infants (Trehub et al. 2016).

Effects of singing on infant attention Infant-directed singing has notable effects on infant attention. For example, infants listen longer to audio-recordings of infant-directed songs than to solo or self-directed recordings of the same songs (Masataka 1999; Trainor 1996), presumably because of the greater positive affect of infant-directed renditions. Just as infants listen longer to happy-sounding speech than to speech with more neutral affect (Singh et al. 2002), they also listen longer to happy-sound- ing songs than to songs or speech that are less affectively positive (Corbeil et al. 2013). In general, their attention to happy-sounding renditions of speech and song is comparable (Corbeil et al. 2013; Costa-Giomi and Ilari 2014; Trehub et al. 2016; but see Tsang et al. 2017). With richer or more natural stimuli, such as video-recordings of previous mother–infant interactions, infants exhibit more intense and more sustained attention for their mother’s singing than for her speech (Nakata and Trehub 2004). Greater attention to audio-visual renditions of song over speech is also evident for unfamiliar speakers and singers (Costa-Giomi 2014). In the case of live interactions, infants are more attentive to their mother when she sings play songs rather than lullabies (Cirelli et al. forthcoming), which is line with the greater attention-eliciting properties of happy-sounding vocalizations (Corbeil et al. 2013).

Effects of singing on infant arousal and mood As noted, happy-sounding audio-recordings of speech and singing tend to be equally effective in capturing infant attention (Corbeil et al. 2013; Costa-Giomi and Ilari 2014). However, those comparisons occurred in the context of very brief test trials lasting seconds rather than minutes. More relevant to everyday considerations is whether speech and singing are comparable or differentially effective in maintaining infants’ attention or composure over longer periods. In one study, 7- to 10-month-old infants were exposed to one of three audio recordings that played continuously until they began to cry: (1) an unfamil- iar Turkish play song performed in an infant-directed manner, (2) an infant- directed recitation of the song lyrics or (3) or an adult-directed or emotionally neutral recitation of the lyrics (Corbeil et al. 2016). The test environment was anything but friendly, involving a dimly lit room and no toys or people within view. On average, infants remained relatively content (i.e. no crying) for about nine minutes when listening to the infant-directed song. By contrast, the onset of crying occurred after four minutes or so of listening to the recited lyrics whether they were infant- or adult-directed. Although infant-directed or happy-sounding speech is much more effective than adult-directed speech in eliciting infants’ attention (Singh et al. 2002), it turned out to be no more effec- tive in sustaining infants’ composure and averting distress. Moreover, infant- directed singing was clearly superior to either speech register in that regard. Infant-directed singing can alter infants’ arousal levels even in the absence of overt mood changes. For example, maternal singing (self-selected songs) to 6-month-old infants succeeded in lowering arousal in infants with higher initial levels and raising arousal in those with lower initial levels (Shenfield

www.intellectbooks.com 11 Sandra E. Trehub

et al. 2003). These effects could not be attributed to song type (e.g. soothing vs. playful) because all mothers chose to sing children’s play songs in a lively manner. It is possible, then, infant-directed singing optimizes infant arousal in relation to the circumstances at hand. In a more recent study, mothers were required to repeatedly sing a designated song, ‘Twinkle, Twinkle, Little Star’, to their 8- to 11-month-old infants (Cirelli et al. forthcoming). On alternating trials, they sang the song in a playful or soothing manner. During the sooth- ing renditions, maternal and infant arousal levels decreased in parallel. By contrast, arousal remained stable from the play phase that preceded each trial to the playful singing that followed. In other words, lively play, with or without song, seems to have similar effects on arousal. If infant-directed singing is effective in modulating the arousal levels of contented infants (Cirelli et al. forthcoming; Shenfield et al. 2003) and maintaining their composure (Corbeil et al. 2016), might it also be effective in lowering the arousal levels of distressed infants? In one study (Ghazban 2013), distress was induced in 10-month-old infants by means of the still- face procedure (Mesman et al. 2009). Specifically, mothers initially played with infants, then became completely unresponsive (adopting a still, inexpressive face), and subsequently resumed their interactions with infants in conjunc- tion with multimodal (face-to-face) singing on some trials and multimodal (face-to-face) speech on others. As expected, arousal levels increased substan- tially during the still-face episode, as did overt negative behaviour (e.g. nega- tive vocalizations and facial expressions). However, maternal singing was considerably more effective than maternal speech in lowering infants’ elevated arousal levels and ameliorating distress. Moreover, play songs were more effective than lullabies in reducing arousal and distress. The consequences of infant-directed singing for arousal regulation in singers and listeners are likely to enhance caregivers’ well-being and feelings of parenting efficacy while also strengthening reciprocal bonds between caregivers and infants (Fancourt and Perkins 2018a, 2018b; Trehub and Cirelli 2018).

Social consequences of singing Caregivers’ songs have implications beyond the caregiving relationship. For example, 5-month-old infants exhibit greater interest in an unfamiliar woman who previously sang a song from the mother’s repertoire as compared with another woman who sang a different song (Mehr et al. 2016). Moreover, 14-month-old infants offer more help to an unfamiliar woman after she sings a familiar song rather than an unfamiliar song (Cirelli and Trehub 2018). In fact, the mothers of infants who helped the most – retrieving the greatest number of ‘accidentally’ dropped objects – had sung the song most frequently. Infants may evaluate singers of those treasured songs as potentially suitable social partners. Not long after their first birthday, many infants start producing song frag- ments, initially adding sound snippets to caregivers’ performances, then partic- ipating more fully in duets, and finally becoming solo singers (Trehub and Gudmundsdottir 2015; Gudmundsdottir and Trehub 2019). Early solo sing- ing generally features songs from the caregiver’s repertoire rather than famil- iar songs linked to well-liked cartoon characters (e.g. Dora). Solitary singing provides infants with a potent tool for emotional self-regulation. Many infants strategically deploy that tool by singing in their crib (Sole 2017), a practice that eases the pain of separation from family members and ensures a calm and pleasurable transition to sleep.

12 international Journal of Music in Early Childhood Nurturing infants with music

Intuitive didactic aspects of maternal singing Although caregivers’ performances are geared towards playful or sooth- ing goals, their performances incorporate adjustments that emphasize vari- ous aspects of musical structure. For example, mothers intuitively highlight the temporal structure of their songs by increased pulse clarity – for example, much louder strong beats than weak beats (Cirelli et al. forthcoming). In addi- tion, they increase the transparency of the metrical and phrase structure of the music by their patterning of actions and sounds (Longhi 2009). Mothers also enhance the dynamic contrasts between higher and lower pitches, highlighting the shapes or contours of melodies (Nakata and Trehub 2011). Finally, caregiv- ers’ use of a limited song repertoire and their propensity to repeat each song at the same pitch level and tempo (Bergeson and Trehub 2002) contribute to the distinctiveness, memorability and emotional significance of their songs. In this manner, primary caregivers lay the foundation for a life of musical engagement.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Preparation of this manuscript was assisted by funding from the National Sciences and Engineering Research Council and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.

REFERENCES Bergeson, T. R. and Trehub, S. E. (2002), ‘ and tempo in mothers’ songs to infants’, Psychological Science, 13:1, pp. 71–74. Cirelli, L. K., Jurewicz, Z. B. and Trehub, S. E. (forthcoming), ‘Effects of maternal singing style on mother–infant arousal and behavior’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, n.pag. Cirelli, L. K. and Trehub, S. E. (2018), ‘Infants help singers of familiar songs’, Music & Science, 1, pp. 1–11. Corbeil, M., Trehub, S. E. and Peretz, I. (2013), ‘Infants prefer speech to singing only when speech sounds happier’, Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 372. ——— (2016), ‘Singing delays the onset of infant distress’, Infancy, 21:3, pp. 373–91. Costa-Giomi, E. (2014), ‘Mode of presentation affects infants’ preferential attention to singing and speech’, , 32:2, pp. 160–69. Costa-Giomi, E. and Ilari, B. (2014), ‘Infants’ preferential attention to sung and spoken stimuli’, Journal of Research in , 62:2, pp. 188–94. Custodero, L. A. and Johnson-Green, E. A. (2003), ‘Passing the cultural torch: Musical experience and musical parenting of infants’, Journal of Research in Music Education, 51:2, pp. 102–14. Fancourt, D. and Perkins, R. (2018a), ‘Maternal engagement with music up to nine months post-birth: Findings from a cross-sectional study in England’, Psychology of Music, 46:2, pp. 238–51. ——— (2018b), ‘The effects of mother–infant singing on emotional closeness, affect, anxiety, and stress hormones’, Music & Science, 1, pp. 1–10. Ghazban, N. (2013), ‘Emotion regulation in infants using maternal singing and speech’, unpublished doctoral dissertation, Toronto: Ryerson University. Gudmundsdottir, H. and Trehub, S. (2018), ‘Adults recognize toddlers’ song renditions’, Psychology of Music, 46:2, pp. 281–91. Ilari, B. (2005), ‘On musical parenting of young children: Musical beliefs and behaviors of mothers and infants’, Early Child Development and Care, 175:7&8, pp. 647–60.

www.intellectbooks.com 13 Sandra E. Trehub

Longhi, E. (2009), ‘“Songese”: Maternal structuring of musical interaction with infants’, Psychology of Music, 37:2, pp. 195–213. Masataka, N. (1999), ‘Preference for infant-directed singing in 2-day-old infants of deaf parents’, Developmental Psychology, 35:4, pp. 1001–05. Mehr, S. A., Singh, M., York, H. W., Glowacki, L. and Krasnow, M. M. (2018), ‘Form and function in human song’, Current Biology, 28:3, pp. 356–68. Mehr, S. A., Song, L. A. and Spelke, E. S. (2016), ‘For 5-month-old infants, melodies are social’, Psychological Science, 27:4, pp. 486–501. Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H. and Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009), ‘The many faces of the still-face paradigm: A review and meta- analysis’, Developmental Review, 29:2, pp. 120–62. Nakata, T. and Trehub, S. E. (2004), ‘Infants’ responsiveness to maternal speech and singing’, Infant Behavior and Development, 27:4, pp. 455–64. ——— (2011), ‘Expressive timing and dynamics in infant-directed singing’, Psychomusicology: Music, Mind and Brain, 21:1&2, pp. 45–53. Phillips-Silver, J. and Trainor, L. J. (2005), ‘Feeling the beat: Movement influen- ces infant rhythm perception’, Science, 308, p. 1430. Shenfield, T., Trehub, S. E. and Nakata, T. (2003), ‘Maternal singing modulates infant arousal’, Psychology of Music, 31:4, pp. 365–75. Singh, L., Morgan, J. L. and Best, C. T. (2002), ‘Infants’ listening preferences: Baby talk or happy talk?’, Infancy, 3:3, pp. 365–94. Sole, M. (2017), ‘Crib song: Insights into functions of toddlers’ private sponta- neous singing’, Psychology of Music, 45:2, pp. 172–92. Trainor, L. J. (1996), ‘Infant preferences for infant-directed versus noninfant- directed playsongs and lullabies’, Infant Behavior and Development, 19:1, pp. 83–92. Trainor, L. J., Clark, E. D., Huntley, A. and Adams, B. (1997), ‘The acoustic basis of preferences for infant-directed singing’, Infant Behavior and Development, 20:3, pp. 383–96. Trehub, S. E. and Cirelli, L. K. (2018), ‘Precursors to the performing arts in infancy and early childhood’, Progress in Brain Research, 237, pp. 225–42. Trehub, S. E. and Gudmundsdottir, H. R. (2019), ‘Mothers as singing mentors for infants’, in G. F. Welch, J. Nix and D. Howard (eds), Oxford Handbook of Singing, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 455–69. Trehub, S. E., Plantinga, J. and Russo, F. A. (2016), ‘Maternal vocal interac- tions with infants: Reciprocal visual influences’, Social Development, 25:3, pp. 665–83. Trehub, S. E. and Prince, R. (2010), ‘Lullabies and other women’s songs in the Turkish village of Akçaeniş’, UNESCO Observatory E-journal, 2:2, n.pag. Trehub, S. E. and Trainor, L. J. (1998), ‘Singing to infants: Lullabies and play songs’, Advances in Infancy Research, 12, pp. 43–77. Trehub, S. E., Unyk, A. M. and Henderson, J. L. (1994), ‘Children’s songs to infant siblings: Parallels with speech’, Journal of Child Language, 21:3, pp. 735–44. Trehub, S. E., Unyk, A. M., Kamenetsky, S. B., Hill, D. S., Trainor, L. J., Henderson, J. L. and Saraza, M. (1997), ‘Mothers’ and fathers’ singing to infants’, Developmental Psychology, 33:3, pp. 500–07. Trehub, S. E., Unyk, A. M. and Trainor, L. J. (1993a), ‘Adults identify infant- directed music across cultures’, Infant Behavior and Development, 16:2, pp. 193–211.

14 international Journal of Music in Early Childhood Nurturing infants with music

——— (1993b), ‘Maternal singing in cross-cultural perspective’, Infant Behavior and Development, 16:3, pp. 285–95. Tsang, C. D., Falk, S. and Hessel, A. (2017), ‘Infants prefer infant-directed song over speech’, Child Development, 88:4, pp. 1207–15. Unyk, A. M., Trehub, S. E., Trainor, L. J. and Schellenberg, E. G. (1992), ‘Lullabies and simplicity: A cross-cultural perspective’, Psychology of Music, 20:1, pp. 15–28.

SUGGESTED CITATION Trehub, S. E. (2019), ‘Nurturing infants with music’, International Journal of Music in Early Childhood, 14:1, pp. 9–15, doi: 10.1386/ijmec.14.1.9_1

CONTRIBUTOR DETAILS Sandra Trehub studied economics and philosophy before obtaining her doctoral degree in psychology at McGill University in 1973. Since that time, she has been a faculty member in the Department of Psychology at the University of Toronto where she is currently Professor Emeritus. Although most of her research is conducted in laboratory contexts, she has travelled extensively to observe cross-cultural differences in musical interactions with infants. Among her scholarly honours are a Lifetime Achievement Award from the Society for Music Perception and Cognition (2013). Her research focuses on young chil- dren’s and adults’ perception of music and on maternal singing to infants. Contact: Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Mississauga, Ontario, L5L 1C6, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

Sandra E. Trehub has asserted her right under the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act, 1988, to be identified as the author of this work in the format that was submitted to Intellect Ltd.

www.intellectbooks.com 15