The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MUSIC Isabelle Peretz Robert J. Zatorre Editors OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Zat-fm.qxd 6/5/03 11:16 PM Page i THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MUSIC This page intentionally left blank THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MUSIC Edited by ISABELLE PERETZ Départment de Psychologie, Université de Montréal, C.P. 6128, Succ. Centre-Ville, Montréal, Québec, H3C 3J7, Canada and ROBERT J. ZATORRE Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada 1 Zat-fm.qxd 6/5/03 11:16 PM Page iv 1 Great Clarendon Street, Oxford Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide in Oxford New York Auckland Bangkok Buenos Aires Cape Town Chennai Dar es Salaam Delhi Hong Kong Istanbul Karachi Kolkata Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Mumbai Nairobi São Paulo Shanghai Taipei Tokyo Toronto Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries Published in the United States by Oxford University Press Inc., New York © The New York Academy of Sciences, Chapters 1–7, 9–20, and 22–8, and Oxford University Press, Chapters 8 and 21. Most of the materials in this book originally appeared in The Biological Foundations of Music, published as Volume 930 of the Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, June 2001 (ISBN 1-57331-306-8). This book is an expanded version of the original Annals volume. The moral rights of the author have been asserted Database right Oxford University Press (maker) First published 2003 All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press, or as expressly permitted by law, or under terms agreed with the appropriate reprographics rights organization. Enquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope of the above should be sent to the Rights Department, Oxford University Press, at the address above You must not circulate this book in any other binding or cover and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer A catalogue record for this title is available from the British Library Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data (Data available) ISBN 0 19 852519 2 (Hbk) 0 19 852520 6 (Pbk) 10987654321 Typeset by Newgen Imaging Systems (P) Ltd., Chennai, India Printed in Great Britain on acid-free paper by T.J. International Ltd, Padstow Zat-fm.qxd 6/5/03 11:16 PM Page v PREFACE Over the past decade there has been an explosion in research activities on music perception and performance, and their correlates in the human brain. This sudden increase in scient- ific work on music has been motivated in part by the idea that music offers a unique oppor- tunity to better understand the organization of the human brain. The other major motivation for exploring the neural substrates of musical activities is that they may shed light on the functional origin and biological value of music. Like language, music exists in all human societies. Like language, music is a complex, rule-governed activity, and appears to be associated with a specific brain architecture. Moreover, sensitivity to musical struc- ture develops early in life, without conscious effort, in the large majority of the population. Music also appears to be specific to humans, although some investigators have begun to examine its possible evolutionary origins in other species’.1 Unlike most other high-level functions of the human brain—and unlike language—only a minority of individuals become proficient performing musicians through explicit tutor- ing. This particularity in the distribution of acquired skills confers to music a privileged role in the study of brain plasticity. Another distinction from language is that a large vari- ety of sensory-motor systems may be studied because of the many different ways of pro- ducing music. These variable modes of auditory expression enable interesting comparisons across systems. Given both its similarities with language and its divergence from it, we believe that the relationship between music and the brain is of central importance for the domain of cog- nitive neuroscience. The fact that musical activities have generally been considered as an exquisite product of human culture, and as such are often assumed to be merely a cultural artifact, should not be taken as an impediment to achieving a scientific understanding of its underlying basis. In fact, we would argue that the cultural overlay associated with music confers upon it a key role for understanding the biology of human cognitive functions. Indeed, the ubiquity of music, its developmental features, and its brain substrates raise the question of the nature and the extent of its biological foundations, and how these interact with culture. In this context, it is remarkable to note that the study of music as a major brain function has been relatively neglected. Neuropsychological questions related to musical abilities have been of occasional interest to neurologists and psychologists since the last century (e.g. Ref. 2 for pioneering work), but systematic, sustained investigations have been rare until recently. Several developments—both theoretical and technological—have created a profound change in the way in which music studies are perceived, thereby enabling us to present this volume as a beginning to the scientific study of the neurobiology of music. First, one should mention the development of cognitive psychology during the latter half of the twentieth century; cognitive psychologists were among the first to recognize the Zat-fm.qxd 6/5/03 11:16 PM Page vi vi value of music as a means of studying perception, memory, attention, and performance (see Refs 2–5 for the three benchmark textbooks). Second, neuropsychology, which had always been based on knowledge from neurophysiology and neuroanatomy, began also to adopt experimental and cognitive paradigms, permitting advances in understanding lesion effects on musical functions. Third, developmental psychologists exploited new techniques that allowed them to probe the mind of even neonates. Finally, and most dramatically per- haps with the arrival on the scene of neuroimaging techniques such as fMRI and MEG, the stage was set for the explosion to which we alluded above, and which forms the core of the present volume. The objective of the conference, from which most of the present texts are derived, was to demonstrate the dynamism and richness of the discipline. The conference was held in May 2000 in New York, and was initiated and sponsored by the New York Academy of Science. Major scientists from six different countries, working in a variety of interrelated disciplines and pursuing sustained research activities on music were invited to present their work. Most responded enthusiastically and addressed issues crucial to a better understanding of the neural substrates underlying musical functions. Their presentations first appeared in the Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, volume 930, under the title ‘The Biological Foundations of Music’ in 2001. Given the success of this issue, Oxford University Press invited us to edit an updated version of it in their collection of books. We eagerly accepted and invited a few other major players in the field who originally presented a poster to pub- lish a full chapter in the present volume. Therefore, you will find here an updated version of the 25 original chapters as well as three new chapters. The contributions in the present volume will reveal how much progress has been made in the field over the last decade. Besides providing research overviews, many texts also delineate significant avenues for future research. Indeed, it is clear that this is a very young field still, and that much remains to be done. We hope that the material presented in this volume, coupled with an increase of interest for the biological foundations of music in the scientific and general community, represent the initial spark for building a solid and stim- ulating cognitive neuroscience of music. I.P. and R.Z. February 2003 References 1. Wallin, N., B. Merker and S. Brown (ed.) (2000) The Origins of Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 2. Bouillaud, J. (1865) Sur la faculté du langage articulé. Bulletin de l’Académie de Médecine 30, 752–68. 3. Deutsch, D. (ed.) (1982) The Psychology of Music. New York: Academic Press. 4. Dowling, W. and D. Harwood (1986) Music Cognition. Series in cognition and perception. New York. Academic Press. 5. Sloboda, J. (1985) The Musical Mind: The Cognitive Psychology of Music.London:Oxford University Press. Zat-fm.qxd 6/5/03 11:16 PM Page vii CONTENTS List of contributors page xi Part I The origins of music 1 Musical predispositions in infancy: an update page 3 Sandra E. Trehub 2 The quest for universals in temporal processing in music page 21 Carolyn Drake and Daisy Bertrand 3 Mechanisms of musical memory in infancy page 32 Jenny R. Saffran 4 Music, cognition, culture, and evolution page 42 Ian Cross 5 Is music an evolutionary adaptation? page 57 David Huron Part II The musical mind 6 The roots of musical variation in perceptual similarity and invariance page 79 Stephen McAdams and Daniel Matzkin 7 Tonal cognition page 95 Carol L. Krumhansl and Petri Toiviainen 8 Learning and perceiving musical structures: further insights from artificial neural networks page 109 Barbara Tillmann, Jamshed J. Bharucha, and Emmanuel Bigand Part III The neurons of music 9 Neurobiology of harmony perception page 127 Mark Jude Tramo, Peter A. Cariani, Bertrund Delgutte, and Louis D. Braida 10 Intracerebral evoked potentials in pitch perception reveal a functional asymmetry of human auditory cortex page 152 Catherine Liégeois-Chauvel, Kimberly Giraud, Jean-Michel Badier, Patrick Marquis, and Patrick Chauvel Zat-fm.qxd 6/5/03 11:16 PM Page viii viii 11 The neural processing of complex sounds page 168 Timothy D.
Recommended publications
  • A Psychological Approach to Musical Form: the Habituation–Fluency Theory of Repetition
    A Psychological Approach to Musical Form: The Habituation–Fluency Theory of Repetition David Huron With the possible exception of dance and meditation, there appears to be nothing else in common human experience that is comparable to music in its repetitiveness (Kivy 1993; Ockelford 2005; Margulis 2013). Narrative arti- facts like movies, novels, cartoon strips, stories, and speeches have much less internal repetition. Even poetry is less repetitive than music. Occasionally, architecture can approach music in repeating some elements, but only some- times. There appears to be no visual analog to the sort of trance–inducing music that can engage listeners for hours. Although dance and meditation may be more repetitive than music, dance is rarely performed in the absence of music, and meditation tellingly relies on imagining a repeated sound or mantra (Huron 2006: 267). Repetition can be observed in music from all over the world (Nettl 2005). In much music, a simple “strophic” pattern is evident in which a single phrase or passage is repeated over and over. When sung, it is common for successive repetitions to employ different words, as in the case of strophic verses. However, it is also common to hear the same words used with each repetition. In the Western art–music tradition, internal patterns of repetition are commonly discussed under the rubric of form. Writing in The Oxford Companion to Music, Percy Scholes characterized musical form as “a series of strategies designed to find a successful mean between the opposite extremes of unrelieved repetition and unrelieved alteration” (1977: 289). Scholes’s characterization notwithstanding, musical form entails much more than simply the pattern of repetition.
    [Show full text]
  • Expectancy and Musical Emotion Effects of Pitch and Timing
    Manuscript Expectancy and musical emotion Effects of pitch and timing expectancy on musical emotion Sauvé, S. A.1, Sayed, A.1, Dean, R. T.2, Pearce, M. T.1 1Queen Mary, University of London 2Western Sydney University Author Note Correspondence can be addressed to Sarah Sauvé at [email protected] School of Electronic Engineering and Computer Science Queen Mary University of London, Mile End Road London E1 4NS United Kingdom +447733661107 Biographies S Sauve: Originally a pianist, Sarah is now a PhD candidate in the Electronic Engineering and Computer Science department at Queen Mary University of London studying expectancy and stream segregation, supported by a college studentship. EXPECTANCY AND MUSICAL EMOTION 2 A Sayed: Aminah completed her MSc in Computer Science at Queen Mary University of London, specializing in multimedia. R.T. Dean: Roger is a composer/improviser and researcher at the MARCS Institute for Brain, Behaviour and Development. His research focuses on music cognition and music computation, both analytic and generative. M.T. Pearce: Marcus is Senior Lecturer at Queen Mary University of London, director of the Music Cognition and EEG Labs and co-director of the Centre for Mind in Society. His research interests cover computational, psychological and neuroscientific aspects of music cognition, with a particular focus on dynamic, predictive processing of melodic, rhythmic and harmonic structure, and its impact on emotional and aesthetic experience. He is the author of the IDyOM model of auditory expectation based on statistical learning and probabilistic prediction. EXPECTANCY AND MUSICAL EMOTION 3 Abstract Pitch and timing information work hand in hand to create a coherent piece of music; but what happens when this information goes against the norm? Relationships between musical expectancy and emotional responses were investigated in a study conducted with 40 participants: 20 musicians and 20 non-musicians.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Processes for Infering Tonic
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Student Research, Creative Activity, and Performance - School of Music Music, School of 8-2011 Cognitive Processes for Infering Tonic Steven J. Kaup University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/musicstudent Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons, Music Practice Commons, Music Theory Commons, and the Other Music Commons Kaup, Steven J., "Cognitive Processes for Infering Tonic" (2011). Student Research, Creative Activity, and Performance - School of Music. 46. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/musicstudent/46 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Music, School of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Student Research, Creative Activity, and Performance - School of Music by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. COGNITIVE PROCESSES FOR INFERRING TONIC by Steven J. Kaup A THESIS Presented to the Faculty of The Graduate College at the University of Nebraska In Partial Fulfillment of Requirements For the Degree of Master of Music Major: Music Under the Supervision of Professor Stanley V. Kleppinger Lincoln, Nebraska August, 2011 COGNITIVE PROCESSES FOR INFERRING TONIC Steven J. Kaup, M. M. University of Nebraska, 2011 Advisor: Stanley V. Kleppinger Research concerning cognitive processes for tonic inference is diverse involving approaches from several different perspectives. Outwardly, the ability to infer tonic seems fundamentally simple; yet it cannot be attributed to any single cognitive process, but is multi-faceted, engaging complex elements of the brain. This study will examine past research concerning tonic inference in light of current findings.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Musical Meaning? Theorizing Music As Performative U Erance
    What is Musical Meaning? Theorizing Music as Performative Uerance * Andrew J. Chung NOTE: The examples for the (text-only) PDF version of this item are available online at: hp://www.mtosmt.org/issues/mto.19.25.1/mto.19.25.1.chung.php KEYWORDS: musical meaning, semiotics, J. L. Austin, performative uerance, speech-act theory, philosophy of language, experimental music ABSTRACT: In this article, I theorize a new conception of musical meaning, based on J. L. Austin’s theory of performative uerances in his treatise How to Do Things with Words. Austin theorizes language meaning pragmatically: he highlights the manifold ways language performs actions and is used to “do things” in praxis. Austin thereby suggests a new theoretic center for language meaning, an implication largely developed by others after his death. This article theorizes an analogous position that locates musical meaning in the use of music “to do things,” which may include performing actions such as reference and disclosure, but also includes, in a theoretically rigorous fashion, a manifold of other semiotic actions performed by music to apply pressure to its contexts of audition. I argue that while many questions have been asked about meanings of particular examples of music, a more fundamental question has not been addressed adequately: what does meaning mean? Studies of musical meaning, I argue, have systematically undertheorized the ways in which music, as interpretable uerance, can create, transform, maintain, and destroy aspects of the world in which it participates. They have largely presumed that the basic units of sense when it comes to questions of musical meaning consist of various messages, indexes, and references encoded into musical sound and signifiers.
    [Show full text]
  • Music to Your Ears
    baffies them as much as Beethoven's ONWAI\D A ND UPWAI\D WITH THE AI\T5 Ninth. They match at music as we snatched at movies, filli ng our heads with plural images. A friend with whom MUSIC TO YOUR EARS I was brooding over the way recorded sound supplies a soundtrack for modem The questfor 3-D recording and other mysteries ifsound life said that I ought to seek out Edgar Choueiri, a rocket scientist- really, a BY ADAM GOPNIK rocket scientist!- who has spent a good part of his life worrying about such things. C houeiri, my friend said, had broken the code of something funda­ mental about the reproduction ofsou nd, and so I went to Princeton, where he has two laboratories, to seek him out. Those of us who have no laboratory at all might regard what Choueiri mod­ estly calls his "other'' laboratory as the only laboratory that anyone would ever need. It is the size of a small airplane hangar, and it is filled with plasma rocket engines that run on electricity: instead of a wasteful explosion of liquid fuel, a judicious leak of ions pushes the craft forward through a vacuum. If we ever start commuting to Mars, it will likely be a Choueiri-style engine that gets us there and back. The president of the Electric Rocket Propulsion Society, Choueiri is also the president of the Lebanese Academy of Sciences, and is very much, in spirit and appearance, a man of the old Levant. There's the elegant classical nose, the high, anxious brows, and the worried ex­ pression.
    [Show full text]
  • A New Illusion in the Perception of Relative Pitch Intervals
    A new illusion in the perception of relative pitch intervals by Maartje Koning A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Humanities of the University of Amsterdam in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Department of Musicology 2015 Dr. M. Sadakata University of Amsterdam Dr. J.A. Burgoyne University of Amsterdam 2 Abstract This study is about the perception of relative pitch intervals. An earlier study of Sadakata & Ohgushi ‘Comparative judgments pitch intervals and an illusion’ (2000) showed that when when people listened to two tone intervals, their perception of relative pitch distance between the two tones depended on the direction and size of the intervals. In this follow-up study the participants had to listen to two tone intervals and indicate whether the size of the second interval was smaller, the same or larger than the first. The conditions were the same as in the study of Sadakata & Ohgushi. These four different conditions were illustrating the relationship between those two intervals. There were ascending and descending intervals and the starting tone of the second interval differed with respect to the starting tone of the first interval. The study made use of small and large intervals and hypothesized that the starting tone of the second interval with respect to the starting tone of the first interval had an effect on the melodic expectancy of the listener and because of that they over- or underestimate the size of the second tone interval. Furthermore, it was predicted that this tendency would be stronger for larger tone intervals compared to smaller tone intervals and that there would be no difference found between musicians and non-musicians.
    [Show full text]
  • The Perception of Melodic Consonance: an Acoustical And
    The perception of melodic consonance: an acoustical and neurophysiological explanation based on the overtone series Jared E. Anderson University of Pittsburgh Department of Mathematics Pittsburgh, PA, USA Abstract The melodic consonance of a sequence of tones is explained using the overtone series: the overtones form “flow lines” that link the tones melodically; the strength of these flow lines determines the melodic consonance. This hypothesis admits of psychoacoustical and neurophysiological interpretations that fit well with the place theory of pitch perception. The hypothesis is used to create a model for how the auditory system judges melodic consonance, which is used to algorithmically construct melodic sequences of tones. Keywords: auditory cortex, auditory system, algorithmic composition, automated com- position, consonance, dissonance, harmonics, Helmholtz, melodic consonance, melody, musical acoustics, neuroacoustics, neurophysiology, overtones, pitch perception, psy- choacoustics, tonotopy. 1. Introduction Consonance and dissonance are a basic aspect of the perception of tones, commonly de- scribed by words such as ‘pleasant/unpleasant’, ‘smooth/rough’, ‘euphonious/cacophonous’, or ‘stable/unstable’. This is just as for other aspects of the perception of tones: pitch is described by ‘high/low’; timbre by ‘brassy/reedy/percussive/etc.’; loudness by ‘loud/soft’. But consonance is a trickier concept than pitch, timbre, or loudness for three reasons: First, the single term consonance has been used to refer to different perceptions. The usual convention for distinguishing between these is to add an adjective specifying what sort arXiv:q-bio/0403031v1 [q-bio.NC] 22 Mar 2004 is being discussed. But there is not widespread agreement as to which adjectives should be used or exactly which perceptions they are supposed to refer to, because it is difficult to put complex perceptions into unambiguous language.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Musicology
    I THE BEGINNING 1 An Introduction to Evolutionary Musicology Steven Brown, Björn Merker, and Nils L. Wallin Abstract In this introduction to the new field of evolutionary musicology, we see that the study of music origins provides a fresh and exciting approach to the under- standing of human evolution, a topic that so far has been dominated by a focus on language evolution. The language-centered view of humanity has to be expanded to include music, first, because the evolution of language is highly inter- twined with the evolution of music, and, second, because music provides a spe- cific and direct means of exploring the evolution of human social structure, group function, and cultural behavior. Music making is the quintessential human cul- tural activity, and music is an ubiquitous element in all cultures large and small. The study of music evolution promises to shed light on such important issues as evolution of the hominid vocal tract; the structure of acoustic-communication signals; human group structure; division of labor at the group level; the capacity for designing and using tools; symbolic gesturing; localization and lateralization of brain function; melody and rhythm in speech; the phrase-structure of lan- guage; parent-infant communication; emotional and behavioral manipulation through sound; interpersonal bonding and synchronization mechanisms; self- expression and catharsis; creativity and aesthetic expression; the human affinity for the spiritual and the mystical; and finally, of course, the universal human attachment to music
    [Show full text]
  • MATTERS and Sound
    Letters to the Editor 833 1 Kennedy WR, Alter M, Sung JH. Progressive seemed in the first days to originate exter- The inhibition was revealed in our studies proximal spinal and bulbar muscular atro- phy of late onset: a sex-linked recessive trait. nally and was heard bilaterally. The melody during a period of voluntary contraction by J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.56.7.833 on 1 July 1993. Downloaded from Neurology 1968;18:671-80. she heard was extremely loud, leading her stimulating the motor cortex at a strength 2 Harding AE, Thomas PK, Baraister M, to ask surrounding people to turn off the lower than that required to produce excita- Bradbury PG, Morgan-Hughes JA, radio, which she believed to be the source tion under the same conditions. A recent Ponsford JR. X-linked recessive bulbospinal neuronopathy: a report of ten cases. J Neurol of the tune. The music began suddenly, was study has reported that the discharge of Neurosurg Psychiatty 1982;45: 1012-19. slow, clear and reminiscent of popular motor neurons in the first dorsal interosseus 3 Mukai E, Mitsuma T, Takahashi A, Sobue I. songs that she had heard in her youth, but muscle of the hand of a patient with multi- Endocrinological study of hypogonadism and feminization in patients with bulbar were still unknown to her. She was able to ple sclerosis could be suppressed by tran- spinal muscular atrophy. Clin Neurol sing this melody. Shortly after the onset of scranial magnetic stimulation of the motor (Tokyo) 1984;24:925-9.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians Sarah E
    University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 2013 Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians Sarah E. Weber University of Massachusetts Amherst Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/theses Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons, Fine Arts Commons, Music Education Commons, and the Music Theory Commons Weber, Sarah E., "Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians" (2013). Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014. 1162. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.umass.edu/theses/1162 This thesis is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses 1911 - February 2014 by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians A Thesis Presented by SARAH WEBER Submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Massachusetts Amherst in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF MUSIC September 2013 Music Theory © Copyright by Sarah E. Weber 2013 All Rights Reserved Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians A Thesis Presented by SARAH WEBER _____________________________ Gary S. Karpinski, Chair _____________________________ Andrew Cohen, Member _____________________________ Brent Auerbach, Member _____________________________ Jeff Cox, Department Head Department of Music and Dance DEDICATION For my parents and Grandma. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Kristen Wallentinsen for her help with experimental logistics, Renée Morgan for giving me her speakers, and Nathaniel Liberty for his unwavering support, problem-solving skills, and voice-over help.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognition, Constraints and Conceptual Blends in Modernist Music the Pleasure of Modernism: Intention, Meaning, and the Compositional Avant-Garde, Ed
    “Tone-color, movement, changing harmonic planes”: Cognition, Constraints and Conceptual Blends in Modernist Music The Pleasure of Modernism: Intention, Meaning, and the Compositional Avant-Garde, ed. Arved Ashby (Rochester: University of Rochester Press, 2004), 121–152. Amy Bauer I. Ligeti and the “Listenability” of Modernist Music György Ligeti has discussed his "micropolyphonic" music of the mid-1960s at some length, in an attempt to explain why its composed structure seems to bear no relation to its actual sound. Although works such as Lontano are based on strict canons, their compositional method assumes a listener will 'mishear' its structure: [In the large orchestral work Lontano] I composed . an extensively branching and yet strictly refined polyphony which, however, veers suddenly into something else. I don’t have a name for it and I don’t want to create a term for it. A kind of complex of tone-color, movement, changing harmonic planes. The polyphonic structure does not actually come through, you cannot hear it; it remains hidden in a microscopic underwater world, to us inaudible. I have retained melodic lines in the process of composition, they are governed by rules as strict as Palestrina's or those of the Flemish school, but the rules of polyphony are worked out by me. the polyphony is dissolved, like the harmony and the tone-color – to such an extent that it does not manifest itself, and yet it is there, just beneath the threshold.1 In the above passages, Ligeti appears to ally himself with modernists such as Boulez and Babbit, composers who use twelve-tone and other methods to systematically organize pitch structure.
    [Show full text]
  • Music and Meaning in the 20Th Century
    Towards the Semantics of Music: the 20th Century1 Mihailo Antovic Faculty of Philosophy, University of Nis, Serbia Abstract This paper discusses the problem of musical meaning from the perspective of some 20th century approaches to linguistic semantics. The text briefly covers the issue as it was viewed in the first half of the previous century, and then reviews some studies of musical meaning within the structural, generative, and cognitive frameworks. The author's opinion is that conceptual metaphor theory, in its search of the conceptualization of music, provides the most solid grounds for the foundation of a true „musico-semantics‟. Key words: language, music, meaning, semantics, cognition. he relationship between language and music has been of interest for centuries. Although T structural („grammatical‟) comparisons have occurred throughout the history of language study, and have been topical in the last twenty odd years, it is the problem of musical meaning that has been central to many theoretical discussions, most notably in the aesthetics of music. In this paper, I will try to outline the principal currents in the modern study of music and meaning, analyzing the issue in the framework of linguistic semantics. To define the problem, I will first elaborate on Bernstein‟s term „musico-linguistics‟ (Bernstein, 1976: 9) and introduce the central question related to „musico-semantics‟: does music have any meaning, and if it does, what is its nature and possible relationship to the same term as used in linguistics? The problem of musical signification emerges from the nature of musical phenomena. While music has no clear reference to extramusical reality, it does provoke psychological reactions in listeners comparable to few other arts.
    [Show full text]