280 TRAUMATIC AMNESIA the Disturbances of Memory

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

280 TRAUMATIC AMNESIA the Disturbances of Memory 280 TRAUMATIC AMNESIA BY W. RITCHIE RUSSELL AND P. W. NATHAN. Page INTRODUCTION 280 POST-TRAUMATIC AMNESIA (P.T.A.) 281 Duration of P.T.A 282 Reduction of P.T.A 283 Delayed P.T.A. and Lucid Interval 283 Islands of Memory in P.T.A 284 Behaviour During P.T.A. 284 Conversation. During P.T.A.—Early Memories of Injury ... ... 285 Lengthening of P.T.A 286 "Visions" Relative to Injury 286 Confabulation 288 Downloaded from RETROGRADE AMNESIA (R.A.) ... ... ... ... ... ... 289 Duration of R.A. 289 AbsenceofR.A 291 Shrinkage of R.A. .... 291 by guest on October 16, 2014 Prolonged R.A 293 Islands in R.A 293 EFFECT OF BARBITURATE HYPNOSIS ON R.A. AND P.T.A. ... ... 295 TRAUMATIC AMNESIA AND TRAUMATIC INTELLECTUAL IMPAIRMENT 297 SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION ... ... ... ... ... ... 298 INTRODUCTION THE disturbances of memory associated with head injury are varied and worthy of special study. The most obvious clinical abnormality which accompanies many head injuries is an instantaneous paralysis of brain function—motor, sensory, reflex and mental. The term concussion is often used to describe this phenomenon. It is obvious that during the period of brain paralysis there can be no registration of surrounding incidents and accordingly there is subsequently a permanent amnesia for events oc- curring during this time. As consciousness returns the mental functions recover as after an anaesthetic, the more complex being the last to return to their normal state. While in slight injuries the stages of recovery occupy only a few minutes, after severe trauma recovery of brain function may occur very gradually during a period of several days or weeks. The stages of recovery, however, are often similar whether they run a brief or prolonged course. The function of memory is usually regained at about the same time as the last trace of confusion disappears, and the period of post- TRAUMATIC AMNESIA 281 traumatic amnesia (P.T.A.) is then at an end. The duration of the P.T.A. can be roughly ascertained, as Russell (1932) has pointed out, by asking the patient at any time after he has finally emerged from the state of con- fusion, when he "came to himself". This end-point, however, is some- times ambiguous, for, as Symonds has emphasized, the patient's first memory after the injury, though clearly described, may be followed by a further period of amnesia for hours or days. In such cases the patient is usually able to record the time or date from which he has continuous memory, and this has been used by us for the measurement of the P.T.A. in the cases to be described. It is thus only in the later stages of mental recovery that the patient becomes sufficiently aware of his surroundings to commit them to memory and thus to end the period of post-traumatic amnesia. POST-TRAUMATIC AMNESIA (P.T.A.) The duration of post-traumatic amnesia (P.T.A.) can only be estimated with accuracy after the patient has recovered from even slight degrees of Downloaded from confusion. This duration of P.T.A. will then usually remain relatively constant and will form a permanent index of the duration, not of un- consciousness, but of impaired consciousness. The P.T.A. therefore pro- by guest on October 16, 2014 vides a useful clinical guide for the physician who may enquire into the case for the first time long after the injury occurred. It is obvious that the duration of P.T.A. will provide some indication of the severity of the general brain commotion caused by the injury, and studies by Symonds and Russell (1943) indicated its usefulness in prognosis. We have again analysed these Service cases with reference to subsequent fitness for full duty in a series followed up successfully through the Unit medical officers. The results are given in Table I and indicate clearly that the prospects of good recovery of efficiency diminish rapidly as the P.T.A. lengthens. TABLE I.—FOLLOW-UP. Duration of Post-Traumatic Amnesia compared with future fitness for full duty —based on follow-up of surviving cases six to twenty-four- months after discharge from hospital. Cases were those admitted in the acute stage and therefore relatively unselected. Only those followed-up successfully are considered. Total cases " Fully duty Duration of admitted to Ret. to duly efficiently " Per cent. P.T.A. hospital from hospital on follow-up of total Nil ' 29' 28 25 86% 1 hour 92 90 80 87% 1-24 hours 73- 70 54 ,,,,7,4% 1-7 days .. 39- 34 23 59% 7 days 41 26 7 17% No record 2 2 1 , 276 250 190 69% 282 W. RITCHIE RUSSELL AND P. W. NATHAN Duration of P.T.A.—This analysis in Table I is concerned with accidental blunt injuries in which the rate of change of velocity to which the skull is subjected probably determines the degree of brain damage (Denny-Brown and Russell, 1941). In gunshot wounds, on the other hand, the brain is often submitted to little acceleration, and as in other penetrating injuries the damage is often quite local. Thus in Table II the TABLE II. Duration of Post-Traumatic Amnesia in a consecutive series of surviving "accidental" and gunshot wound cases admitted to a Military Hospital for head injuries. Duration of Post-Traumatic Amnesia Total Nil < 1 hr. 1-24 hr. 1-7 days > 7 days No record "Accidental" head in- 1,022 99 208 312 231 167 5 jury. — 10% 20% 31% 23% 16% J% Gunshot wounds of the 200 64 -10 22 42 24 6 Brain. — 32% 20% 11% 21% 12% 3% duration of P.T.A. is compared in a series of accidental injuries and of Downloaded from gunshot wounds of the head with dural penetration or skull fracture. No less than 32 per cent, of the gunshot wound cases have no amnesia, and this must be due to a failure of many of these missiles (which, include by guest on October 16, 2014 small bomb fragments) to concuss the brain as a whole. There is also a remarkably small proportion of cases of gunshot wound with P.T.A. of one to twenty-four hours. This is in striking contrast to the "accidental" group and indicates that when gunshot wounds do cause loss of conscious- ness the latter is likely to be either of very short or of much prolonged duration. This can probably be-explained by the assumption that when such wounds are of sufficient severity to cause severe general commotion the associated brain destruction will ensure by its extent or its complica- tions that the P.T.A. is prolonged. TABLE III. Duration of Post-Traumatic Amnesia in a series of surviving cases admitted to hospital in the acute stage and therefore relatively unselected. Duration of P.T.A. Total Nil < 1 hr. 1-24 hr. 1-7 days > 7 days No record Military Hospital cases 331 34 115 91 45 43 3 Percent 10% 35% 27% 14% 13% 1% Russell's (1932) cases. Edinburgh .... 184 80 57 47 43% 31% 25% Gutmann's (1943) cases. Oxford .... 179 118 39 19 3 Percent — 66% 22% 11% 2% TRAUMATIC AMNESIA 283 Table III gives the number of cases in the various P.T.A. groups in an unselected series of acute hospital admissions. The cases were admitted to a Military Hospital for Head Injuries, and were those admitted in the acute stage only; they are compared with figures for general hospitals published by one of us (W. R. R.) and also by Gutmann. There is a close similarity between the figures in the first two groups of cases, while those in Gutmann's series have a larger proportion of slight injuries. It is now generally agreed (Symonds, 1932; Russell, 1932; Greenfield, 1938; Jefferson, 1942) that the disorders of consciousness following head injury are usually dependent on neuronal trauma and are rarely due to vascular complications such as haemorrhage, oedema or increased intra- cranial pressure. Study of the duration of amnesias following injury carries us a step further, for this makes it necessary to postulate many degrees of neuronal commotion which, for example, may recover fully in ten minutes or may "feel their way" gradually to recovery in ten days. Reduction of P.T.A.—Though the duration of P.T.A. is usually more Downloaded from or less permanent and unchanging, there are exceptions. Occasionally it becomes reduced spontaneously as in the following case: Fus. R. sustained a head injury on July 19, 1942. On August 4 he had a by guest on October 16, 2014 momentary retrograde amnesia (R.A.) and a P.T.A. of six days with an "island" on the fourth day. Ten weeks after the accident he regained spontaneously another island in the P.T.A., for while sitting quietly in bed he suddenly remembered being on the floor of a moving truck, and this was. in fact, the way he was brought to hospital. Cases of reduction in P.T.A. under barbiturate hypnosis are described later. These recoveries of things forgotten are reminiscent of hysterical amnesia and yet there is no reason to suppose they are hysterical. Delayed P.T.A. and lucid interval.—In some few cases of accidental head injury all details of the injury and of events which closely followed it are clearly recalled, but there follows later a period of confusion and amnesia. This can be referred to as delayed post-traumatic amnesia. In most cases, especially in gunshot wounds of the brain, the delayed P.T.A.
Recommended publications
  • The Three Amnesias
    The Three Amnesias Russell M. Bauer, Ph.D. Department of Clinical and Health Psychology College of Public Health and Health Professions Evelyn F. and William L. McKnight Brain Institute University of Florida PO Box 100165 HSC Gainesville, FL 32610-0165 USA Bauer, R.M. (in press). The Three Amnesias. In J. Morgan and J.E. Ricker (Eds.), Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis/Psychology Press. The Three Amnesias - 2 During the past five decades, our understanding of memory and its disorders has increased dramatically. In 1950, very little was known about the localization of brain lesions causing amnesia. Despite a few clues in earlier literature, it came as a complete surprise in the early 1950’s that bilateral medial temporal resection caused amnesia. The importance of the thalamus in memory was hardly suspected until the 1970’s and the basal forebrain was an area virtually unknown to clinicians before the 1980’s. An animal model of the amnesic syndrome was not developed until the 1970’s. The famous case of Henry M. (H.M.), published by Scoville and Milner (1957), marked the beginning of what has been called the “golden age of memory”. Since that time, experimental analyses of amnesic patients, coupled with meticulous clinical description, pathological analysis, and, more recently, structural and functional imaging, has led to a clearer understanding of the nature and characteristics of the human amnesic syndrome. The amnesic syndrome does not affect all kinds of memory, and, conversely, memory disordered patients without full-blown amnesia (e.g., patients with frontal lesions) may have impairment in those cognitive processes that normally support remembering.
    [Show full text]
  • POST TRAUMATIC AMNESIA SCREENING and MANAGEMENT GUIDELINE Trauma Service Guidelines Title: Post Traumatic Amnesia Screening and Management Developed By: K
    TRM 01.01 POST TRAUMATIC AMNESIA SCREENING AND MANAGEMENT GUIDELINE Trauma Service Guidelines Title: Post Traumatic Amnesia Screening and Management Developed by: K. Gumm, T, Taylor, K, Orbons, L, Carey, PTA Working Party Created: Version 1.0, April 2007 Revised by: K. Liersch, K. Gumm, E. Hayes, E. Thompson, K. Henderson & Advisory Committee on Trauma Revised: V 5.0 Jun 2020, V 4.0 Oct 2017, V3.0 Mar 2014, V2.0 Apr 2011 Table of Contents What is Post Traumatic Amnesia (PTA)?........................................................................................................................................................1 Screening Criteria for PTA..............................................................................................................................................................................5 Abbreviated Assessment of MBI using the Abbreviated Westmead PTA Scale (A-WPTAS) ..........................................................................5 Discharging a Patient Home with a Mild Brain Injury ....................................................................................................................................6 Moderate and Severe Brain Injury.................................................................................................................................................................7 Assessment of Moderate and Severe Brain Injury using the Westmead PTA Scale (Westmead)..................................................................7 Management of the patient suffering
    [Show full text]
  • Partners Healthcare: Clinical Pathway for Imaging in Mild Traumatic Brain Injury
    Partners Healthcare: Clinical Pathway for Imaging in Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Please answer the following 3 questions, selecting all of the responses that apply: QUESTION 1: Does the patient have mild traumatic brain injury (MTBI)? Injury to the head, resulting from blunt trauma or acceleration or deceleration forces, AND, ONE OR MORE OF THE FOLLOWING conditions attributable to the head injury: Transient confusion, disorientation, or impaired consciousness Dysfunction of memory (amnesia) around the time of injury Observed signs of other neurologic or neuropsychological dysfunction Loss of consciousness lasting 30 minutes or less QUESTION 2: Is the patient eligible for the clinical pathway? Exclusion Criteria: The patient must have NONE of the following: Penetrating head trauma GCS score < 15 on initial evaluation Age < 16 years Presenting to ED more than 24‐hours after injury Severe Multisystem Trauma, defined as: Hemodynamic instability (HR > 120, SBP <90) Major trauma with suspected or proven life‐threatening injuries QUESTION 3: Did the patient have loss of consciousness (LOC) or post‐traumatic amnesia? YES NO Head CT is unlikely to be helpful UNLESS the Head CT is unlikely to be helpful UNLESS the patient has one or more of the following: patient has one or more of the following: Focal neurologic deficit Focal neurologic deficit Coagulopathy Coagulopathy Vomiting Vomiting GCS score < 15 GCS score < 15 Age > 60 years Age 65 years Headache Severe headache Physical evidence of trauma above the clavicles
    [Show full text]
  • Consolidation Theory and Retrograde Amnesia in Humans
    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2002, 9 (3), 403-425 THEORETICAL AND REVIEW ARTICLES Consolidation theory and retrograde amnesia in humans ALAN S. BROWN Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas Recent researchon the cognitive dysfunctions experienced by human anmesic patients indicates that very long term (multidecade) changes may occur in memory. Flat retrograde amnesia (RA), consisting of a uniform memory deficit for information from all preamnesia time periods, indicates a simple, mono- lithic retrievalproblem, whereas graded RA, with greater memory deficits for information from recent as opposed to remote time periods, suggests the presence of a gradual long-term encoding, or consol- idation, process. An evaluation of 247 outcomes from 61 articlesprovides strong evidence of graded RA across different cerebral injuries, materials, and test procedures, as well as in measures of both ab- solute and relative(patientvs. control) performance.Future conceptualizationsof human memory should address the possibility that memories increase in resistanceto forgetting,or reduction in trace fragility, across many decades. The concept of consolidation was introduced over a A major problem with integrating the concept of con- centuryago by Müller and Pilzecker(1900; see also Burn- solidationinto mainstream cognitivepsychologyis the dif- ham, 1903).While Müller and Pilzecker (1900) originally ficulty of a direct empirical test in humans. Whereas ani- conceivedof consolidationoccurringovershort periods of mal research has repeatedlyevaluatedconsolidationtheory
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Reactivation and Consolidation During Sleep Ken A
    Sleep & Memory/Review Memory reactivation and consolidation during sleep Ken A. Paller1 and Joel L. Voss Institute for Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208-2710, USA Do our memories remain static during sleep, or do they change? We argue here that memory change is not only a natural result of sleep cognition, but further, that such change constitutes a fundamental characteristic of declarative memories. In general, declarative memories change due to retrieval events at various times after initial learning and due to the formation and elaboration of associations with other memories, including memories formed after the initial learning episode. We propose that declarative memories change both during waking and during sleep, and that such change contributes to enhancing binding of the distinct representational components of some memories, and thus to a gradual process of cross-cortical consolidation. As a result of this special form of consolidation, declarative memories can become more cohesive and also more thoroughly integrated with other stored information. Further benefits of this memory reprocessing can include developing complex networks of interrelated memories, aligning memories with long-term strategies and goals, and generating insights based on novel combinations of memory fragments. A variety of research findings are consistent with the hypothesis that cross-cortical consolidation can progress during sleep, although further support is needed, and we suggest some potentially fruitful research directions. Determining how processing during sleep can facilitate memory storage will be an exciting focus of research in the coming years. The idea that memory storage is supported by events that take otherwise intellectually intact.
    [Show full text]
  • BHS 499-07 Memory and Amnesia
    BHS 499-07 Memory and Amnesia Memory Disorders Influences on Memory z Alcohol – Bits & Pieces z Stress -- Kolb & Whishaw Seg 32 (CD 2) z Diabetes – Kolb & Whishaw Ch 13 Seg 6 (CD 3) Kinds of Memory Disorders z Organic – having a physical cause z Functional – having a psychological cause z Dys (as a prefix) means difficulty or limited ability to perform. z A (as a prefix) means complete inability or lack of a function. Alcohol & Memory z Alcoholic amnesia – alcohol prevents consolidation so nothing is remembered and no memory can be recovered. z Alcoholic blackout – state-dependent memory, so recall is possible if one is back in the same state. z Because many crimes are committed while drunk, memory failure is frequently blamed on alcohol. Sleep & Memory z New sleep studies suggest a "memory life- cycle” with three stages - stabilization, consolidation, and re-consolidation. • Initial stabilization takes up to 6 hours. • Sleep needed for consolidation, deep non-REM • Alcohol disrupts consolidation z Sleep deprivation produces affects similar to aging. • Procedural memory and recognition memory are most strongly affected. Sources of Organic Dysfunction z Accident • Car accidents and other injuries (e.g., N.A.) • War z Disease • Encephalitis (viral) – inflammation of the lining of the brain, causing swelling. • Stroke • Alzheimer’s disease • Korsakov’s syndrome (prolonged alcoholism) Alzheimer’s Disease z A fatal degenerative disease caused by cell failure – neurofibrillary tangles and plaques that interfere with cell function. • All areas of the brain are eventually affected, but frontal lobes and memory go first. z Confusions and memory problems do not resemble normal aging, amnesia or other memory problems.
    [Show full text]
  • Severely Deficient Autobiographical Memory (SDAM)
    Neuropsychologia 72 (2015) 105–118 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia Severely deficient autobiographical memory (SDAM) in healthy adults: A new mnemonic syndrome Daniela J. Palombo a,b,1, Claude Alain a,b, Hedvig Söderlund c, Wayne Khuu a,b, Brian Levine a,b,d,n a Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, ON, Canada M6A 2E1 b Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada M5S 1A1 c Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala 751 42, Sweden d Department of Medicine (Neurology), University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada M5S 1A1 article info abstract Article history: Recollection of previously experienced events is a key element of human memory that entails recovery Received 13 January 2015 of spatial, perceptual, and mental state details. While deficits in this capacity in association with brain Received in revised form disease have serious functional consequences, little is known about individual differences in autobio- 7 April 2015 graphical memory (AM) in healthy individuals. Recently, healthy adults with highly superior autobio- Accepted 12 April 2015 graphical capacities have been identified (e.g., LePort, A.K., Mattfeld, A.T., Dickinson-Anson, H., Fallon, J. Available online 17 April 2015 H., Stark, C.E., Kruggel, F., McGaugh, J.L., 2012. Behavioral and neuroanatomical investigation of Highly Keywords: Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM). Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 98(1), 78–92. doi: 10.1016/j. Episodic memory nlm.2012.05.002). Here we report data from three healthy, high functioning adults with the reverse Autobiographical memory pattern: lifelong severely deficient autobiographical memory (SDAM) with otherwise preserved Hippocampus cognitive function.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory and Metamemory: a Study of the Feeling-Of-Knowing Phenomenon in Amnesic Patients
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyrighl 1986 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Learning, Memory, and Cognition O278-7393/86/SO0.75 1986, Vol. 12, No. 3,452-460 Memory and Metamemory: A Study of the Feeling-of-Knowing Phenomenon in Amnesic Patients Arthur P. Shimamura and Larry R. Squire Veterans Administration Medical Center, San Diego and Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine Accuracy of the feeling of knowing was tested in patients with KorsakofTs syndrome, patients pre- scribed electroconvulsive therapy, four other cases of amnesia, and control subjects. In Experiment 1, we tested feeling-of-knowing accuracy for the answers to general information questions that could not be recalled. Subjects were asked to rank nonrecalled questions in terms of how likely they thought they would be able to recognize the answers and were then given a recognition test for these items. Only patients with KorsakofTs syndrome were impaired in making feeling-of-knowing predictions. The other amnesic patients were as accurate as control subjects in their feeling-of-knowing predic- tions. In Experiment 2, we replicated these findings in a sentence memory paradigm that tested newly learned information. The results showed that impaired metamemory is not an obligatory feature of amnesia, because amnesia can occur without detectable metamemory deficits. The im- paired metamemory exhibited by patients with KorsakofTs syndrome reflects a cognitive impair- ment that is not typically observed in other forms of amnesia. Often one experiences a sense or feeling of knowing some effects (Diamond & Rozin, 1984; Graf, Squire, & Mandler, information without being able to recall it.
    [Show full text]
  • The Relationship Between Anterograde and Retrograde Amnesia in Alcoholic Wernicke-Korsakoff Syndrome1
    Psychological Medicine, 1991, 21, 11-14 Printed in Great Britain EDITORIAL The relationship between anterograde and retrograde amnesia in alcoholic Wernicke-Korsakoff Syndrome1 The Wernicke-Korsakoff Syndrome (WKS) can be described as a constellation of neuro- psychological deficits the most prominent of which is a profound amnesic syndrome. The neurological impairment underlying WKS is directly attributable to thiamine deficiency, which has the consequential effect of Wernicke's encephalopathy, resulting in vascular lesions of the midline diencephalic nuclei (Victor et al. 1989). The most common aetiology of this thiamine deficiency is chronic alcoholism, although it must be emphasized that the same form of neurological impairment can arise in other forms of nutritional disorder (e.g. Becker el al. 1990). WKS patients exhibit both a severe anterograde amnesia, manifest by poor performance on memory tasks such as free recall and recognition, and retrograde amnesia, as shown by an inability to recall or recognize information relating to the pre-morbid period. Reviews of WKS indicate that the co-occurrence of anterograde and retrograde amnesia is a persistent feature of the syndrome (Parkin & Leng, 1988). Indeed, to my knowledge, there is no well-described instance of WKS in which this relationship has not been reported. This regular state of affairs leads directly to the possibility that the anterograde and retrograde deficits may have a common origin. The consistent association of WKS with chronic alcoholism has been invoked in attempts to provide a single explanation for the anterograde and retrograde deficits associated with WKS. This view, which has been termed the continuity hypothesis (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Consolidation
    Memory Consolidation Larry R. Squire1,2,3, Lisa Genzel4, John T. Wixted3, and Richard G. Morris4 1VA San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, California 92161 2Departments of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093 3Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093 4Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, United Kingdom Correspondence: [email protected] Conscious memory for a new experience is initially dependent on information stored in both the hippocampus and neocortex. Systems consolidation is the process by which the hippo- campus guides the reorganization of the information stored in the neocortex such that it eventually becomes independent of the hippocampus. Early evidence for systems consoli- dation was provided by studies of retrograde amnesia, which found that damage to the hippocampus-impaired memories formed in the recent past, but typically spared memories formed in the more remote past. Systems consolidation has been found to occur for both episodic and semantic memories and for both spatial and nonspatial memories, although empirical inconsistencies and theoretical disagreements remain about these issues. Recent work has begun to characterize the neural mechanisms that underlie the dialogue between the hippocampus and neocortex (e.g., “neural replay,” which occurs during sharp wave ripple activity). New work has also identified variables, such as the amount of preexisting knowledge, that affect the rate of consolidation. The increasing use of molecular genetic tools (e.g., optogenetics) can be expected to further improve understanding of the neural mech- anisms underlying consolidation. emory consolidation refers to the process Ribot (1881).
    [Show full text]
  • Three Cases of Enduring Memory Impairment After Bilateral Damage Limited to the Hippocampal Formation
    The Journal of Neuroscience, August 15, 1996, 16(16):5233–5255 Three Cases of Enduring Memory Impairment after Bilateral Damage Limited to the Hippocampal Formation Nancy L. Rempel-Clower,3 Stuart M. Zola,1,2,3 Larry R. Squire,1,2,3 and David G. Amaral4 1Veterans Affairs Medical Center, San Diego, California 92161, Departments of 2Psychiatry and 3Neurosciences, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, and 4Department of Psychiatry and Center for Neuroscience, University of California at Davis, Davis, California 95616 Patient RB (Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: anterograde memory impairment. (2) Bilateral damage beyond enduring memory impairment following a bilaterial lesion limited the CA1 region, but still limited to the hippocampal formation, to field CA1 of the hippocampus, S. Zola-Morgan, L. R. Squire, can produce more severe anterograde memory impairment. (3) and D. G. Amaral, 1986, J Neurosci 6:2950–2967) was the first Extensive, temporally graded retrograde amnesia covering 15 reported case of human amnesia in which detailed neuropsy- years or more can occur after damage limited to the hippo- chological analyses and detailed postmortem neuropathologi- campal formation. Findings from studies with experimental an- cal analyses demonstrated that damage limited to the hip- imals are consistent with the findings from amnesic patients. pocampal formation was sufficient to produce anterograde The present results substantiate the idea that severity of mem- memory impairment. Neuropsychological and postmortem ory impairment is dependent on locus and extent of damage neuropathological findings are described here for three addi- within the hippocampal formation and that damage to the tional amnesic patients with bilateral damage limited to the hippocampal formation can cause temporally graded retro- hippocampal formation.
    [Show full text]
  • An Emotion-Induced Retrograde Amnesia in Humans Is Amygdala- and ␤-Adrenergic-Dependent
    An emotion-induced retrograde amnesia in humans is amygdala- and ␤-adrenergic-dependent B. A. Strange*†‡, R. Hurlemann§, and R. J. Dolan* *Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, Institute of Neurology, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, United Kingdom; †Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom; and §Department of Neurology II, Otto von Guericke University, Leipziger Strasse 44, 39120 Magdeburg, Germany Communicated by Endel Tulving, Rotman Research Institute of Baycrest Centre, North York, ON, Canada, August 10, 2003 (received for review May 20, 2003) The influence of emotion on human memory is associated with two A previously reported blockade of enhanced memory for contradictory effects in the form of either emotion-induced en- emotional stimuli by propranolol (2) indicates central noradren- hancements or decrements in memory. In a series of experiments aline͞norepinephrine (NE) release in response to emotional involving single word presentation, we show that enhanced mem- stimuli. In animals, posttraining amygdala stimulation, electri- ory for emotional words is strongly coupled to decrements in cally or with adrenergic agonists, typically leads to enhanced memory for items preceding the emotional stimulus, an effect that memory for previously learned tasks but can evoke memory is more pronounced in women. These memory effects would impairment (12, 13). For example, retrograde amnesia in rodents appear to depend on a common neurobiological substrate, in that for visual discrimination learning is produced by postlearning enhancements and decrements are reversed by propranolol, a stimulation of the amygdala (14), an effect attenuated by pre- ␤-adrenergic antagonist, and abolished by selective bilateral treatment with propranolol. Hence amygdala stimulation- amygdala damage.
    [Show full text]