An Optimality-Theoretic Analysis of Navajo Sibilant Harmony

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

An Optimality-Theoretic Analysis of Navajo Sibilant Harmony An optimality-theoretic analysis of Navajo sibilant harmony Item Type text; Article Authors Oberly, Stacey Publisher University of Arizona Linguistics Circle (Tucson, Arizona) Journal Coyote Papers Rights Copyright © is held by the author(s). Download date 03/10/2021 14:21:12 Item License http://rightsstatements.org/vocab/InC/1.0/ Link to Item http://hdl.handle.net/10150/126386 An Optimality-Theoretic Analysis of Navajo Sibilant Harmony Stacey Oberly University of Arizona Abstract This paper presents an Optimality-Theoretic analysis (Prince and Smolensky, 1993) of sibilant harmony in Navajo. This Optimality- Theoretic (OT) analysis uses correspondence theory (McCarthy and Prince, 1995) to account for changes in the [ anterior] feature in coro- ± nal segments in the verbal conjunct domain. Specifically, the place of articulation of the rightmost coronal fricative segment determines the place of articulation of all other coronal fricatives in the ver- bal conjunct domain via Ident, Agree and Max constraints. This OT analysis is innovative in that it posits a constraint that protects pronominal-argument morphemes from deletion. 1 Introduction This discussion presents an Optimality Theoretic analysis (Prince and Smo- lensky, 1993) of sibilant harmony in Navajo using correspondence theory. In correspondence theory (McCarthy and Prince, 1995), Gen produces a candidate set with a correspondence function expressing the dependence of the output on the input. Eval considers each candidate pair S1, S2 and its correspondence function. In Navajo, verbs are made up of three parts: the disjunct domain, conjunct domain and verb stem. Coronal fricative segments in the conjunct domain and verb stem must agree in regards to the [anterior] feature. The feature [anterior] distinguishes coronal segments produced at or in front on the alveolar ridge [+anterior] and further behind the alveolar ridge [-anterior]. That is, the place of articulation of the right-most coronal fricative segment determines the place of articulation for all other coronal fricatives in the verbal conjunct. 35 Stacey Oberly This OT analysis is innovative in that is posits a constraint which bans the deletion of argument morphemes represented as segments. That is, sub- ject and object arguments, which are prefixed to the verb stem, are never deleted to satisfy phonological constraints. The data suggest that phonology, morphology and syntax are inter-related and should not be separated into discrete areas. Navajo provides an excellent testing ground for phonological, morphological and syntactic theoretic analysis. This analysis consists of eight sections. Section 2 briefly discusses Navajo and the Athabaskan language family. Section 3 provides a sketch of the Navajo verb. Sections 4 and 5 discuss the distribution of Navajo consonants and vowels in the three domains of the verb. Section 6 presents sibilant harmony data in-depth followed by section 7 which consists of an OT analysis of sibilant harmony. Finally, section 8 provides concluding remarks. 2 Navajo Language Background Navajo is a member of the large Athabaskan language family. It is considered a pronominal argument language, which in effect states that the pronomi- nal affixes on the verb are the verbal arguments and the overt nominals are adjuncts Jelinek (1984, 1993); Jelinek and Demers (1994); Jelinek (1995); Je- linek and Demers (1997); Jelinek (2000). Given that Navajo is a pronominal argument language its verb structure is complex. There are several dialects of Navajo spoken in the southwestern United States. The number of Navajo speakers is declining. See the Introduction in this volume for more detailed background information. 3 The Navajo Verb A Navajo verb surfaces as minimally disyllabic. The Navajo verb consists of three domains as shown in table 1: disjunct, conjunct and verb stem.1 Young and Morgan (1987) posit the tripartite templatic model of the Navajo verb structure used in this analysis. The pound-sign, #, separates the disjunct from the conjunct. 1There are varied interpretations of what morphemes constitute the three domains of the Navajo verbs. For an alternate view see Faltz (1998). 36 An OT Analysis of Navajo Sibilant Harmony ObjectNull of Postposition PostpositionPostpositional:ReflexiveReversionary—returning AdverbialIterativeDistributive Thematic: modeDO Pronominal Nominalback PluralSubjectThematic/Adverbial Pronoun:Thematic/Adverbial Non-localAdverbial-ThematicModal elements ConjugationelementsSubjectClassifier Pronoun: Marker/PegStem Local 0 Ia Ib Ic Id II III IV V VI VI VI VI VII IX X abc I I Disjunct Conjunct Stem Table 1: The Structure of the Navajo Verb (Young and Morgan, 1987) Four Navajo verbs are shown in (1–4). It is important to note that there are a number of phonological alternations in the language other than those that will be discussed in depth here. (1) Intransitive Verb Postition: VII VIII X UR: / -sh -cha/ PR: yi -sh -cha peg peg -1s -cry ‘I cry’ (McDonough, 2003, p.42) Navajo verbs are formed exclusively (with few exceptions) through prefixa- tion. In example (1), the underlying form consists of the first person sin- gle pronoun, sh, and the verb stem, cha, ‘cry.’ Without epenthesis, this form would not be allowed to surface due to the disyllabic minimum. The epenthetic consonant and vowel, yi, also called the peg is inserted to meet the disyllabic condition. 37 Stacey Oberly (2) Intransitive Verb2 Postition: VI VII IX X UR: /y´a -sh - l -ti’/ PR: y´a -sh -ti’ peg preverb -1s.S -Cl -speak ‘I speak’ (Hale, 2001, p.685) In example (2), the preverb, ya, appears in position VI followed by the subjective marker, sh, the classifier, l, and finally the verb stem, ti’, ‘speak.’ The classifier does not surface faithfully. (3) Intransitive Verb3 Postition: IV VIII IX X UR: /sh -j - l -ghozh/ PR: shi -ji - l -hozh 1s.O 3a.S Cl -tickle ‘s/he/they/one is tickling me’ (Hale, 2001, p.698) In example (3), the transitive verb consists of four morphemes. The first person object pronoun, shi, appears on the left edge of the verb followed by the third person pronoun, j, the classifier, l, and finally the verb stem, ghozh, ‘speak.’ (4) Transitive Verb with Disjunct Domain Postition: I #VIII X UR: /ch’´ı -nish -ts’´o´od/ PR: ch’´ı -nis -ts’´o´od out -ni-imp.1S -stretch ‘I lean out’ (McDonough, 2003, p.53) In example (4), the transitive verb consists of three morphemes. The adver- bial, ch´ı, appears on the left edge of the verb followed by the first person pronoun ni-imperfection morpheme, nish, and finally the verb stem, ts’´o´od, 2Cl is a classifier which marks valence or voice. 3“O” is for object. 38 An OT Analysis of Navajo Sibilant Harmony ‘stretch.’ Notice the [-anterior] adverbial, ch’´ı, does not change it anteriority to agree with the [+anterior] verb stem, ts’´o´od, ‘stretch’ while the [-anterior] first person pronoun ni-imperfection morpheme, nish, does. 3.1 The Verb Stem The Navajo verb stem exhibits most consonantal contrasts. The stem is a prominent position in Navajo phonology. The verb stem appears in the far right position of the verb. Most verb stems consist of one syllable and begin with a consonant (Faltz, 1998). This is shown in the verb stems in (1–4) repeated here as (5). (5) Navajo Verb Stems a. -cha -cry b.-ti’ -speak c. -hozh -speak d. - ts’´o´od -stretch Notice that three out of the four verb stems have codas. 3.2 The Conjunct The conjunct can consist of eight morphemes to the left of the verb stem. These morphemes are obligatory in Navajo. It is important to notice that two locations are marked for the obligatory subject prefix: V and VIII. Local subject pronouns that are in the first and second person appear in position VIII, while non-local subject pronouns, third and fourth persons, appear in position V. The epenthetic consonant, y, and vowel, i, (also called the peg) appear in position VIII. The consonants that surface in the conjunct are limited and many phonological alternations occur in this domain. This will be further discussed in Section 4.4.1. 3.3 The Disjunct The disjunct domain can consist of seven morphemes which appear to the left of the conjunct and verb stem. All of the disjunct morphemes are not obligatorily present although some verb stems may require certain disjunct morphemes. Morphemes in the disjunct are more loosely attached to the 39 Stacey Oberly Labial Alveolar Palatal Lateral Velar Glottal Stops Unaspirated b d g ’ Aspirated t k Glottalized t’ k’ Affricates Unaspirated dz j tl Aspirated ts ch t l Glottalized ts’ ch’ t l’ Fricative Voiced z zh l gh h Voiceless s sh l x Nasals Plain m n Glides w y Table 2: Navajo consonant chart, after Young and Morgan (1987) verb than the conjunct morphemes, meaning these morphemes may not be mandatory. The vowels that appear in the disjunct are less likely to be epenthetic. The disjunct morphemes are less likely to undergo phonological alternations than the conjunct morphemes. 4 Navajo Consonants Like other Athabaskan languages, Navajo has a large consonant inventory. Written in standard Navajo orthography, the consonant inventory is shown in table 2. Notice that out of thirty-two consonants, nineteen consonants are coronal making manner and laryngeal contrasts important. 4.1 Bilabials The bilabials (b, m) are limited to the positions shown in 6. (6) Distribution of Bilabials a. only word-intial (b) b. stem inital (b & m) (McDonough, 2003, p.42) 40 An OT Analysis of Navajo Sibilant Harmony 4.2 Stops There is a three-way contrast among the stops (except bilabials) in manner and release as shown in (7). (7) Three-Way Contrast in Stops a. Manner: Plain Aspirated Ejective/Glottalized b. Release: Plain Fricative Laterals (McDonough, 2003, p.42) Stops resist phonological alternations (McDonough, 2003). 4.3 Fricatives The fricatives are divided into four sets as shown in (8).
Recommended publications
  • A Typology of Consonant Agreement As Correspondence
    A TYPOLOGY OF CONSONANT AGREEMENT AS CORRESPONDENCE SHARON ROSE RACHEL WALKER University of California, San Diego University of Southern California This article presents a typology of consonant harmony or LONG DISTANCE CONSONANT AGREEMENT that is analyzed as arisingthroughcorrespondence relations between consonants rather than feature spreading. The model covers a range of agreement patterns (nasal, laryngeal, liquid, coronal, dorsal) and offers several advantages. Similarity of agreeing consonants is central to the typology and is incorporated directly into the constraints drivingcorrespondence. Agreementby correspon- dence without feature spreadingcaptures the neutrality of interveningsegments,which neither block nor undergo. Case studies of laryngeal agreement and nasal agreement are presented, demon- stratingthe model’s capacity to capture varyingdegreesof similarity crosslinguistically.* 1. INTRODUCTION. The action at a distance that is characteristic of CONSONANT HAR- MONIES stands as a pivotal problem to be addressed by phonological theory. Consider the nasal alternations in the Bantu language, Kikongo (Meinhof 1932, Dereau 1955, Webb 1965, Ao 1991, Odden 1994, Piggott 1996). In this language, the voiced stop in the suffix [-idi] in la is realized as [ini] in 1b when preceded by a nasal consonant at any distance in the stem constituent, consistingof root and suffixes. (1) a. m-[bud-idi]stem ‘I hit’ b. tu-[kun-ini]stem ‘we planted’ n-[suk-idi]stem ‘I washed’ tu-[nik-ini]stem ‘we ground’ In addition to the alternation in 1, there are no Kikongo roots containing a nasal followed by a voiced stop, confirmingthat nasal harmony or AGREEMENT, as we term it, also holds at the root level as a MORPHEME STRUCTURE CONSTRAINT (MSC).
    [Show full text]
  • An Optimality-Theoretic Analysis of Navajo Sibilant Harmony
    An optimality-theoretic analysis of Navajo sibilant harmony Item Type text; Article Authors Oberly, Stacey Publisher University of Arizona Linguistics Circle (Tucson, Arizona) Journal Coyote Papers Rights Copyright © is held by the author(s). Download date 01/10/2021 17:26:00 Item License http://rightsstatements.org/vocab/InC/1.0/ Link to Item http://hdl.handle.net/10150/126386 An Optimality-Theoretic Analysis of Navajo Sibilant Harmony Stacey Oberly University of Arizona Abstract This paper presents an Optimality-Theoretic analysis (Prince and Smolensky, 1993) of sibilant harmony in Navajo. This Optimality- Theoretic (OT) analysis uses correspondence theory (McCarthy and Prince, 1995) to account for changes in the [ anterior] feature in coro- ± nal segments in the verbal conjunct domain. Specifically, the place of articulation of the rightmost coronal fricative segment determines the place of articulation of all other coronal fricatives in the ver- bal conjunct domain via Ident, Agree and Max constraints. This OT analysis is innovative in that it posits a constraint that protects pronominal-argument morphemes from deletion. 1 Introduction This discussion presents an Optimality Theoretic analysis (Prince and Smo- lensky, 1993) of sibilant harmony in Navajo using correspondence theory. In correspondence theory (McCarthy and Prince, 1995), Gen produces a candidate set with a correspondence function expressing the dependence of the output on the input. Eval considers each candidate pair S1, S2 and its correspondence function. In Navajo, verbs are made up of three parts: the disjunct domain, conjunct domain and verb stem. Coronal fricative segments in the conjunct domain and verb stem must agree in regards to the [anterior] feature.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Significance of Eloise Jelinek's Pronominal Argument Hypothesis
    On the Significance of Eloise Jelinek's Pronominal Argument Hypothesis Ken Hale Massachusetts Institute of Technology 0. Introduction In 1984, Eloise Jelinek proposed a theory of Navajo grammar designed to account for certain observations which indicated that the organization of clauses in the language was "nonconfigurational" in a very particular sense (Jelinek, 1984). Her framework has since become widely known, and the name Pronominal Argument (PA) is now attached to a wide range of polysynthetic "head-marking languages" (cf. Baker, 1996; Nichols, 1986). She has written a number of works developing the PA model in relation not only to Southern Athabaskan but to Salish as well (e.g., Jelinek and Demers, 1994). In this essay, I will review certain aspects of the PA hypothesis, with emphasis on its relevance (i) to the grammar of Navajo and (ii) to the grammar of noun incorporation. In past work on the Navajo verb (e.g., Hale 2000b), I assumed that the surface organization of the elements appearing within it was due to a 2 KENNETH HALE series of movement rules — applications of Head Movement or Verb Raising. The verb began its upward journey from a position low in the syntactic structure of a sentence, moved from head to head within its extended projection, leaving certain residue (nonnuclear elements). In the first step of this scenario, the verb adjoins to the right edge of the first functional head above it (the Voice marker, traditionally called the Classifier in Athabaskan linguistics). In the second step, the classifier (now burdened with the verb) right-adjoins to Mode (an element similar in function to Infl or Tense in more familiar languages, representing the major aspectual, temporal, and mood oppositions in the language).
    [Show full text]
  • A Gestural Account of the Velar Fricative in Navajo
    A gestural account of the velar fricative in Navajo Khalil isKarousa, Joyce McDonoughb and D. h. Whalenc aUniversity of Southern California and Haskins Laboratories bUniversity of Rochester cCUNY Graduate Center and Haskins Laboratories Abstract Using the framework of Articulatory Phonology, we offer a phonological account of the allophonic variation undergone by the velar fricative phoneme in Navajo, a Southern or Apachean Athabaskan language spoken in Arizona and New Mexico. The Navajo velar fricative strongly co-articulates with the following vowel, vary- ing in both place and manner of articulation. The variation in this velar fricative seems greater than the variation of velars in many well-studied languages. The coronal central fricatives in the inventory, in contrast, are quite phonetically sta- ble. The back fricative of Navajo thus highlights 1) the linguistic use of an extreme form of coarticulation and 2) the mechanism by which languages can control coarticulation. It is argued that the task dynamic model underlying Articulatory Phonology, with the mechanism of gestural blending controlling coarticulation, can account for the multiplicity of linguistically-controlled ways in which velars coarticulate with surrounding vowels without requiring any changes of input spec- ification due to context. The ability of phonological and morphological constraints to restrict the amount of coarticulation argues against strict separation of phonet- ics and phonology. 1. Introduction There have been two major themes in the laboratory phonology approach to linguistic sound patterns. The first is a concern with the effects of language use, production, and perception on linguistic structure, and the second is the investigation of how linguistic knowledge affects motor and perceptual pat- terns (Pierrehumbert et al.
    [Show full text]
  • A Bibliography of Navajo and Native American Teaching Materials = Dine K'eeji Naaltsoos Bee Nida'nitinigii
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 232 801 RC 014 198 AUTHOR McCarty, T. L., Comp.; And Others TITLE A Bibliography of Navajo and Native AmericanTeaching Materials = Dine K'eeji Naaltsoos BeeNida'nitinigii. Revised Edition. INSTITUTION Rough Rock Demonstration School,AZ. Navajo Curriculum Center. SPONS AGENCY Office of Indian Education (ED), Washington,D.C. REPORT NO ISBN-0-936008-15-6 PUB DATE Jun 83 NOTE 106p. AVAILABLE FROMNavajo Curriculum Center, RoughRock Demonstration School, Star Route 1, Rough Rock AZ86503 ($7.50; 5 or more, 15% discount). PUB TYPE Reference Materials- Bibliographies (131) EDRS PRICE MF01 Plus Postage. PC Not Availablefrom EDRS. DESCRIPTORS American Indian Culture; *AmericanIndian Education; American Indian History; American IndianLiterature; American Indians; Annotated Bibliographies; Audiovisual Aids; Bilingual InstructionalMaterials; Cultural Background; *Cultural Education;Elementary Secondary Education; *InstructionalMaterials; Mathematics; Music; Native LanguageInstruction; *Navajo; Periodicals; Physical Education;Publishing Industry; *Resource Materials; SocialStudies; SupplemerCzary Reading Materials IDENTIFIERS *Navajo (Nation) ABSTRACT A revised annotated bibliography of Navajoand Native American teaching materials publishedbetween 1910 and 1982 (most from 1970 to 1982), compiledas part of the Title IV-B Navajo Materials Development Project, listsresources for teachers of Navajo and other Native American students.Most citations are of written materials, although some posters,non-textual materials and catalogs of audio-visual aidsare described. The first two sections divide text materials by grade level and by language:148 written primarily in Navajo (Section I), and 193 bilingual(Navajo-English) or primarily in English (Section II).Both sections cite: fiction and non-fiction; Navajo-based texts in socialstudies, history, mathematics, physical education, musicand art; and teacher's guides to accompany texts.
    [Show full text]
  • Copyright by Leighton Craig Peterson 2006
    Copyright by Leighton Craig Peterson 2006 The Dissertation Committee for Leighton Craig Peterson certifies that this is the approved version of the following dissertation: TECHNOLOGY, IDEOLOGY, AND EMERGENT COMMUNICATIVE PRACTICES AMONG THE NAVAJO Committee: _________________________________ Joel Sherzer, Co-Supervisor _________________________________ Pauline Turner Strong, Co-Supervisor _________________________________ Samuel Wilson _________________________________ Elizabeth Keating _________________________________ Keith Walters TECHNOLOGY, IDEOLOGY, AND EMERGENT COMMUNICATIVE PRACTICES AMONG THE NAVAJO by Leighton Craig Peterson, B.A.; M.A. Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of the University of Texas at Austin in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy The University of Texas at Austin August 2006 Acknowledgments There are numerous people who both supported me personally and participated directly and indirectly in the planning, research, and writing of this dissertation. First and foremost, I’d like to thank my extended relations on the Rez and off, including the Halonas, the Nalwoods, the Walters, and the Bitonis. I owe an especially large debt to both Oswald Werner and Tazbah McCullah, who supported and nurtured both me and my work on the Navajo Nation from the beginning, many years ago. Thank you. I would also like to thank my many other consultants and colleagues (most of whom shall remain anonymous, but I hope you know who you are), including Bennie Klain, Linda
    [Show full text]
  • Proquest Dissertations
    An optimality theoretic account of Navajo prefixal syllables Item Type text; Dissertation-Reproduction (electronic) Authors Fountain, Amy Velita, 1963- Publisher The University of Arizona. Rights Copyright © is held by the author. Digital access to this material is made possible by the University Libraries, University of Arizona. Further transmission, reproduction or presentation (such as public display or performance) of protected items is prohibited except with permission of the author. Download date 05/10/2021 11:02:24 Link to Item http://hdl.handle.net/10150/288795 INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced froni die niicro£Qm master. UMI fikos the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in ^pewriter fiice, while others may be from any type of computer printer. Hie qnality of this repiodnction is d^endoit Qpon the quality of the copy snbinitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard maTgiwc and in^n-oper alignment can adverse^ affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are miissing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unautharized copyright material had to be removed, a note win indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photogr^hed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. Photogrs^hs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy.
    [Show full text]
  • Motivating Morphological Constituents: a Learning-Based Analysis of Hebrew, Navajo, and Spanish
    City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works All Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects 9-2017 Motivating Morphological Constituents: A Learning-Based Analysis of Hebrew, Navajo, and Spanish Ignacio Lorenzo Montoya The Graduate Center, City University of New York How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/gc_etds/2163 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] MOTIVATING MORPHOLOGICAL CONSTITUENTS: A LEARNING-BASED ANALYSIS OF HEBREW, NAVAJO, AND SPANISH by IGNACIO L. MONTOYA A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty in Linguistics in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, The City University of New York 2017 © 2017 IGNACIO L. MONTOYA All Rights Reserved ii Motivating Morphological Constituents: A Learning-Based Analysis of Hebrew, Navajo, and Spanish by Ignacio L. Montoya This manuscript has been read and accepted for the Graduate Faculty in Linguistics in satisfaction of the dissertation requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Juliette Blevins ____________________________________________ Chair of Examining Committee Gita Martohardjono ____________________________________________ Executive Officer Supervisory Committee: Daniel Kaufman Ricardo Otheguy James P. Blevins THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK iii ABSTRACT Motivating Morphological Constituents: A Learning-Based Analysis of Hebrew, Navajo, and Spanish by Ignacio L. Montoya Advisor: Juliette Blevins In both description and theory, a variety of linguistic structures have been posited to account for a multitude of linguistic patterns. In the sub-field of morphology, such units include roots, stems, and morphemes.
    [Show full text]
  • Academic Catalog 2019-2020
    UNIVERSITY OF NEW MEXICO GOVERNING BOARD UNM Board of Regents Sandra K. Begay, Albuquerque Douglas M. Brown, Albuquerque Robert M. Doughty III, Albuquerque UNM GALLUP LOCAL BOARD Melissa C. Henry, Albuquerque Ralph Richards, Member Marron Lee, Albuquerque Priscilla Smith, Member Kimberly Sanchez Rael, Albuquerque Teri Lynn Garcia, Member Robert L. Schwartz, Albuquerque Edwin Begay, Member Rebecca Apel, Member EX-OFFICIO MEMBERS The Honorable Michelle Lujan Grisham, J.D. Governor of New Mexico, Santa Fe UNM GALLUP ADMINISTRATORS Ryan Stewart, Ed.L.D. James Malm, DM, Chief Executive Officer Secretary of Public Education, Santa Fe Daniel Primozic, P.h.D., Dean of Instruction Secretary of Higher Education, Santa Fe – Vacant Jayme McMahon, Director of Student Affairs Robert Griego, Director of Business Operations UNM OFFICERS Garnett S. Stokes, Ph.D., President Paul B. Roth, M.D., Executive Vice President and Chancellor: Health Sciences James P. Holloway, Ph.D., Executive Vice President: Academic Affairs and Provost Teresa Costantinidis, M.B.A., Senior Vice President for Finance and Administration Semester Dates 2019-2020 CALENDAR UNIVERSITY OF NEW MEXICO – GALLUP Fall 2019: Full Term First Half Second Half (16 Weeks) (1st 8 weeks) (2nd 8 Weeks) Aug 19-Dec 14 Aug 19-Oct 12 Oct 14-Dec 14 Spring 2020: Full Term First Half Second Half (16 Weeks) (1st 8 weeks) (2nd 8 Weeks) Jan 20-May 16 Jan 20-Mar 14 Mar 23-May 16 NTRODUCTION TO programs). The Director and Title IX Coordinator can be I UNM reached at the Office of Equal Opportunity (OEO), 609 Buena Located in Gallup, New Mexico, the adobe-style facilities sits GALLUP Vista NE, University of New Mexico 87131, (505) 277-5251.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Markedness Effects in Sentence Formation Canaan Breiss, Bruce Hayes
    Phonological markedness effects in sentence formation Canaan Breiss, Bruce Hayes Language, Volume 96, Number 2, June 2020, pp. 338-370 (Article) Published by Linguistic Society of America DOI: https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2020.0023 For additional information about this article https://muse.jhu.edu/article/757631 [ Access provided at 1 Jul 2020 21:07 GMT with no institutional affiliation ] PHONOLOGICAL MARKEDNESS EFFECTS IN SENTENCE FORMATION CANAAN BREISS BRUCE HAYES University of California, Los Angeles University of California, Los Angeles Earlier research has found that phonological markedness constraints (for example, against stress clash or sibilant sequences) statistically influence speakers’ choices between particular syn - tactic constructions and between synonymous words. In this study, we test phonological con - straints not just in particular cases, but across the board. We employ a novel method that statistically models the distribution of word bigrams (consecutive two-word sequences) and how this distribution is influenced by phonological constraints. Our study of multiple corpora indicates that several phonological constraints do indeed play a statistically significant role in English sen - tence formation. We also show that by examining particular subsets of the corpora we can diag - nose the mechanisms whereby phonologically marked sequences come to be underrepresented. We conclude by discussing modes of grammatical organization compatible with our findings.* Keywords : markedness, phrasal phonology, syntax-phonology interface, grammatical architec - tures 1. Goals and setting . The focus of this article is how different domains of linguistic knowledge—here, syntactic, phonological, and lexical—interact in the creation of sen - tences. An influential proposal for how these domains interact is the componential feed- forward model, laid out in Chomsky 1965:16 and shown in Figure 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Psycho-Physiological Problems of the Indians of New Mexico Learning A
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 050 832 RC 004 YjO AUTHOR Sande, Joe S. TITLE Psycho-Physiological Problems. of the Indians of New Mexico Learning a Second Language. INSTITUTION All Indian Pueblo Council, Albuquerque, N. Mex. PUB DATE [65] NOTE 21p. EDRS PRICE EDRS Price MF-$0.65 HC-'.!3.29 DESCRIPTORS *American Indians, *Bilingualism, *Education, *English (Second Language), *Language, Linguistics, Navaho, Oral Communication, Spanish IDENTIFIERS *New Mexico ABSTRACT According to the document, learning the English language is comparable to learning a foreign language for the American Indian child. Thus, a brief history of the Indians of New Mexico is given in order to emphasize language differences and problems arising in learning a new tongue. As pointed out, second-language learning requires a change in pattern of intonation, stress, rhythv, and meanings inaddition to the phonemes. Major linguistic families of the American Indians in New Mexico are discussed, thereby pointing up problematic areas which could relate to academic difficulties for the American Indian. It is believed that the monolingual Indian 6-year-old, entering school for the first time, is at a great disadvantage when compared with an English-speaking 6-year-old entering the same school and having a listening vocabulary of perhaps 8,000 to 10,000 English words. (EL) S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, EDUCATION 'ARE OPFICE ON .7 !!:" THIS DOCUMENT HAS itEPhODUctu EXACTLY AS RECEIVED FAO M THE PERSON OR ORGANITATION ORIGINATING IT. POINTS OF VIEW OH OPINIONS siArCD DO NOT NECES- OD SARILY REPRESENT OFFICIAL OFFICE OP EDU- CATION POSITION OR POLICY. RECEIVED AUG 369 O PSYCHO-PHYSIOLOGICAL PROBLEMS OF ! W vr, THE iNDWNIS OF NEW MEXICO 6.
    [Show full text]
  • Curriculum Vitae – Siri G. Tuttle May 6, 2011 SIRI G. TUTTLE Associate Professor Alask
    SIRI G. TUTTLE Associate Professor Alaska Native Language Center P.O. Box 757680 University of Alaska Fairbanks Fairbanks, Alaska 99775-7680 907-474-5708 907-474-6586 Fax <[email protected]> PRESENT: Associate professor, Alaska Native Language Center and Linguistics Program, University of Alaska Fairbanks. Teaching and research responsibilities include graduate and undergraduate classes, field research and support of language revitalization efforts in the Ahtna and Lower Tanana language areas. 7/01-7/03: Mitarbeiter in DGfS project 'Lexicon-Syntax Interface' with Dr. Elke Nowak, at the Technische Universität Berlin. EDUCATION: 6/98-6/01: Post-Doctoral Fellow, Department of Linguistics, UCLA; study and projects in phonetics under Peter Ladefoged focusing on phonetic questions in Western and Eastern Apache. PhD: (1998) Metrical and Tonal Structures in Tanana Athabaskan. University of Washington, Sharon Hargus, Advisor. MA: (1990) Stress and Vowel Length in Tolowa. University of Washington, Sharon Hargus, Advisor. TEACHING: University of Alaska, Fairbanks Fall Semester 2010: Introduction to Athabascan Linguistics; Linguistics 101 Spring Semester 2010: Koyukon Athabascan (with Lorraine David) Fall Semester 2009: Linguistics 101, Beginning Koyukon Athabascan (with Lorraine David). Spring semester 2009: Linguistics 318, Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology; English 318, Modern English Grammar; Native Language Apprenticeship (distance); Field Internship. Fall Semester 2008: Linguistics 101, Native Language Apprenticeship (distance) Fall
    [Show full text]