Domains on the Border: Between Morphology and Phonology Beata A

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Domains on the Border: Between Morphology and Phonology Beata A University of Connecticut OpenCommons@UConn Doctoral Dissertations University of Connecticut Graduate School 8-24-2015 Domains on the Border: Between Morphology and Phonology Beata A. Moskal University of Connecticut - Storrs, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://opencommons.uconn.edu/dissertations Recommended Citation Moskal, Beata A., "Domains on the Border: Between Morphology and Phonology" (2015). Doctoral Dissertations. 892. https://opencommons.uconn.edu/dissertations/892 Domains on the Border: Between Morphology and Phonology Beata Moskal, PhD University of Connecticut, 2015 In this dissertation, I show that a difference in structure between functional and lexical items has a restricting effect on both the morphology and the phonology. Morphologically, we observe two asymmetries: (i) in lexical nouns, number-driven root-suppletion is com- mon whilst case-driven root-suppletion is virtually unattested; (ii) in contrast, pronouns commonly supplete for both number and case. By and large, we see the same pattern in verbs, observing a contrast between lexical verbs and auxiliaries with regard to suppletion for aspect and tense. In order to account for these asymmetries, I appeal to structural differ- ences between lexical and functional material, combined with locality effects as proposed in Distributed Morphology (Halle & Marantz 1993). Crucially, lexical material contains a category-defining node which has a delimiting effect that causes case/tense to be insuffi- ciently local to the root to condition its suppletion. In contrast, functional material lacks category-defining nodes and thus no delimiting effect is observed and case/tense are free to condition suppletion of the functional base. Phonologically, we see a correlation between the presence of a category-defining node and the absence of dominant prefixes in vowel harmony and lexical accent, whilst in the absence of a category-defining node dominant prefixes are attested. Thus, I argue for universal limitations on suppletion patterns and dominant prefixes, which crucially derive from a difference in morpho-syntactic structure between lexical and functional material. Domains on the Border: Between Morphology and Phonology Beata Alexandra Moskal B.A., Leiden University, 2006 M.A., Leiden University, 2009 M.A., University of Connecticut, 2013 A Dissertation Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy at the University of Connecticut 2015 i . Copyright by Beata Alexandra Moskal 2015 ii APPROVAL! PAGE Doctor of Philosophy Dissertation ! Domains on the Border: Between Morphology and Phonology ! ! ! Presented by Beata Alexandra Moskal, B.A., M.A. ! Major Advisor ___________________________________________________________________ Jonathan Bobaljik ! Major Advisor ___________________________________________________________________ Harry van der Hulst ! Associate Advisor ___________________________________________________________________ Andrea Calabrese ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! University of Connecticut 2015 iii . ACKNOWLEDGMENTS . Over the years, so many versions of the acknowledgments have flitted through my mind. Some, if I would have actually written them down, would have taken up ten pages. Or more. Some are shorter: “The less said the better.” Turns out I settled on a slightly extended version of the latter. The only reason you are reading this dissertation is Pete, my husband. Just like the summer was not done by you for you but by us for us, this dissertation was not done by me for me but for us by us. I love you. The other people most involved in creating this monster were my committee members. Jonathan, spasibo bolshoe1 - mostly for being willing to advise me, despite all the frus- tration I must have caused you - but also for making me the academic I am today. Harry, thank you for always believing in me, and our elemental discussions. Andrea, thank you for always raising unexpected questions. Also, I would very much like to thank Norbert Corver and Caroline Fery´ for offering me a future after graduating. I would also like to take this opportunity to thank my friends, most notably Tessa (maid of honour and partner in crime) and Leah (my “local” (Canadian!) friend); also my closest friends at the Linguistics department: Jungmin (need I say more than LF?) and Neda (our year is a special one, no two ways about it). And a thank you to the Monterey Bay Aquarium for having a webcam of their sea otters. 1Apologies if the Russian transliteration is wrong - I could not decide between bolshoe and bolsoeˇ .I have the suspicion it is the latter. The rest of the dissertation should be fine, though. iv Thanks also to my siblings. Ju, thanks for always being there for me (and making sure Mum and Dad got to the wedding alright). Tamara and Edwin (and Stimpie, Chuli and Lindi): your tropical island was the best possible place to figure out what we want out of our future. Finally, this dissertation is dedicated to my parents: zonder jullie was ik nooit de per- soon die ik nu ben, met mijn eigen(aardige) karakter en al; daar ben ik eeuwig dankbaar voor. Dzie¸kuje¸. v . I love deadlines. I like the whooshing sound they make as they fly by. - Douglas Adams vi Contents 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Locality . 5 1.2 Overview . 11 2 Limits in the nominal domain 14 2.1 Suppletion in lexical nouns . 20 2.1.1 Case-driven root-suppletion . 24 2.1.2 Interlude: Non-suppletion . 38 2.1.3 Diminutive blocking . 41 2.2 Suppletion in pronouns . 47 2.2.1 Decomposing case . 51 2.3 Locality . 61 2.4 Portmanteaux part one . 66 2.5 Adjacency . 71 2.5.1 Adjacency blocking effects . 75 2.5.2 Against adjacency . 91 2.5.3 Towards a theory without adjacency . 99 2.6 Summary of this chapter . 107 3 Limits in the verbal domain 109 3.1 Aspect-driven suppletion . 114 3.2 Tense-driven suppletion . 118 3.2.1 Auxiliaries . 118 3.2.2 Non-suppletion . 144 3.2.3 Interim summary . 153 3.2.4 Root-v-T-Asp . 153 3.2.5 Weak hypothesis . 160 3.2.6 Potentially problematic cases . 165 3.2.7 Summary of T-driven suppletion . 189 vii 3.3 Mood-driven suppletion . 191 3.4 Portmanteaux part two . 192 3.4.1 Root portmanteaux . 196 3.4.1.1 Domain Suspension . 196 3.4.1.2 123 is easier than ABC . 199 3.5 Summary of this chapter . 209 4 A Modest Proposal 211 4.1 Introduction . 211 4.2 Vowel harmony . 214 4.2.1 Dominant prefixes . 219 4.2.2 No Dominant Prefix Hypothesis . 235 4.2.2.1 Dominancy . 245 4.2.3 Compounds . 255 4.2.4 Conclusion . 259 4.3 Lexical accent . 262 4.3.1 Dominant prefixes . 264 4.3.1.1 Deletive accent resolution . 265 4.3.1.2 Additive accent resolution . 275 4.3.1.3 Functional material . 290 4.3.2 Compounds . 294 4.3.2.1 Root compounds . 297 4.3.2.2 Stem compounds . 310 4.3.3 Conclusion . 314 4.4 Discussion . 316 4.4.1 (In)direct reference . 317 4.4.2 Cyclicity and phases . 323 4.4.3 The boundary . 327 4.5 Summary of this chapter . 335 5 Conclusion 337 5.1 Summary . 337 5.2 Implications for linguistics . 339 5.3 Future directions . 341 Bibliography 346 viii Chapter 1 Introduction This dissertation constitutes an exploration into a range of morphological and morpho- phonological asymmetries between lexical and functional items. This leads to a proposal that at the heart of the asymmetries identified here is that there is more structure in lexical items than in functional items, which has a delimiting effect on the former but not the latter. Morphologically, I identify universal restrictions on patterns of suppletion (the phe- nomenon where a single lexical item is associated with two phonologically unrelated forms) in nominals and verbs. Specifically, we observe the following asymmetries in nominals: (1) In lexical nouns number-driven root-suppletion is common while case-driven root- suppletion seems unattested, bar a few counterexamples discussed later. (2) In contrast to lexical nouns, pronouns commonly supplete for both number and case. 1 Introduction 2 This is exemplified by the following contrasts in lexical nouns where we see suppletion in the context of the plural in Ket (3), and pronouns where we see suppletion in the context of number and case in German first person pronouns (4): (3) SINGULAR PLURAL gloss o;ks’ aPq ‘tree’ d1;l’ k2Pt ‘child’ kEPtdEP-N ‘man’ (4) SINGULAR PLURAL NOM ich wir DAT mir uns ACC mich uns In the verbal domain, we, by and large, observe a similar pattern: (5) In lexical verbs aspect-driven root-suppletion is attested while tense-driven root- suppletion seems unattested, bar a handful of counterexamples discussed later. (6) In contrast to lexical verbs, auxiliaries commonly supplete for tense. This is exemplified by the following pairs, where (7) is a lexical verb and (8) is a functional verb in Lezgian: (7) t’u-na¨ eat-AOR ne-z eat-IPFV (8) ja be.PRESENT tir be.PAST Introduction 3 We see that the lexical verb eat suppletes for aspect, the aorist-imperfective contrast, whilst the functional verb be suppletes for tense, present vs. past. Crucially, what we see based on the suppletion patterns in nominal and verbal material is that lexical items are more restrictive than functional items. In the following, I will argue that this discrepancy is categorical in nature and is not explained by exclusively appealing to frequency effects. In a similar vein, we observe an asymmetry in the morpho-phonological process of vowel harmony: in lexical material a root cannot be influenced by a prefix, but in functional material the prefix is free to interact. In (9), we see that in Tunen the lexical item and ‘roof’ is unaffected by the [+ATR] class 3 prefix mu- and surfaces as disharmonic [-ATR] and; in contrast, in (10), the functional item tana ‘this’ is realised as [+ATR] t@n@ in the context of the same [+ATR] class 3 prefix mu-, even though otherwise it surfaces as [-ATR], as in ba-tana ‘cl.2-this!’.
Recommended publications
  • Slavic Morphology
    Frank Y. Gladney University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Slavic Morphology Introduction. Slavic speakers are able to communicate because they share an inventory of sound-meaning pairings, or morphemes—a lexicon. The strings of meaningful sound which they exchange (sentences) are too long and various to be contained in the lexicon, so they need rules for combining morphemes into sentences— syntax. Occurring in sentences, morphemes assume various shapes, and rather than have all these shapes listed in the lexicon, some of them are described as the results of sound change—phonology. A few decades ago lexicon, syntax, and phonology were thought to suffice for describing a language. Syntax arranges the lexical items in sentences and phonology gives their pronunciation. Morphology? Word forms in Slavic are largely made up of morphemes, so in that sense Slavic, unlike Chinese, has morphology. It does not necessarily follow that Slavic has a separate grammar component called morphology. It is possible the facts of morphology can be accounted for with (sublexical) syntax and phonology. The facts in question include derivation, the formation of words,1 and inflection, their formal alteration as governed by the syntactic features of their sentence environment. Although it may seem reasonable to claim that words must be formed before they can be inflected—in most descriptions of Slavic languages, Stammbildungslehre (formation des mots, slovoobrazovanie) is treated before Formenlehre (flexion des mots, slovoizmenenie)—this selective survey of Slavic morphology will begin with inflection, for the reason that, first, it lies closer to the surface and, second, much that is regarded as word- (or stem-) formation is dependent on inflection.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Irish Spelling
    Understanding Irish Spelling A Handbook for Teachers and Learners Nancy Stenson and Tina Hickey Understanding Irish Spelling A Handbook for Teachers and Learners Nancy Stenson and Tina Hickey i © Stenson and Hickey 2018 ii Acknowledgements The preparation of this publication was supported by a grant from An Chomhairle um Oideachas Gaeltachta agus Gaelscolaíochta, and we wish to express our sincere thanks to COGG, and to Muireann Ní Mhóráin and Pól Ó Cainín in particular. We acknowledge most gratefully the support of the Marie Skłodowska-Curie Fellowship scheme for enabling this collaboration through its funding of an Incoming International Fellowship to the first author, and to UCD School of Psychology for hosting her as an incoming fellow and later an as Adjunct Professor. We also thank the Fulbright Foundation for the Fellowship they awarded to Prof. Stenson prior to the Marie Curie fellowship. Most of all, we thank the educators at first, second and third level who shared their experience and expertise with us in the research from which we draw in this publication. We benefitted significantly from input from many sources, not all of whom can be named here. Firstly, we wish to thank most sincerely all of the participants in our qualitative study interviews, who generously shared their time and expertise with us, and those in the schools that welcomed us to their classrooms and facilitated observation and interviews. We also wish to thank the participants at many conferences, seminars and presentations, particularly those in Bangor, Berlin, Brighton, Hamilton and Ottawa, as well as those in several educational institutions in Ireland who offered comments and suggestions.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 3 Distributed Morphology and the Pieces of Inflection Morris Halle
    Chapter 3 Distributed Morphology and Morris Halle and the Pieces of Inflection Alec Marantz 1 Morphology with or without Affixes The last few years have seen the emergence of several clearly articulated alternative approaches to morphology. One such approach rests on the notion that only stems of the so-called lexical categories (N, V, A) are morpheme "pieces" in the traditional sense—connections between (bun- dles of) meaning (features) and (bundles of) sound (features). What look like affixes on this view are merely the by-product of morphophonological rules called word formation rules (WFRs) that are sensitive to features associated with the lexical categories, called lexemes. Such an a-morphous or affixless theory, adumbrated by Beard (1966) and Aronoff (1976), has been articulated most notably by Anderson (1992) and in major new studies by Aronoff (1992) and Beard (1991). In contrast, Lieber (1992) has refined the traditional notion that affixes as well as lexical stems are "mor- pheme" pieces whose lexical entries relate phonological form with mean- ing and function. For Lieber and other "lexicalists" (see, e.g., Jensen 1990), the combining of lexical items creates the words that operate in the syntax. In this paper we describe and defend a third theory of morphol- ogy, Distributed Morphology,1 which combines features of the affixless and the lexicalist alternatives. With Anderson, Beard, and Aronoff, we endorse the separation of the terminal elements involved in the syntax from the phonological realization of these elements. With Lieber and the lexicalists, on the other hand, we take the phonological realization of the terminal elements in the syntax to be governed by lexical (Vocabulary) entries that relate bundles of morphosyntactic features to bundles of pho- nological features.
    [Show full text]
  • Distributed Morphology As a Regular Relation Marina Ermolaeva University of Chicago, [email protected]
    Proceedings of the Society for Computation in Linguistics Volume 1 Article 20 2018 Distributed Morphology as a regular relation Marina Ermolaeva University of Chicago, [email protected] Daniel Edmiston University of Chicago, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/scil Part of the Computational Linguistics Commons Recommended Citation Ermolaeva, Marina and Edmiston, Daniel (2018) "Distributed Morphology as a regular relation," Proceedings of the Society for Computation in Linguistics: Vol. 1 , Article 20. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7275/R51834PC Available at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/scil/vol1/iss1/20 This Extended Abstract is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Proceedings of the Society for Computation in Linguistics by an authorized editor of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Distributed Morphology as a regular relation Marina Ermolaeva and Daniel Edmiston University of Chicago {mermolaeva,danedmiston}@uchicago.edu 1 Introduction structure (FS). An FS F is defined as a pair M, E , h i where the feature bundle M = feat(F ) is a sub- This research reorganizes the Distributed Morphol- set of some finite set of features (including syntac- ogy (DM, (Halle and Marantz, 1993)) framework tic category labels) and the phonological exponent to work over strings. That the morphological mod- E = exp(F ) (Σ None, ), where Σ is a fi- ule should operate over strings is desirable, since it ∈ ∪ { } nite set of phonemes. The default exponent of each is assumed that most (arguably all) morphological syntactic unit is None, a place-holder to be replaced processes can be modelled with regular languages by vocabulary insertion (VI) in the morphological (Karttunen et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Attrition: the Next Phase Barbara Köpke, Monika Schmid
    Language Attrition: The next phase Barbara Köpke, Monika Schmid To cite this version: Barbara Köpke, Monika Schmid. Language Attrition: The next phase. Monika S. Schmid, Barbara Köpke, Merel Keijzer, Lina Weilemar. First Language Attrition: Interdisciplinary perspectives on methodological issues, John Benjamins, pp.1-43, 2004, Studies in Bilingualism, 9027241392. hal- 00879106 HAL Id: hal-00879106 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00879106 Submitted on 31 Oct 2013 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Language Attrition: The Next Phase Barbara Köpke (Université de Toulouse – Le Mirail) and Monika S. Schmid Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam Barbara Köpke Laboratoire de Neuropsycholinguistique Jacques Lordat Institut des Sciences du Cerveau de Toulouse Université de Toulouse-Le Mirail 31058 Toulouse Cedex France [email protected] Monika S. Schmid Engelse Taal en Cultuur Faculteit der Letteren Vrije Universiteit 1081 HV Amsterdam The Netherlands [email protected] Published in : M.S. Schmid, B. Köpke, M. Keijzer & L. Weilemar (2004). First Language Attrition. Interdisciplinary perspectives on methodological issues (pp. 1-43). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Köpke, B. & Schmid, M.S. (2004). Language Attrition: The Next Phase. In M.S. Schmid, B.
    [Show full text]
  • 002.Alex.Comitato.2A Bozza
    Xaverio Ballester /A/ Y EL VOCALISMO INDOEUROPEO Negli últimi quarant’anni la linguística indo- europea si è in gran parte perduta dietro al mito delle laringali, di cui non intendo tenere alcun conto, e dello strutturalismo, di cui ten- go un conto molto limitato. Giuliano Bonfante, I dialetti indoeuropei, p. 8 De la regla a la ley o de mal en peor Bien digna de mención entre las primerísimas descripciones del mode- lo vocálico indoeuropeo es la propuesta de un inventario fonemático con únicamente tres timbres vocálicos /a i u/, una propuesta empero que fue desgraciadamente y demasiado pronto abandonada, siendo quizá la más conspicua consecuencia de este abandono el hecho de que para la Lin- güística indoeuropea oficialista la ausencia de /a/ devino en la práctica un axioma, de modo que, casi en cualquier posterior propuesta sobre el voca- lismo indoeuropeo, se ha venido adoptando la idea de que no hubiese exis- tido nunca la vocal /a/, y explicándose los ineluctables casos de presencia de /a/ en el material indoeuropeo con variados y bizarros argumentos del tipo de vocalismo despectivo, infantil o popular. Sin embargo, si conside- rada hoy spregiudicatamente, la argumentación que motivó el desalojo de la primitiva /a/ indoeuropea no presenta, al menos desde una perspectiva fonotipológica hodierna, ninguna validez en absoluto. Invocaremos un testimonio objetivo del tema para exponer brevemen- te la cuestión. Escribía O. Szemerényi: «Sotto l’impressione dell’arcai- cità del sanscrito, i fondatori dell’indoeuropeistica e i loro immediati suc- cessori pensavano che il sistema triangolare del sanscrito i–a–u rappre- sentasse la situazione originaria.
    [Show full text]
  • Suppletion Replication in Grammaticalization and Its Triggering Factors
    Language Dynamics and Change 5 (2015) 52–91 brill.com/ldc Suppletion Replication in Grammaticalization and Its Triggering Factors Eugen Hill University of Erfurt [email protected] Abstract The paper tries to account for several instances of emerging suppletion by estab- lishing a cross-linguistic tendency of suppletion replication in grammaticalization. It can be shown that words which acquire new grammatical functions and therefore enter a different class of lexemes tend to copy suppletion patterns already present in other members of this class. This development can be triggered by factors of differ- ent nature, either internal to the language in question or rooted in contact between different languages or dialects of the same language. The suppletion replication ten- dency is demonstrated on several cases of grammaticalization of demonstrative or relative pronouns into 3rd person pronouns. This typologically common development is known to have led to the creation of new suppletion in several languages of Europe. In the present paper, three particularly telling cases from Slavonic, dialects of Lithua- nian and early West Germanic dialects spoken on the continent are discussed in detail. Keywords inflectional morphology – suppletion – pronouns – Slavonic – Baltic – Germanic 1 Introduction In recent years, suppletion in inflectional paradigms has often been the sub- ject of linguistic studies. Many instances of inflectional suppletion, both known and newly discovered, have received high-quality detailed descrip- © koninklijke brill nv, leiden, 2015 | doi: 10.1163/22105832-00501003 suppletion replication in grammaticalization 53 tions.1 On this empirical basis, substantial advances in the synchronic under- standing of suppletion and its place in the inflection of natural languages have been made.2 The time seems ripe for an attempt to gain deeper insight into the diachronic dimension of suppletion in inflectional paradigms, i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Distributed Morphology and the Syntax/Morphology Interface
    Distributed Morphology and the Syntax/Morphology Interface David Embick and Rolf Noyer University of Pennsylvania ***Draft of October 25, 2004*** (To appear in G. Ramchand and C. Reiss eds., The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Interfaces, Oxford University Press) 1 Introduction: The Syntax/Morphology Interface A theory of the syntax/morphology interface is first, a theory of how ‘words’ and their internal structure – the traditional domain of morphology – relate to the structures generated by the syntax, and second, a theory of how the rules for deriving complex words relate to the rules for deriving syntactic structures. A prominent line of research in this area consists of approaches assuming some version of the Lexicalist Hypothesis. For present purposes, this is the claim that (at least some) words are special in ways that e.g. phrases are not, and that this ‘specialness’ calls for an architecture in which the derivation of words and the derivation of syntactic objects occur in different modules of the grammar (the Lexicon versus the syntax).1 While the ‘words’ derived in the Lexicon serve as the terminals in the syntactic derivation, there is a sharp division between syntax and morphology according to this approach. In this way, the interface between syntax and morphology in such a theory is opaque or indirect: there is no reason to expect the structure and composition of ‘words’ to relate to the structure and composition of syntactic objects in any transparent or for that matter systematic fashion. A second line of research advances the hypothesis that ‘words’ are assembled by rules of the syntax.
    [Show full text]
  • Morphology in Language Processing DAY 2: VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION and SEMITIC MORPHOLOGY
    Morphology in Language Processing DAY 2: VISUAL WORD RECOGNITION AND SEMITIC MORPHOLOGY ACTL 2017, June 2017 1 Roadmap •More on Semitic morphology •Review of the literature investigating Semitic visual word recognition 2 But first… 3 Happy 4-week birthday, Aldo! 4 Concatenative morphology •Concatenative morphology involves linear affixation; i.e., adding affixes to the beginning or the end of a root or stem. •Agglutinative languages have the most concatenative type of morphology, in which each morphological piece is contiguous and predictable. •A good example of such a language is Turkish, in which inflectional paradigms for nouns show very regular concatenative morphology: ip ‘string’ ip-ler ‘strings’ ip-ler-i ‘strings, acc.’ ip-ler-im ‘my string’ ip-ler-imiz ‘our strings’ ip-ler-imiz-den ‘from our strings 5 Concatenative morphology in Semitic •Important: not all morphology in Semitic is different from this. For instance, Hebrew – and all other Semitic languages – have concatenative morphology. •An example is regular plural formation in Hebrew, in which the plural suffixes –im (masc.) or –ot (fem.) are added to nouns: Singular Plural Gloss xatul xatulim cat/cats et etim pen/pens mafteax maftexot key/keys 6 Nonconcatenative morphology •In nonconcatenative morphology, related words are not as transparently form- relatable; that is, affixation does not have as straightforward a phonological exponence as “add something to the beginning or the end of the root or stem.” •There are many, many types of nonconcatenative morphology. 7 Nonconcatenative morphology •Any time the phonological exponence of a particular morphological category is not linearly appended to the edge of a root or a stem, then we’re dealing with some variety of nonconcatenative morphology.
    [Show full text]
  • ABRALIN 2007 Course on Distributed Morphology
    Distributed Morphology ABRALIN 2007, February 22-27, 2007 Heidi Harley, University of Arizona [email protected]; http://dingo.sbs.arizona.edu/~hharley/ Readings: Downloadable from the course home page, http://dingo.sbs.arizona.edu/~hharley/courses/ABRALIN/ABRALIN2007/ (Note: Schedule is only approximate! We will deviate from this if we need to.) Feb. 22 Introduction to (post-)syntactic morphology Harley and Noyer 2000: "Distributed Morphology" Bobaljik 2000: "The ins and outs of contextual allomorphy" Feb. 23 Complex verb structure Harley, 2006, "On the causative construction" Travis, 2000, "Event Structure in Syntax" Feb. 24 Irregularity, blocking and getting morphemes in order Embick, David. "Locality, Listedness and Morphological Identity" Barragan, Luis. "Movement and allomorphy in the Cupeno verb construction" Feb. 25 Phi-features and post-syntactic morphological operations Williams, Edwin. "Remarks on Lexical Knowledge" Bobaljik, Jonathan. "Syncretism without paradigms" (Frampton, John. "Syncretism, Impoverishment and the structure of person features") Feb. 26 DM as a psychological model Pfau, R. 2000. "Speech Errors and Distributed Morphology" Chapts 1, 2 and 4 only. Longtin, C-M et al. 2000"Morphological Priming Without Morphological Relationship" Feb. 27 Wrap-up: DM and semantics Marantz, Alec. 1997. "No escape from syntax: Don't try morphological analysis in the privacy of your own lexicon." Kratzer, Angelika. 1996. "Severing the external argument from its verb" Harley/ Distributed Morphology Lecture Notes ABRALIN 2007 1 Background: Distributed Morphology and the Syntax-Morphology Interface Harley and Noyer 2000: "Distributed Morphology" Some morphological terminology: stem for ‘suffix2’ [[prefix[[root]suffix1]]suffix2] root=stem for suffix1 stem for ‘prefix’ Example: [[in[[describ]abil]ity] derivation vs.
    [Show full text]
  • International Journal of the Sociology of Language
    IJSL 2018; 252: 97–123 Hannah Carlan* “In the mouth of an aborigine”: language ideologies and logics of racialization in the Linguistic Survey of India https://doi.org/10.1515/ijsl-2018-0016 Abstract: The Linguistic Survey of India (LSI),editedandcompiledbyGeorge Abraham Grierson, was the first systematic effort by the British colonial gov- ernment to document the spoken languages and dialects of India. While Grierson advocated an approach to philology that dismissed the affinity of language to race, the LSI mobilizes a complex, intertextualsetofracializing discourses that form the ideological ground upon which representations of language were constructed and naturalized. I analyze a sub-set of the LSI’s volumes in order to demonstrate how Grierson’s linguistic descriptions and categorizations racialize minority languages and their speakers as corrupt, impure, and uncivilized. I highlight how semiotic processes in the text con- struct speakers as possessing essential “ethnic” characteristics that are seen as indexical of naturalized linguistic differences. I argue that metapragmatic statements within descriptions of languages and dialects are made possible by ethnological discourses that ultimately reinforce an indexical relationship between language and race. This analysis of the survey sheds light on the centrality of language in colonial constructions of social difference in India, as well as the continued importance of language as a tool for legitimating claims for political recognition in postcolonial India. Keywords: language surveys, language ideologies, racialization, British colonialism, India 1 Introduction The Linguistic Survey of India (1903–1928), hereafter the LSI, was the first attempt to capture the spoken languages and dialects of British India as part of the wider goal of the colonial government to learn, through bureaucratic documentation, about the social makeup of its subjects.
    [Show full text]
  • Computational Approaches to Mapping Historical Irish Cognate Verb Forms
    Past, present and future: Computational approaches to mapping historical Irish cognate verb forms Theodorus Leman Franciscus Fransen Thesis submitted for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Centre for Language & Communication Studies School of Linguistic, Speech and Communication Sciences Trinity College Dublin The University of Dublin 2019 Declaration I declare that this thesis has not been submitted as an exercise for a degree at this or any other university and it is entirely my own work. I agree to deposit this thesis in the University’s open access institutional repository or allow the Library to do so on my behalf, subject to Irish Copyright Legislation and Trinity College Library conditions of use and acknowledgement. Summary This thesis investigates how computational methods can be used to enhance our understanding of the significant historical developments in the verbal system between Old Irish (c. 8th–9th centuries A.D.) and Modern Irish (13th century onwards). Out of all grammatical subsystems, the verbal system is subject to the most severe morphological changes between Old and Modern Irish, i.e., during the Middle Irish period (c. 10th–12th centuries). The main contribution of this thesis is the creation of a morphological Finite-State Transducer (FST) for Old Irish, focusing on verbs, successfully implemented in the finite-state tool foma (Hulden 2009). The FST is an important advancement in Natural Language Processing for Old Irish and will assist research in various linguistic subdisciplines as well as in medieval Irish philology. Chapter 1 demonstrates that a hiatus exists in digital linguistic support for historical Irish language periods. This hiatus in coverage and continuity is particularly true for Early Modern Irish (c.
    [Show full text]