Chapter 3 Distributed Morphology and the Pieces of Inflection Morris Halle
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Slavic Morphology
Frank Y. Gladney University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Slavic Morphology Introduction. Slavic speakers are able to communicate because they share an inventory of sound-meaning pairings, or morphemes—a lexicon. The strings of meaningful sound which they exchange (sentences) are too long and various to be contained in the lexicon, so they need rules for combining morphemes into sentences— syntax. Occurring in sentences, morphemes assume various shapes, and rather than have all these shapes listed in the lexicon, some of them are described as the results of sound change—phonology. A few decades ago lexicon, syntax, and phonology were thought to suffice for describing a language. Syntax arranges the lexical items in sentences and phonology gives their pronunciation. Morphology? Word forms in Slavic are largely made up of morphemes, so in that sense Slavic, unlike Chinese, has morphology. It does not necessarily follow that Slavic has a separate grammar component called morphology. It is possible the facts of morphology can be accounted for with (sublexical) syntax and phonology. The facts in question include derivation, the formation of words,1 and inflection, their formal alteration as governed by the syntactic features of their sentence environment. Although it may seem reasonable to claim that words must be formed before they can be inflected—in most descriptions of Slavic languages, Stammbildungslehre (formation des mots, slovoobrazovanie) is treated before Formenlehre (flexion des mots, slovoizmenenie)—this selective survey of Slavic morphology will begin with inflection, for the reason that, first, it lies closer to the surface and, second, much that is regarded as word- (or stem-) formation is dependent on inflection. -
The Check-List Halle's Condition 2 and the Form and Meaning Of
The check-list Halle’s Condition 2 and the form and meaning of phonological features Daniel Currie Hall Meertens Instituut University of Toronto Sixth North American Phonology Conference Concordia University, Montréal April 30–May 2, 2010 1 D. C. Hall: The check-list NAPhC 6 O: C 2 Halle (1959: 19) Condition (2): e phonetic properties in terms of which segments are characterized belong to a specific, narrowly restricted set of such properties called the distinctive features. All distinctive features are binary. In accepting Condition (2), one commits oneself to characterizing all segments in all languages in terms of a restricted check list of aributes like “nasality, voicing, palatalization, etc.”, with regard to which the only relevant question is “does the segment possess the particular aribute?” It follows, therefore, that differences between segments can be expressed only as differences in their feature composition and that consequently segments (even in different languages) can differ from each other only in a restricted number of ways. 2 D. C. Hall: The check-list NAPhC 6 O: C 2 In other words: 1. Segments are sets of features 2. Features are binary 3. Features are drawn from an innate universal set 4. Features have phonetic content These fundamental assumptions of SPR are all more or less controversial 51 years later. 3 D. C. Hall: The check-list NAPhC 6 1: S H ? One possibility: I Segments (or unsegmented uerances) are represented exactly as spoken/heard, in full phonetic detail. I This is the view of Exemplar Theory (e.g., Johnson 1996, 2007; Pierrehumbert 2001, 2002; Cole 2009). -
Prolegomena To, Prolegomena to a Theory of Wordformation. a Reply To
AMSTERDAM STUDIES IN THE THEORY AND HISTORY OF LINGUISTIC SCIENCE E. F. K. KOERNER, General Editor Series IV - CURRENT ISSUES IN LINGUISTIC THEORY Advisory Editorial Board Henning Andersen (Albany, N.Y.); Raimo Anttila (Los Angeles) Tomaz V. Gamkrelidze (Tiflis); Klaus J. Kohler (Kiel) J. Peter Mäher (Hamburg);Ernst Pulgram (Ann Arbor, Mich.) E. Wyn Roberts (Vancouver, B.C.); Danny Steinberg (Honolulu) Volume 1 E. F. K. Koerner, ed. The Transformational-Generative Paradigm and Modern Linguistic Theory THE TRANSFORMATIONAL-GENERATIVE PARADIGM AND MODERN LINGUISTIC THEORY edited by E. F. K. KOERNER with the assistance of JOHN ODMARK and J. HOWARD SHAW AMSTERDAM / JOHN BENJAMINS B.V. 1975 © Copyright 1975 - John Benjamins B.V. ISBN 90 272 0901 4/90 272 0902 2 No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by print, photoprint, microfilm or any other means, without written permission from the publisher. CONTENTS Preface v I. SYNTAX AND SEMANTICS Dwight Bolinger: Meaning and Form: Some fallacies of asemantic grammar 3 Adam Makkai: Stratificational Solutions to Unbridgeable Gaps in Transformational-Generative Grammar 37 Fred C. C. Peng: Non-Uniqueness in the Treatment of the Separabil- ity of Semantics and Syntax in Compound Expressions 87 II. PHONOLOGY AND MORPHOLOGY Hsin-I Hsieh: How Generative is Phonology? (On listing phonolog- ical surface forms in the lexicon) 109 Michael Kenstowicz: Rute Application in Pre-Generative American Phonology 145 Leonhard Lipka: Prolegomena to "Prolegomena to a Theory of Word- Formation":, A reply to Morris Halle 175 Royal Skousen: On the Nature of Morphophonemic Alternation . .185 Danny D. Steinberg and Robert K. -
LSA Update #128: Laurels to Linguists; Career Planning Webinar; LG
LSA Update #128: Laurels to Linguists; Career Planning Webinar; LG... https://us10.campaign-archive.com/?e=[UNIQID]&u=001f7eb7302f6ad... Subscribe Past Issues Translate Laurels to Linguists, Career Webinar, LGBTQ+ Special Interest Group, View this email in your browser and More! News from the Linguistic Society of America Update #128 - April 17, 2018 In This Issue: Laurels to Linguists Laurels to Linguists Career Webinar The LSA is Special Interest Group delighted to CoLang News announce that a Committee Appointments SALT News number of its In Memoriam members have Deadlines/Reminders recently Linguistics in the News received major awards and honors. Lenore Facebo Twitte Grenoble (University of Chicago), the LSA's Secretary-Treasurer, and Charles Yang (University of Pennsylvania), the recipient of Facebook Twitter the LSA's 2018 Leonard Bloomfield Book 1 of 5 4/16/2018, 3:22 PM LSA Update #128: Laurels to Linguists; Career Planning Webinar; LG... https://us10.campaign-archive.com/?e=[UNIQID]&u=001f7eb7302f6ad... Subscribe Past Issues Translate Award, both received fellowships from the John Follow the LSA on Simon Guggenheim Memorial Foundation. In addition, Marianne Mason (James Madison University) was Linked YouTu named a Fellow of the American Council of LinkedIn YouTube Learned Societies, and four LSA members -- Amy Fountain (University of Arizona), Jonathan Bobaljik (University of Connecticut), Shannon Bischoff (Indiana University-Purdue University Fort Wayne) and Patience Epps (University of Texas at Austin) -- received research grants from the National Endowment for the Humanities. Read more about the Guggenheim awardees here and about the ACLS and NEH awardees here. Moving Beyond Academia: Making A Smooth Career Transition Are you unsure about your next career move? Do you feel like everyone else has it figured out? Join us on April 25 for the latest in a series of webinars on career topics sponsored by the LSA's Special Interest Group (SIG) on Linguistics Beyond Academia. -
Distributed Morphology As a Regular Relation Marina Ermolaeva University of Chicago, [email protected]
Proceedings of the Society for Computation in Linguistics Volume 1 Article 20 2018 Distributed Morphology as a regular relation Marina Ermolaeva University of Chicago, [email protected] Daniel Edmiston University of Chicago, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/scil Part of the Computational Linguistics Commons Recommended Citation Ermolaeva, Marina and Edmiston, Daniel (2018) "Distributed Morphology as a regular relation," Proceedings of the Society for Computation in Linguistics: Vol. 1 , Article 20. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7275/R51834PC Available at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/scil/vol1/iss1/20 This Extended Abstract is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Proceedings of the Society for Computation in Linguistics by an authorized editor of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Distributed Morphology as a regular relation Marina Ermolaeva and Daniel Edmiston University of Chicago {mermolaeva,danedmiston}@uchicago.edu 1 Introduction structure (FS). An FS F is defined as a pair M, E , h i where the feature bundle M = feat(F ) is a sub- This research reorganizes the Distributed Morphol- set of some finite set of features (including syntac- ogy (DM, (Halle and Marantz, 1993)) framework tic category labels) and the phonological exponent to work over strings. That the morphological mod- E = exp(F ) (Σ None, ), where Σ is a fi- ule should operate over strings is desirable, since it ∈ ∪ { } nite set of phonemes. The default exponent of each is assumed that most (arguably all) morphological syntactic unit is None, a place-holder to be replaced processes can be modelled with regular languages by vocabulary insertion (VI) in the morphological (Karttunen et al. -
LSA Update #197: #LSA2021, Awards, Publications, and More!
LSA Update #197: #LSA2021, Awards, Publications, and More! https://us10.campaign-archive.com/?e=[UNIQID]&u=001f7eb7302f6ad... Subscribe Past Issues Translate Get ready for #LSA2021! View this email in your browser LSA Update News from the Linguistic Society of America Update #197 - December 28, 2020 In This Issue: Support Linguistics Scholarship Donate Today with a Year-End Donation to the #LSA2021 LSA Language Online Publications News As 2020 draws to a close, Awards and Honors Major Gift please consider a charitable Webinars donation to one of the LSA's In Case You Missed It contribution funds. Read Linguistics in the News more about how to donate, and how a special provision of the US CARES Act allows donors to deduct charitable contributions from your federal income taxes. Donations to the LSA general fund are particularly helpful in sustaining the LSA's core mission. You can also Facebook Twitter support the LSA while shopping online, now and at any time of the year. Follow the LSA on social media! LinkedIn YouTube Get ready for #LSA2021 The LSA's first-ever virtual Annual Meeting is 1 of 4 12/28/2020, 4:58 PM LSA Update #197: #LSA2021, Awards, Publications, and More! https://us10.campaign-archive.com/?e=[UNIQID]&u=001f7eb7302f6ad... Subscribe Past Issues Translate edge research, professional development events, and student-centered activities you've come to expect, all at a fraction of the usual cost! Click here for more information about the meeting, including registration, an online schedule, and abstracts, and click here for a news release (.pdf) about the meeting. -
Morris Halle (1923-2018)
Morris Halle (1923-2018) Paul Kiparsky Stanford University In 1940 Morris Halle, 17 years old, managed to emigrate in the nick of time with his parents from his native Latvia to the United States. After two years of engineering study in City College, New York, he was drafted into the U.S. Army and sent to Europe, where he ended up participating in the liberation of Paris. Discharged in 1946, he began to study Slavic and general linguistics, first at the University of Chicago, and then at Columbia University with Roman Jakobson, whom he followed to Harvard in 1949. In 1951 he was hired at MIT to teach Russian and German in the department of Modern Languages, and to work on phonetics in the Research Laboratory of Electronics (RLE), the peacetime incarnation of the Radiation Laboratory where much of the early work on radar had been done. Shabbily housed in the legendary Building 20, where it would later be joined by the Linguistics Department, RLE was a rich intellectual environment that reflected the scientific ferment of the postwar era, with electrical engineers, mathematicians, biologists, psychologists, as well as researchers on language engaged in a variety of projects on machine translation, acoustics, and speech communication. During this decade Halle steadily built up MIT’s linguistics course repertoire and piloted it into a full-fledged Ph.D. program, adroitly maneuvering in MIT’s intricate archipelago of departments and research laboratories. In 1953 he introduced a graduate course on “Hearing, Speech and Lan- guage” co-taught with Walter Rosenblith, a specialist in the electrophysiology of hearing, which was offered jointly by the Electrical Engineering Department and the Modern Language Depart- ment. -
Phonology in Generative Grammar
WORD ISSN: 0043-7956 (Print) 2373-5112 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rwrd20 Phonology in Generative Grammar Morris Halle To cite this article: Morris Halle (1962) Phonology in Generative Grammar, WORD, 18:1-3, 54-72, DOI: 10.1080/00437956.1962.11659765 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1962.11659765 Published online: 04 Dec 2015. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 1365 View related articles Citing articles: 182 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rwrd20 MORRIS HALLE------------------------- Phonology in Generative Grammar* A generative grammar is formally a collection of statements, rules or axioms which describe, define or generate all well-formed utterances in a language and only those. The theory of generative grammars consists of a set of abstract conditions which determine the form of the statements admitted in such grammars and which govern the choice among alternative descriptions of a given body of data.l In the part of the grammar that is of interest here, all statements are of the form (1a) A-4B in the environment X __Y z where A, B, X, Y, Z are symbols of a particular alphabet or zero, and"~" can be read "is to be rewritten as". The statements are, moreover, subject to a special notational convention which allows us to coalesce partly identical statements by factoring the parts that are identical. For instance, (la) and (1b) C-+D in the environment X__ y z can be coalesced into A-4B}. -
Roman Jakobson on Language by Linda R. Waugh and Monique Monville-Burston Review By: Morris Halle Source: Language, Vol
Linguistic Society of America Review Reviewed Work(s): Roman Jakobson on Language by Linda R. Waugh and Monique Monville-Burston Review by: Morris Halle Source: Language, Vol. 68, No. 1 (Mar., 1992), pp. 182-186 Published by: Linguistic Society of America Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/416378 Accessed: 09-07-2018 13:32 UTC JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://about.jstor.org/terms Linguistic Society of America is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Language This content downloaded from 18.40.21.93 on Mon, 09 Jul 2018 13:32:05 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms 182 LANGUAGE, VOLUME 68, NUMBER 1 (1992) and Peter Muhlhausler (Pacific Linguistics C-70), 443-83. Canberra: Australian National University. -- . 1986. Pidgin and creole languages. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. SINGLER, JOHN VICTOR. 1986. Short note. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 1.141- 45. TODD, LORETO. 1974. Pidgins and creoles. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Department of Linguistics [Received 2 August 1991.] New York University 719 Broadway, Room 502 New York, New York 10003 Roman Jakobson on language. Edited by LINDA R. WAUGH and MONIQUE MONVILLE-BURSTON. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press, 1990. -
Distributed Morphology and the Syntax/Morphology Interface
Distributed Morphology and the Syntax/Morphology Interface David Embick and Rolf Noyer University of Pennsylvania ***Draft of October 25, 2004*** (To appear in G. Ramchand and C. Reiss eds., The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Interfaces, Oxford University Press) 1 Introduction: The Syntax/Morphology Interface A theory of the syntax/morphology interface is first, a theory of how ‘words’ and their internal structure – the traditional domain of morphology – relate to the structures generated by the syntax, and second, a theory of how the rules for deriving complex words relate to the rules for deriving syntactic structures. A prominent line of research in this area consists of approaches assuming some version of the Lexicalist Hypothesis. For present purposes, this is the claim that (at least some) words are special in ways that e.g. phrases are not, and that this ‘specialness’ calls for an architecture in which the derivation of words and the derivation of syntactic objects occur in different modules of the grammar (the Lexicon versus the syntax).1 While the ‘words’ derived in the Lexicon serve as the terminals in the syntactic derivation, there is a sharp division between syntax and morphology according to this approach. In this way, the interface between syntax and morphology in such a theory is opaque or indirect: there is no reason to expect the structure and composition of ‘words’ to relate to the structure and composition of syntactic objects in any transparent or for that matter systematic fashion. A second line of research advances the hypothesis that ‘words’ are assembled by rules of the syntax. -
ABRALIN 2007 Course on Distributed Morphology
Distributed Morphology ABRALIN 2007, February 22-27, 2007 Heidi Harley, University of Arizona [email protected]; http://dingo.sbs.arizona.edu/~hharley/ Readings: Downloadable from the course home page, http://dingo.sbs.arizona.edu/~hharley/courses/ABRALIN/ABRALIN2007/ (Note: Schedule is only approximate! We will deviate from this if we need to.) Feb. 22 Introduction to (post-)syntactic morphology Harley and Noyer 2000: "Distributed Morphology" Bobaljik 2000: "The ins and outs of contextual allomorphy" Feb. 23 Complex verb structure Harley, 2006, "On the causative construction" Travis, 2000, "Event Structure in Syntax" Feb. 24 Irregularity, blocking and getting morphemes in order Embick, David. "Locality, Listedness and Morphological Identity" Barragan, Luis. "Movement and allomorphy in the Cupeno verb construction" Feb. 25 Phi-features and post-syntactic morphological operations Williams, Edwin. "Remarks on Lexical Knowledge" Bobaljik, Jonathan. "Syncretism without paradigms" (Frampton, John. "Syncretism, Impoverishment and the structure of person features") Feb. 26 DM as a psychological model Pfau, R. 2000. "Speech Errors and Distributed Morphology" Chapts 1, 2 and 4 only. Longtin, C-M et al. 2000"Morphological Priming Without Morphological Relationship" Feb. 27 Wrap-up: DM and semantics Marantz, Alec. 1997. "No escape from syntax: Don't try morphological analysis in the privacy of your own lexicon." Kratzer, Angelika. 1996. "Severing the external argument from its verb" Harley/ Distributed Morphology Lecture Notes ABRALIN 2007 1 Background: Distributed Morphology and the Syntax-Morphology Interface Harley and Noyer 2000: "Distributed Morphology" Some morphological terminology: stem for ‘suffix2’ [[prefix[[root]suffix1]]suffix2] root=stem for suffix1 stem for ‘prefix’ Example: [[in[[describ]abil]ity] derivation vs. -
Distributed Morphology, Spans, Portmanteaux, Suppletion
Nonlocal stem allomorphy 2 Answer from Bobaljik 2012: (7) a. α ... ]X0 ... β b.* α ... ] ... β Distributed Morphology, spans, portmanteaux, suppletion XP Answer from Embick 2010: Jason Merchant, University of Chicago (8) (A1) Insertion proceeds from the inside-out. LOT 2017 (9) (A2) Contextual allomorphy requires linear adjacency. (10) (A3) Two nodes can see each other for allomorphic purposes only when they are both active in the same cycle. 1 Locality for stem allomorphy, including suppletion (11) Complex head: a.Z (1) vP = λev.P (e) (2) a. PERFECTIVE = λPvt.λii. ev[P (e)& τ(e) i] Y Z J K ∃ ⊆ b. IMPERFECTIVE = λPvt.λii. ev[P (e)& t(t i)[t τ(e)]], ∃ Q ⊆ ⊆ X Y whereJ = (forK the progressive reading) or GEN (for the habitual) Q ∀ J K √ROOT X (3) a. b. Linearization: √ROOT X Y Z Tense − − − Aspect By (A1), VI occurs first at X, then at Y , then at Z. Thus, ... VI at Y could in principle Voice vVP see either phonological or morphosyntactic features of X but can look “outwards” only to morphosyntactic features of Z; and so on. In short, a node may show inward sensitivity ...V... to either morphosyntactic or phonological features, but it may show outward sensitivity b. Mirror Principle expectation: V-v-Voice-Aspect-Tense only to morphosyntactic features . by (A2) insertion at e.g. X could only be affected by √ROOT or Y . The reason for this is that only the Root and Y are concatenated with X. (4) a. Yo quise ir al circo. (Spanish) (Embick 2012:26) I wanted.PERF.1s go to.the circus ‘I wanted to go the circus.’ b.