Chapter 6 What comes first in language emergence? Wendy Sandler

There has been much speculation about what came first in the evolution ofhu- man language – repetitive syllables that took on meaning (MacNeilage 1998) or that provided a structural basis for syntax (Carstairs-McCarthy 1999); words (Bickerton 1990; Jackendoff 1999); undecomposable holophrases (e.g., Arbib 2012); or musical protolanguage (Darwin 1871; Fitch 2010; see Newmeyer 2002 and Fitch 2005 for informative overviews).1 Others have argued that the defining property at the evolutionary core of the human language faculty is syntactic recursion (Hauser, Chomsky & Fitch 2002), more recently described as a computational operation combining and recombining linguistic units (Bolhuis et al. 2014), or “discrete infinity” (Hauser et al. 2014). Whatever one takes to have been funda- mental, it is reasonable to assume that language must have evolved in stages, and that, in some cases, the emergence of one property must have depended on another that preceded it, in the sense that it could not have evolved without it. It is difficult to support, refute, or flesh out hypotheses about these stagesof evolution with evidence from spoken languages alone, because they are all thou- sands of years old, or descended from old languages, with their full linguistic structure intact. However, sign languages can arise anew at any time, and lin- guists look to them for clues to the course of language emergence. The fact that the emergence of sign languages can be observed in realtime does not guarantee that they will provide clues to the course of evolution of the human language capacity. If these young sign languages were to make their appearance replete with complex linguistic structures, they would be of little

1 Some theorists have proposed that spoken language emerged from gesture (Corballis 2002; Armstrong, Stokoe & Wilcox 1995; Arbib 2012). I do not deal with that issue here, but see also e.g., MacNeilage (1998); and Sandler (2013); Emmorey (2013); and other papers in Kemmerer (2013) for discussion.

Wendy Sandler. 2017. What comes first in language emergence? In N. J. En- field (ed.), Dependencies in language, 63–84. Berlin: Language Science Press. DOI:10.5281/zenodo.573788 Wendy Sandler help in determining how such structure emerged in evolution. It is only if they develop gradually, and if the stages in this process can be identified, that they might offer concrete contemporary evidence of the path of language emergence. Here I will identify such evidence in a new that arose in relative isolation, to show that modest linguistic machinery – holistic words and prosodic organization of semantically related words – are the first things to emerge, and that they are enough to support fully functional language. Other, more compu- tational, aspects of linguistic form, such as phonological, morphological,2 and syntactic structuring, are later arrivals, apparently requiring the prior scaffold- ing provided by simplex words and by prosodic constituents that temporally or- ganize semantically related units and characterize them with intonation.3 Of course, it cannot be assumed that the emergence of new sign languages in biologically modern humans faithfully replicates the evolution of language in our species. But the modernity of these languages does not nullify their significance in the context of evolution, and it would be a mistake to dismiss them. Emerg- ing sign languages offer an exciting opportunity to identify two central facets of language emergence that no other naturally occurring system can provide. One is the nature of the communicative elements that are required minimally in order for a system to function as language. The other facet, relevant for the theme of this volume, is the path along which one kind of structure follows, or is dependent on, another over time before arriving at the kind of rule governed complexity in language that we often take for granted. In this sense, new sign languages can offer a uniquely empirical and plausible reference point for models of language evolution. New sign languages have a heuristic advantage over spoken languages in an- other way as well. The nature of the physical system, in which movements of different parts of the body (the two hands, the head, the face, the torso) visually manifest different linguistic functions, makes it possible for linguists to match form to function more directly than they can for spoken languages, and literally to see it unfold (Sandler 2012a). I refer to this correspondence between the recruit- ment of articulators for linguistic purposes and language form as the Grammar of the Body.

2 Sign languages in general have certain types of modality-typical complex morphology (e.g., Aronoff, Meir & Sandler 2005). We were surprised not to have found this complexity at the morphological level in Al Sayyid Bedouin Sign Language, although the beginnings of a system can be discerned in compounds. See Meir et al. (2010) and Padden et al. (2010) for treatments of the emergence of morphology in ABSL. 3 I am assuming here that prosody includes intonation as well as rhythm (timing) and stress.

64 6 What comes first in language emergence?

Investigation of Al-Sayyid Bedouin Sign Language (ABSL), a young sign lan- guage that arose in relative isolation, has shown that a language does not spring forth fully formed, but rather evolves gradually across generations (see Aronoff et al. 2008; Sandler et al. 2014 for overviews).4 Studying this language in differ- ent age groups, and tracing the step-by-step recruitment of different articulators to create a linguistic system (Sandler 2012a), allows us to observe the gradual emergence of linguistic form over time. Our data suggest that language develops very efficiently, first, by creating holistic units to signify concepts – words with no phonology. This is followed by combining words into short propositions and later into larger discourse units, and organizing them prosodically into a fully functional linguistic system. Word order comes in early as well (Sandler et al. 2005), although we now have reason to believe that it is determined by the fundamental opposition between human and inanimate referents, and not by syntax (Meir et al. 2017). I will extrapolate from our findings on Al-Sayyid Bedouin Sign Language to propose that certain basic elements of language must be present before other components commonly thought of as fundamental can arise. First, the crystal- lization of phonology depends on conventionalization of lexical items, which in turn depends on repeated social interactions with the same social group. These factors lead to automaticity, which results in a split between form and meaning. This split paves the way for duality of patterning (Hockett 1960) – meaningful and meaningless (phonological) levels of structure. The second two related prop- erties are prosody and syntax. In ABSL, prosodic structure organizes semantic relations in the absence of concrete evidence for any syntactic means of marking the same relations. With little evidence for syntax in ABSL, I conclude that syn- tactic structure is not a prerequisite for the emergence of prosodic organization. The pattern of emergence we see suggests that central properties of language that are considered universal – phonology and autonomous syntax – do not come ready-made in the human brain, and that a good deal of language can be present without clear evidence for them. I begin with a snapshot of the Grammar of the

4 As Keren Rice pointed out to me, no criteria are offered for measuring whether language emer- gence is gradual or abrupt, and indeed, the characterization depends a lot on one’s expectations. Coming from the generative tradition that attributes a fair amount of linguistic structure to innate propensities, our group was surprised by the lack of much linguistic structure in the early stages of ABSL, and by the seemingly arduous path to its accrual and conventionaliza- tion, leading us us to characterize emergence of linguistic form as gradual. For an overview of our ABSL findings, see Sandler et al. 2014.

65 Wendy Sandler

Body in established sign languages to show how linguistic structure manifests itself in these visual languages5, and then go on to emergence.

1 The Grammar of the Body

Sign languages are sometimes described as manual languages because the hands convey words, the most essential linguistic units. But sign languages also sys- tematically exploit the whole upper body to convey language: movements of the head, facial articulators, and the torso, and independent use of the nondominant hand. Different movements of the extra-manual bodily articulators individually and in combination convey important elements of structure, including subordi- nation, adjectival- or adverbial-type modification, contrast, intonation, and more, as shown in Figure 3 below.6 The two levels to be traced here are the word and prosody/intonation. In established sign languages, words have phonological structure: different configurations of the fingers, orientations of the palm, and movements ofthe hand on or near different body locations are combined to create signs and to dis- tinguish them from one another, and they are altered in phonological processes such as assimilation (Stokoe 1960; Sandler 1989; Liddell & Johnson 1989; Brentari 1998). Figure 1 shows a minimal pair in (ISL) distinguished by differences in major place of articulation alone. A sign in sign language roughly corresponds to a word in spoken language: it bears a conventionalized form-meaning relation and is constrained in form both phonotactically (Battison 1978; Mandel 1981) and prosodically (Sandler 1999). Signs are typically monosyllabic, characterized by a single movement of the hands from one location to another. Even morphologically complex signs are usually monosyllabic, since grammatical morphemes are nonconcatenatively (si- multaneously) overlaid on the base sign, by changes in locations, types of move- ment, and/or rhythm, and with particular conventionalized facial expressions (Sandler 1999). At the level of phrasal prosody, manual timing establishes rhythm, and facial expression and head movement function systematically as intonation (Nespor & Sandler 1998; Dachkovsky, Healy & Sandler 2013). To prepare for the discussion

5 For comprehensive treatments of sign language linguistic structure at all levels, see Sandler & Lillo-Martin (2006) and Pfau, Steinbach & Woll (2012). 6 There is a large literature on nonmanual linguistic use of the body in sign languages. See Pfau & Quer (2010), Sandler (2012b), and a special issue of Sign Language and Linguistics (2011), Hermann and Steinbach (Eds.).

66 6 What comes first in language emergence?

Figure 1: Minimal pair in Israeli Sign Language distinguished by place of articulation: (a) SEND (torso) and (b) TATTLE (head) of prosody as an early feature of ABSL, a brief discussion of the way the body expresses prosody in sign languages is in order. In an established sign language, the end of an intonational phrase is signaled by phrase final lengthening on the hands, coordinated with a change in facial expression and head position.7 Figure 2 shows the boundary between the two in- tonational phrases in the Israeli Sign Language sentence glossed roughly [[DOG SMALL THAT] [WEEK-AGO I FIND IT]] // [[ESCAPE]] meaning ‘The little dog that I found last week // ran away.’8 Figure 2 shows that there is an across the board change in facial expression and head position between the end of the first constituent (…FIND IT) and the second (ESCAPE).9 In this sentence of ISL, the dependency between the two constituents is indi- cated by raised brows and head forward and down at the end of the first major constituent, the sentence topic, and by an across the board change of face and head configurations for the second, the comment. Squinted eyes indicate shared information – the little dog that the signer and addressee know about – a reliable signal for relative clauses (Nespor & Sandler 1998; Dachkovsky & Sandler 2009). The nondominant hand retains its shape and position from ‘small dog’ through- out the first constituent (through ‘find it’), signaling topic continuity. Thismeans that the anaphoric pronoun ‘it’ and the topic antecedent ‘small dog’ overlap tem-

7 These intonational phrase markers are documented for two unrelated sign languages: Israeli and American (Dachkovsky, Healy & Sandler 2013). 8 The first intonational phrase in the sentence is comprised of two lower level phonological phrases. 9 In the context of language typology featured in this volume, it is worth mentioning that well studied established sign languages seem to have similar articulator-to-linguistic function cor- respondence to that shown in Figure 3, and thus constitute a language type.

67 Wendy Sandler

Figure 2: Complete change in facial expression and head position at intonational phrase boundary between (a) [[…IT]] and (b) [[ESCAPE]] i.e., between the topic, ’The little dog that I found a week ago,’ andthe comment, ’ran away’. porally in the signal, as do the intonational and rhythmic markings of prosodic structure. In Figure 3a, a close-up of Figure 2a, the articulators are labeled for the specific functions they convey at the end of the first constituent. Figure 3blists some of the linguistic functions conveyed by movements of articulators in the language generally. This complex simultaneous layering of bodily signals sys- tematically organizes information in sign language sentences (Wilbur 2000). We can now turn to the order of emergence of the two pairs of structures of interest here: words and phonology, and prosody and syntax. In the case of words, it is commonly believed that it would not be possible to amass a large vocabulary with holistic signals, and that a lower level of recombin- able meaningless units (i.e., phonology) must have been a prerequisite for a large lexicon (Hockett 1960; Pinker & Jackendoff 2005). As for prosody, two compet- ing predictions can be put forward, either prosody and then syntax or syntax and then prosody. Specifically, it has been hypothesized that, in a young language, such as a pidgin, prosody might precede syntactic marking to indicate different sentence types and subordination (Givón 1979). On the other hand, synchronic linguistic theory typically points in the opposite direction, holding that prosodic constituents are projected from syntactic constituents (Selkirk 1984; Nespor & Vogel 1986).

68 6 What comes first in language emergence?

(a)

head forward and down: TOPIC FINAL

brow raise: CONTINUATION squint: SHARED INFORMATION

torso tilt: DISCOURSE CONTRAST

nondominant hand: DISCOURSE TOPIC hand: CONTINUITY WORD 'SMALL DOG' 'IT'

(b) • Eyeballs: gaze (pointing; questioning; referential shift) • Head: topic marking; question marking; prominence; continuation/dependency; referential shift; constituent boundary marking • Upper Face (brows, lids, cheeks): utterance type and information status (questions; old information; focus, etc.); constituent boundary marking (with blink); character perspective • Lower Face (tongue lips, cheeks): adj., adv. modification; mouthing of spoken words • Torso: referential shift; discourse contrast • Hand(s): words (phonology; morphology); rhythm; prominence; boundary strength • Nondominant Hand: phonological element in words; independent classifier morpheme; discourse topic continuity

Figure 3: (a) Functions signalled by movement of articulators at the end of the topic constituent. (b) list of functions signaled by various articulators in the language generally.

69 Wendy Sandler

It is striking that neither in the case of words/phonology nor of prosody/syn- tax, do these paired elements appear at the same time in ABSL. Instead, one precedes the other: the language accrues a relatively large lexicon before phono- logical structure crystallizes, and prosodic markers of relations such as coordina- tion and dependency between propositions appear in the absence of identifiable syntactic marking of these relations. While there is already evidence for the be- ginnings of phonology, there is in fact very little in the way of overt syntax even in third generation ABSL signers.

2 Words first, phonology later

We have followed the emergence of ABSL by recording and analyzing the lan- guage of people of different ages in the village. This investigation reveals that the word is the first linguistic unit to appear, and that this symbolic pairing of form and meaning is at the heart of human language (Sandler 2013). Zooming in to the structure of words in an emerging language shows a considerable amount of variation as well as the beginnings of structure.

2.1 Lexical form Our earliest data consist of a videotaped story told by an elderly man who was one of the first four deaf children born into one family in the village. Hisut- terances consist mainly of a series of one or two word-like manual signs, e.g., RIFLE, or HORSE RUN, occasionally interspersed with pantomimic movement of the whole body, e.g., ‘strike-with-sword’.10,11 Restriction of linguistic form to the hands is in stark contrast with the linguis- tic uses of the body schematized in Figure 3. Given the availability of the whole body, and the complex and systematic use of different parts of the body in es- tablished sign languages, it is striking that only the hands are used for linguistic function at the beginning of language (Sandler 2012a), to symbolize word-level concepts. In fact, the language used by this first generation signer is as simple and vague as the content of his story is detailed and complex, suggesting that a high level of cognitive complexity is possible without a concomitant degree of linguistic

10 Pantomimic use of the body means that the body represents a human body performing some action: the hands are hands; the head is a head; the torso is a torso. 11 Some utterances in the narrative have more words in a constituent, including what mightbe analyzed as a complex sentence or two. However, the majority of utterances are minimal, and often vague, as exemplified here.

70 6 What comes first in language emergence? complexity. The story comes from the history of Al-Sayyid, and was translated for us by the man’s hearing son, who filled in a good deal of information shared by members of the community which was necessary for understanding the story but was not overtly conveyed. Studying vocabulary in ABSL generally, we were surprised to find quite a lot of variation in lexical items across this small community, with more convergence within families, prompting us to coin the term, “familylect”. Certain patterns can be identified at the level of the word, such as iconically motivated regularities in lexeme formation (Padden et al. 2013; Lepic et al. 2016). The only evidence we have found of complexity at the word level is in the formation of compounds, which show considerable variation in structure, with the exception of a language- particular subset involving classifier morphemes that typically follow anoun (Meir et al. 2010; Sandler et al. 2011b).

2.2 Articulatory variation: no crystallized phonological system In our investigation of sign production across the community, we also found a surprising amount of articulatory variation in the production of the same lexi- cal item ( & Sandler 2011). In this way, the words of ABSL are unlike the words of more established sign languages because they function as iconic wholes, and we concluded that a phonological system has not yet crystallized across the community (Sandler et al. 2011b). Our team created a dictionary with 300 entries, presumably only a fraction of the lexicon in the language, since the signs had mostly been elicited through pic- ture naming and the majority are thus concrete nouns. Yet, despite a relatively large vocabulary, we could not detect evidence of a discrete, systematic, mean- ingless level of structure. Even broad phonological specifications in established sign languages, such as major place of articulation categories, on a par with LA- BIAL or DORSAL in spoken languages, varied across signers for the same sign, as exemplified in Figure 4 for the sign DOG. The two places of articulation shown here, head and torso, are major place categories and contrastive in more estab- lished sign languages (cf., SEND and TATTLE in ISL, Figure 1). We did discover kernels of phonology. For example, we encountered signs among younger signers whose form had been consistently altered to accommo- date ease of articulation, resulting in signs that are counter-iconic. This suggests that smaller units of meaningless form are taking precedence over iconic, holistic signals. Within what we dubbed a “familylect”, we also found consistent form- based handshape assimilation in a frequently used compound rendering it, too, non-iconic, and suggesting the beginning of a phonological level (Sandler et al.

71 Wendy Sandler

Figure 4: The ABSL sign DOG signed by different signers at twodif- ferent places of articulation, the head (a) and torso (b). The same two places of articulation are contrastive in established sign languages (see Figure 1).

2011b; Sandler 2014). We deduce from these studies that the emergence of phonol- ogy, at least in a contemporary sign language, depends first on the conventional- ization of words and then on frequency of use and automaticity. The answer to the empirical question of how many meaningful holistic signals humans can pro- duce and perceive in the vocal/auditory modality is not known, and it is possible that sign languages can tolerate a larger number than spoken languages can, due to the iconicity of form and the nature of visual perception. But even if there is some difference between modalities in this regard, ABSL shows surprisingly that it is possible for a functioning human language to have a relatively large vocab- ulary without a crystallized phonological system, making phonology dependent, in the sense intended here, on a stable, conventionalized, and frequently shared lexicon.

3 Prosodic organization first, syntax later

How are these words combined into meaningful utterances? In established lan- guages, prosodic signals – rhythm, intonation, and phrasal stress – are typically coextensive with syntactic constituents such as the phrase or the clause. It has been argued that phrasal stress is determined by the order of heads and com- plements in a language (Nespor & Vogel 1986), and that children, sensitive to prosody of their native language since infancy (e.g., Mehler & Dupoux 1994; Jusczyk 1997), use the prominence patterns of prosody to bootstrap the syntactic structure (e.g., Nespor, Guasti & Christophe 1996).

72 6 What comes first in language emergence?

Because of this syntax-prosody correspondence, linguists propose that the prosody is read off the syntax, and is in this sense dependent onit(Selkirk 1984; Nespor & Vogel 1986). Given these observations, one might expect syntactic structure to be a prerequisite for prosodic structure in a new language. This pre- diction runs contrary to that of Givón (1979) and others who reason that prosody is likely to precede syntax in young languages. The difference between these two views may depend to some extent onwhat one calls syntax. Our approach throughout has been to refrain from attributing autonomous syntactic form to an expression in ABSL without explicit evidence for it.12 We find word groupings by meaning and even consistencies in word or- der (Sandler et al. 2005), but no evidence so far that favors autonomous syntactic structure over a much more basic driving force. In a recent and detailed study, Meir et al. (2017) show that word order in new sign languages and in gesture (without speech) is governed by the salience of human referents and not by syn- tactic rules.13 In ABSL, the groupings of words into constituents and the relations between them are marked by prosody, which emerges gradually over time in the community (Sandler et al. 2011a). On the whole, evidence from a small sample of narratives in four ABSL age groups suggests that prosody – consisting of timing and intonation – is the ear- liest organizing force, and that it emerges gradually. This overall picture is tem- pered by the fact that certain indications of syntactic relations within clauses begin to appear together with intonational marking of dependency across them. The findings are summarized in Tables 1 and 2. We are currently investigating these preliminary results further, across three young sign languages. Age group 1. As I pointed out in the introduction, the story told by the oldest signer (age group 1), is characterized largely (though not exclusively) by one or two-word propositions, separated by pauses. Only the hands are recruited for linguistic components.

12 Apart from overt markers, syntactic tests can identify syntactic structure. For example, early research on American Sign Language distinguished coordinate from subordinate clauses by the coreference properties of a process called final subject pronoun copy (Padden 1988). In ABSL we have not found syntactic processes of this kind, nor do we see evidence of morphosyntax, such as verb agreement (Padden et al. 2010), although it is common in established sign lan- guages (Aronoff, Meir & Sandler 2005), or case marking. While one cannot rule out the covert presence of syntactic structure driving the prosodic structure we see, neither can we identify evidence for its existence. The more parsimonious account, therefore, is one that takes prosody as the prior mechanism for organizing and relating essentially semantic constituents. 13 Based on word orders of ABSL and other new sign languages, and on experimental work with gesture, Meir et al. (2017) found that human arguments occur before inanimate arguments, irrespective of their syntactic or semantic roles.

73 Wendy Sandler

Table 1: Recruitment of additional articulators for grammatical func- tions according to age group, from oldest (group 1, the earliest stage of the language) to youngest (group 4, the later stage)

Age group Hands Head Face Body Nondominant hand 1 × 2 × × 3 × × × 4 × × × × ×

Table 2: Complexity added through recruitment of additional articula- tors for linguistic functions (adapted from Sandler et al. 2011a; Sandler 2012a)

Age Complex sentences Discourse/reference group Words cohesion 1 Signs 2 Signs Unsystematic clause linking (coordination); 1 NP per 2.5 predicates (vague one-word constitutents); 1st person subject pronouns only 3 Signs Many dependent constituents Parentheticals, (conditionals, temporal reported speech expressions, reported speech); 1-2 NPs per predicate; 3rd person pronouns 4 Signs Addition of modifiers, Addition of topic quantifiers, embedding inside continuity marker reported speech (double and torso shift for embedding) different discourse referents

74 6 What comes first in language emergence?

Age group 2. In the second age group (short stretches of narratives of two people in the study reported in Sandler et al. 2011a), movement of the head was added to the hands to separate constituents. Some separated constituents were lists, and some (e.g., temporal expressions such as DAYS THREE meaning ‘for three days’) were related semantically to adjacent propositions, but no special syntactic or prosodic marking distinguished these from coordinated units. Many propositions in this age group did not associate nominal arguments with verbs in the same constituent, and no pronouns were used except occasionally first person (pointing to the chest). Age group 3. In the third age group (short stretches of narratives of two younger people), facial expression was added to show continuation/dependency between constituents such as conditionals, and, together with head position, to signal parentheticals in a discourse. Although utterances clearly involve sub- ordination semantically (e.g., in conditionals), this subordination is not marked syntactically – no complementizers, time adverbials, or conditional expressions like ‘if’. Instead it is marked with prosodic signals of timing of the hands and intonation of the face and head. Together with prosodic signaling of dependency between clauses, we see some- what richer structure within clauses: verbs are more likely to occur with nominal arguments, and third person pronouns – abstract syntactic elements – are com- mon. Relations between clauses are signaled prosodically by timing and intona- tion, and not syntactically, but a tendency that might be considered syntactic is emerging: an increase in overt arguments associated with verbs, some of them pronominal forms. We see no implicational relation between these syntactic ele- ments within clauses and the prosody connecting them, however. While we cannot rule out the covert presence of syntactic structure driving the prosodic structure we see, neither can we identify evidence for its existence. The more parsimonious account, therefore, is one that takes prosody as theprior mechanism for organizing and relating essentially semantic constituents. We conclude that the mechanism for connecting clauses and indicating dependency relations between them is prosodic, and that syntactic mechanisms serving this function have not (yet) arisen. For further discussion of what you can say without syntax, see Jackendoff & Wittenberg’s (2014) paper with that title. Age group 4. We are just beginning to analyze the language of age group 4. The narrative of a single signer in the fourth age group was chosen for analysis for two reasons: he is the oldest of five deaf siblings in one household and his deaf

75 Wendy Sandler mother and hearing father know only ABSL and no ISL,14 so that the young man is able to distinguish the two languages and provide a good example of “pure”, fluent ABSL in his age group. In his signing we found refinement and coordination of the nonmanual signals for subordination/dependency (cf. ISL example in Figure 3). Even double embed- ding of constituents occurs. An example is an utterance translated (with the help of prosody) as, “Father (said to) me about marriage, ‘If you marry a deaf girl, all of your children will be deaf. No way.’” The boldface constituent in the gloss has conditional prosody: FATHER ME MARRIAGE, DEAF TWO DEAF BOTH MARRY, OFFSPRING DEAF ALL – REJECT. As with age group 3 signers, this embedding of one proposition within another is signaled by prosody only and not by overt morpho-syntactic elements such as a conditional word like ’if’. In his narrative, the signer added the nondominant hand for topic continuity (essentially, discourse level coreference) and shifts in body posture to identify ref- erents in a discourse. All of these phenomena are structural advances over the narratives of the earlier stages of the language of the older people studied. Ta- ble 3 is a gloss and translation to English of an excerpt in which he describes the vocations (professions) he had to choose from at vocational school. A parenthet- ical segment is set off in the gloss by square brackets. The large curly bracket along the side indicates the stretch of signing during which the nondominant hand is held in the signal to mark continuity of the topic – ‘the third vocation’ (welding) – dropping to his side at the end of the discourse segment relating to the topic. Figure 5 illustrates the physical manifestation of linguistic properties of the utterance. The signer’s budding Grammar of the Body may not yetbeas systematic and complex as that of more established sign languages, but it has the scaffolding in place.

14 The young people of the Al-Sayyid village have had a good deal of exposure to signsfrom Israeli Sign Language in school settings, while exposure to ISL grammatical structure asitis signed by deaf people is limited. In school, the teachers speak Arabic, accompanied by ISL signs. This is not ISL, since the grammar of the sign language is very different fromthatof the spoken language, and, as with other sign-supported speech systems, when both channels are used at the same time, one or the other (usually the sign language) is seriously disrupted. Some of the young deaf men in Al-Sayyid (including the Group 4 example discussed here) did have extended exposure to ISL in their late teens when they attended a mixed vocational high school (Jewish and Arab pupils with ISL signing deaf teachers), now closed down. The bottom line is that people under the age of 30 have had considerable exposure to ISL vocabulary and sporadic, uneven exposure to its grammatical structure. A general description of the spoken and signed linguistic mosaic in Israel is offered in Sandler (2014).

76 6 What comes first in language emergence?

Table 3: Excerpt from 4th age group signer’s narrative (from Sandler 2012a)

Gloss Translation   ONE COOKING  [[One, cooking, two, mechanics, three,  TWO MECHANICS  welding.   THREE WELDING  [I LONG-AGO I SMALL [Long ago, when I was small, my father FATHER ME HE WELD  was a welder. I remembered it well and  REMEMBER WELL  didn’t want that, not welding.]  NOT, REJECT]   FOUR, COMPUTERS  Four computers, all the professions.]] ALL PROFESSIONS

ME MECHANICS. I wanted mechanics.

Figure 5: Use of the body for grammatical functions (from Sandler 2012a).

77 Wendy Sandler

4 Conclusion

From a grammatical point of view, ABSL across the community is relatively sim- ple. Nevertheless, the semantic/cognitive conceptualization and relations it re- flects are far from simple. With these conceptualizations and relations, andmini- mal linguistic machinery, ABSL functions as a full language. Its users talk about life histories, folk remedies no longer in use, dreams, fertility, deafness, national insurance, wedding preparations, suspicions, personal relations – all fluently, without hesitation or pantomimic “acting out”, and without noticeable commu- nication failures. While further grammatical structures may develop over time, it seems that fully functional language is possible with relatively simple linguistic structure (see Klein & Perdue 1997; Gil 2005; Jackendoff & Wittenberg 2014 for more support for this claim). ABSL and other new languages provide novel evidence for theories about the relation between community structure and language structure (Meir et al. 2012). For example, the language of age groups 1 and 2 corresponds to Bernstein’s (1971) notion of a restricted code used in circumstances where the speakers share knowledge and assumptions. A restricted code is economical in that it can con- vey a good deal of meaning with a few words, as speakers can rely on the shared knowledge of their interlocutors to interpret what they say. We have reported elsewhere that tolerance of irregularity, in the form of lex- ical variation and variation in the order of constituents in compounds in the Al- Sayyid village, reported in Meir et al. (2010), is compatible with Wray & Grace’s (2007) conception of an esoteric code. Acquired in childhood and used within a homogeneous group with shared culture and environment, esoteric codes are characterized by irregularities of form that are less typical of more regular exo- teric codes, used with outsiders.15 The overview presented here suggests that the emergence of a crystallized phonological system follows – in other words, depends on – the prior existence of a sizable, conventionalized lexicon. As for the emergence of prosody and syntax, our findings suggest that an autonomous syntax is not a prerequisite for prosody, or, in other words, that prosody does not depend on syntax. Prosody is a critical factor in organizing semantic relations relatively early in a language, while overt indications of syntax have yet to emerge. Language needs this basic scaffolding of words and prosody, which emerges gradually over a few generations, and it

15 As a very young language, ABSL has not had a chance to develop characteristics attributed to esoteric codes such as morphophonemic alternations and irregular morphological paradigms.

78 6 What comes first in language emergence? seems that it is all the linguistic machinery you need for a perfectly good human language. Simple maybe, compared to millennia-old languages. But no other species even comes close.

Acknowledgements

The research on ISL is supported by grant 580/09 from the Israel Science Founda- tion to Irit Meir and the author. The ABSL research is supported by a grant from the U.S. Israel Binational Science Foundation and by NIH grant R01 DC006473. The Grammar of the Body project is supported by a grant to the author bythe European Research Council, project 340140. Many thanks to Nick Enfield and other participants for useful and stimulating discussions at the meeting on De- pendencies among Systems of Language, Ardennes, June 2014. I am grateful to Mark Aronoff, Ray Jackendoff, and Keren Rice for their insightful comments and suggestions on this paper.

References

Arbib, Michael. 2012. How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Armstrong, David F., William C. Stokoe & Sherman E. Wilcox. 1995. Gesture and the nature of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Aronoff, Mark, Irit Meir & Wendy Sandler. 2005. The paradox of sign language morphology. Language 81. 301–344. Aronoff, Mark, Irit Meir, Carol Padden & Wendy Sandler. 2008. The roots oflin- guistic organization in a new language. In Derek Bickerton & Michael Arbib (eds.), Holophrasis, compositionality and protolanguage (special issue of Interac- tion Studies), 133–149. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Battison, Robbin. 1978. Lexical borrowing in American Sign Language. Silver Spring, MD: Linstok Press. Bernstein, Basil. 1971. Class, codes and control, volume 1. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Bickerton, Derek. 1990. Language and species. Chicago: Chicago University Press. Bolhuis, Johan, Ian Tattersal, Noam Chomsky & Robert Berwick. 2014. How could language have evolved? PLOS Biology 12(8). 1–7. Brentari, Diane. 1998. A prosodic model of sign language phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

79 Wendy Sandler

Carstairs-McCarthy, Andrew. 1999. The origins of complex language. Oxford: Ox- ford University Press. Corballis, Michael C. 2002. From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Prince- ton: Princeton University Press. Dachkovsky, Svetlana, Christina Healy & Wendy Sandler. 2013. Visual intonation in two sign languages. Phonology 30(2). 211–252. Dachkovsky, Svetlana & Wendy Sandler. 2009. Visual intonation in the prosody of a sign. Language and Speech 52(2-3). 287–314. Darwin, Charles. 1871. The descent of man and selection in relation. tosex London: John Murray. Emmorey, Karen. 2013. The neurobiology of sign language and the mirror system hypothesis. In David Kemmerer (ed.), Language and cognition 5/2-3, 205–210. Berlin: De Gruyter. Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2005. The evolution of language: A comparative view. Biol- ogy and Philosophy 20. 193–230. Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2010. The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni- versity Press. Gil, David. 2005. Word order without syntactic categories: How Riau Indonesian does it. In H. Harley A. Carnie & S.A. Dooley (eds.), Verb first: On the syntax of verb-initial languages, 243–262. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Givón, Talmy. 1979. From discourse to syntax: Grammar as a processing strategy. In Talmy Givón (ed.), Discourse and syntax (Syntax and Semantics, vol. 12), 81–111. New York: Academic Press. Hauser, Marc, Noam Chomsky & W. Tecumseh Fitch. 2002. The faculty of lan- guage: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298. 1569–1579. Hauser, Marc, Charles Yang, Robert Berwick, Ian Tattersall, Michael Ryan, Jef- frey Watumull, Noam Chomsky & Richard Lewontin. 2014. The mystery of language evolution: Frontiers in psychology. Language Sciences 401(5). 1–12. Hockett, Charles F. 1960. The origin of speech. Scientific American 203. 89–96. Israel, Assaf & Wendy Sandler. 2011. Phonological category resultion: A a study of handshapes in younger and older sign languages. In Rachel Channon & Harry van der Hulst (eds.), Formational units in sign language, vol. 3, 177–202. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Jackendoff, Ray. 1999. Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity. Trends in Cognitive Science 3. 272–279. Jackendoff, Ray & Eva Wittenberg. 2014. What you can say without syntax: Ahi- erarchy of grammatical complexity. In Frederick Newmeyer & Lauren Preston

80 6 What comes first in language emergence?

(eds.), Measuring grammatical complexity, 65–83. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Jusczyk, Peter. 1997. The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. Kemmerer, David (ed.). 2013. Language and cognition. 5(2-3): Special issue in re- sponse to arbib (2012). Klein, Wolfgang & Clive Perdue. 1997. The basic variety (or: Couldn’t natural languages be much simpler?) Second Language Research 13. 301–347. Lepic, Ryan, Carl Börstell, Gal Belsitzman & Wendy Sandler. 2016. Taking mean- ing in hand: Iconic motivation in two-handed signs. Sign Language and Lin- guistics 19(1). 37–81. Liddell, Scott K. & Robert E. Johnson. 1989. American Sign Language: The phono- logical base. Sign Language Studies 64. 195–277. MacNeilage, Peter F. 1998. The frame/content theory of evolution of speech pro- duction. Behavior and Brain Science 21. 499 – 546. Mandel, Mark. 1981. Phonotactics and morphophonology in ASL. University of Cal- ifornia, Berkeley PhD thesis. Mehler, Jacques & Emmanuel Dupoux. 1994. What infants know. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell. Meir, Irit, Mark Aronoff, Wendy Sandler & Carol Padden. 2010. Sign languages and compounding. In Sergio Scalise & Irene Vogel (eds.), Compounding, 301– 322. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Meir, Irit, Assaf Israel, Wendy Sandler, Carol Padden & Mark Aronoff. 2012. The influence of community on language structure: Evidence from two young sign languages. Linguistic Variation 12(2). 247–291. Meir, Irit, Mark Aronoff, Carl Börstell, So-One Hwang, Deniz Ikbasaran, Itamar Kastner, Ryan Lepic, Adi Lifshitz Ben Basat, Carol Padden & Wendy Sandler. 2017. The effect of being human and the basis of grammatical word order:In- sights from novel communication systems and young sign languages. Cogni- tion 158. 189–207. Nespor, Marina, Maria-Theresa Guasti & Anne Christophe. 1996. Selecting word order: The rhythmic activation principle. In Ursula Kleinhenz (ed.), Interfaces in phonology, 1–26. Berlin: Akademie Verlag. Nespor, Marina & Wendy Sandler. 1998. Prosodic phonology in Israeli Sign Lan- guage. Language and Speech 52(2-3). 143–176. Nespor, Marina & Irene Vogel. 1986. Prosodic phonology. Dordrecht: Foris. Newmeyer, Frederick J. 2002. Uniformitarian assumptions and language evolu- tion research. In Alison Wray (ed.), The transition to language, 359–375. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

81 Wendy Sandler

Padden, Carol. 1988. Interaction of morphology and syntax in American Sign Lan- guages (Outstanding Dissertations in Linguistics). New York: Garland. Padden, Carol, Irit Meir, Mark Aronoff & Wendy Sandler. 2010. The grammar of space in two new sign languages. In Diane Brentari (ed.), Sign languages, 573– 595. New York: Cambridge University Press. Padden, Carol, Irit Meir, So-One Hwang, Ryan Lepic & Sharon Seegers. 2013. Where do nouns come from? Gesture 13(3). 287–308. Pfau, Roland & Josep Quer. 2010. Nonmanuals: Their grammatical and prosodic roles. In Diane Brentari (ed.), Sign languages. 381–402. New York: Cambridge University Press. Pfau, Roland, Markus Steinbach & Bencie Woll (eds.). 2012. Sign language: An international handbook. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. Pinker, Steven & Ray Jackendoff. 2005. The faculty of language: What’s special about it? Cognition 95. 201–236. Sandler, Wendy. 1989. Phonological representation of the sign: Linearity and non- linearity in American Sign Language. Dordrecht: Foris. Sandler, Wendy. 1999. Cliticization and prosodic words in sign language. In Tracy Hall & Ursula Kleinhenz (eds.), Studies on the phonological word, 223–255. Am- sterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Sandler, Wendy. 2012a. Dedicated gestures and the emergence of sign language. Gesture 12(3). 265–307. Sandler, Wendy. 2012b. Visual prosody. In Roland Pfau, Markus Steinbach & Ben- cie Woll (eds.), Sign language: An international handbook, 55–76. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. Sandler, Wendy. 2013. Vive la différence: Sign language and spoken language in language evolution. Language and Cognition 5(2-3). 189–203. Sandler, Wendy. 2014. The emergence of phonetic and phonological features in sign language. Nordlyd 41(1). 183–212. Sandler, Wendy & Diane Lillo-Martin. 2006. Sign language and linguistic univer- sals. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press. Sandler, Wendy, Irit Meir, Carol Padden & Mark Aronoff. 2005. The emergence of grammar in a new sign language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102(7). 2661–2665. Sandler, Wendy, Irit Meir, Svetlana Dachkovsky, Carol Padden & Mark Aronoff. 2011a. The emergence of complexity in prosody and syntax. Lingua 121. 2014– 2033.

82 6 What comes first in language emergence?

Sandler, Wendy, Mark Aronoff, Irit Meir & Carol Padden. 2011b. The gradual emergence of phonological form in a new language. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 29. 503–543. Sandler, Wendy, Mark Aronoff, Carol Padden & Irit Meir. 2014. Language emer- gence Al-Sayyid Bedouin Sign Language. In Jack Sidnell, Paul Kockelman & N. J. Enfield (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of linguistic anthropology, 246–278. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press. Selkirk, Elizabeth. 1984. Phonology and syntax: The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Stokoe, William C. 1960. Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communi- cations systems of the American deaf (Studies in Linguistics: Occasional Papers 8). Buffalo: University of Buffalo. Wilbur, Ronnie B. 2000. Phonological and prosodic layering of nonmanuals in American Sign Language. In Karen Emmorey & Harlan Lane (eds.), The signs of language revisited: An anthology to honor Ursula Bellugi and Ed Klima, 190– 214. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Wray, Alison & George W. Grace. 2007. The consequences of talking to strangers: Evolutionary corollaries of socio-cultural influences on linguistic form. Lingua 117. 543–578.

83