Quick viewing(Text Mode)

Sweet Cherries Following Preharvest Treatment with Hydrogen

Sweet Cherries Following Preharvest Treatment with Hydrogen

HORTSCIENCE 41(3):745–748. 2006. ing, and advancing maturation in sweet (Weis et al., 1998, 1999), as well as effects on Sensory Quality of ‘Bing’ Sweet objective fruit quality (Clayton et al., 2003). However, the postharvest effects of CH2N2 and CaNH4NO3 treatment on the sensory quality of Following Preharvest sweet cherry have not been documented. Dever et al. (1996) cited studies identifying Treatment with Hydrogen Cyanamide, important sensory qualities of sweet cherries and consumer preferences for sweet cherries at retail markets. These sensory qualities were Calcium Ammonium Nitrate, or divided into visual or taste quality parameters with the objective of forming sensory profiles Gibberellic Acid of promising in sweet cherry breeding programs (Cliff et al., 1996; Dever et al., 1996). Murray Clayton and William V. Biasi For ‘Bing’, an established sweet cherry , Department of Plant Sciences, University of , One Shields Avenue, visual quality of non-stored fruit appeared Davis, CA 95616 largely unaffected by preharvest treatments of

CH2N2, CaNH4NO3, and GA3 (Clayton et al., I. Tayfun Agar 2003); however, the sensory taste quality of Department of Horticulture, University of Çukurova, 01330 Adana, Turkey treated fruit may be affected. The objectives of this study were to determine if preharvest Stephen M. Southwick and Elizabeth J. Mitcham1 treatments of CH2N2, CaNH4NO3, and GA3 Department of Plant Sciences, University of California, One Shields Avenue, impacted the postharvest sensory taste quality Davis, CA 95616 of ‘Bing’ sweet cherry fruit and whether any such impact was consistent for fruit of different Additional index words. avium, CH2N2, CaNH4NO3, GA3, firmness, sweetness, levels of maturity. tartness, flavor Abstract. ‘Bing’ sweet cherry ( L.) trees were treated with hydrogen cyana- Materials and Methods mide (CH N ) or calcium ammonium nitrate (CaNH NO ) during dormancy, or gibberellic 2 2 4 3 In 1998, 1999, and 2000, ‘Bing’ sweet acid (GA ) 26 days before harvest during three consecutive years. Fruit were evaluated 3 cherries were obtained from moderately crop- at harvest for sensory taste quality using twenty trained panelists sampling for firmness, ping, mature trees of moderate vigor grown on sweetness, tartness, and cherry flavor. Nondestructive instrumental firmness preceded Maheleb rootstocks in separate commercial destructive sensory firmness on the same untreated and GA -treated cherries in one year 3 orchards each year located on loam-textured when used as a supplementary evaluation. Sensory firmness was consistently higher in soils in the Central Valley of California. Within GA fruit and to a lesser extent in CH N fruit than in CaNH NO and untreated fruit. 3 2 2 4 3 each orchard the experiment was organized as Instrumental firmness of GA fruit did not increase significantly compared with untreated 3 a randomized complete block design comprised fruit yet instrumental firmness of each treatment correlated relatively well with perceived of three blocks and four treatments with one sensory firmness. Sensory sweetness and cherry flavor scored very similarly, yet both tree representing a treatment plot within each attributes simultaneously varied between treatments across the years. Perceived sensory block. Treatments included CH N (Dormex, tartness of treated fruit was variable among years; yet, on average, was rated among 2 2 SKW Trostberg, Trostberg, Germany) at 7.8 treated and untreated fruit as similar. Under the assumption that elevated sensory firm- kg·ha–1 a.i. in 935 L of water, CaNH NO —9% ness, sweetness, and cherry flavor intensity reflects improved sweet cherry quality, GA 4 3 3 Ca and 17% N w/w (CAN17; Unocal Corp., fruit were rated of higher quality than untreated fruit given their increased firmness and El Segundo, Calif.) at 468 L·ha–1 of solution similar or occasionally elevated sweetness and cherry flavor intensity. CH N fruit main- 2 2 in 1400 L of water applied with surfactant tained quality similar to that of untreated fruit, despite often having marginally higher (Optima, Helena Chemical Co., Memphis, firmness, due to similar or reduced ratings for sweetness and cherry flavor intensity. Tenn.) at 37 L·ha–1, GA (ProGibb 4%, Valent Notwithstanding similar firmness between CaNH NO and untreated cherries, sensory 3 4 3 USA Corp., Walnut Creek, Calif.) at 60 g·ha–1 quality of CaNH NO -treated cherries was reduced due to their often-diminished levels 4 3 a.i. in 1870 L of water, and untreated cherries of perceived sweetness and cherry flavor. representing the Control treatment. The CH2N2 Significant quality-related problems in et al., 2003; Facteau, 1982; Facteau et al., and CaNH4NO3 treatments were applied to eastern retail markets have been 1985; Looney and Lidster, 1980; Proebsting the trees during the dormant period in mid- documented in sweet cherry fruit supplied from et al., 1973), and titratable acidity (Proebst- January of all 3 years. GA3 was applied just the western U.S. (Ceponis and Butterfield, ing et al., 1973). There appears to be little before fruit straw color development in May

1981). In combination with increased cherry consensus as to the effects of GA3 on size and of all 3 years. production, additional pressure on traditional soluble solids content. More recently cherry Cherry harvest occurred during the com- markets to absorb the supply of fruit has em- producers have utilized hydrogen cyanamide mercial harvest period for fruit of each treat-

phasized the need for improved fruit quality. (CH2N2) in an attempt to control fruit quality ment, typically when most of the crop was In recent years, producers have increased their and yield problems related to insufficient or at the red and mahogany stages of skin color utilization of compounds capable of regulating marginal chilling during dormancy (Weis et development. This corresponded to 25, 31, and the fruiting and/or growth characteristics of the al., 1998, 1999). CH N decreases the activity 2 2 18 May for CH2N2-treated cherries, 4 and 7 trees or fruit. Many growers apply gibberellic of the enzyme catalase, causing a sequence of June and 21 May for CaNH4NO3-treated cher- acid (GA3) before harvest with the intention reactions eventually coupled to the oxidative ries, 8 June and 8 and 23 May for the control of increasing fruit firmness. Researchers have pentose phosphate pathway and leading to an treatment, and 9 June and 9 and 29 May for documented the effects of GA on cherry increase in the level of reduced nucleotides, 3 GA3-treated fruit for the years 1998, 1999, firmness, size, soluble solids content (Clayton essential for enhanced metabolism and subse- and 2000, respectively. At each harvest, pro- quently budbreak. Researchers have studied portionate samples of cherries were obtained

Received for publication 3 Jan. 2006. Accepted for the effects of CH2N2 and calcium ammonium from the inner and outer canopy of all sides publication 21 Feb. 2006. nitrate (CaNH4NO3) applied in combination of the trees. Defect-free fruit were segregated 1Corresponding author; e-mail ejmitcham@ucdavis. with surfactants or dormant oils, on breaking into four maturity categories based on skin edu. dormancy, coordinating budbreak and flower- color: salmon (1), red (3), mahogany (4), and

HORTSCIENCE VOL. 41(3) JUNE 2006 745

JuneBook 745 4/4/06 11:03:23 AM Table 1. Sensory taste scores of ‘Bing’ cherries between preharvest treatments, years, and skin color cluded that GA3-treated sweet cherries were categories. firmer than untreated cherries (Proebsting et Parameter Firmnessz Sweetnessz Tartnessz Cherry flavorz al., 1973; Facteau and Rowe, 1979; Facteau, Treatmenty 1982; Facteau et al., 1985; Looney and Lidster, x CH2N2 5.7 b 4.1 b 5.1 4.1 ab 1980), our data indicate that such differences CaNH4NO3 5.3 c 4.0 b 5.1 3.9 b are within human sensory perception. Overall, Control 5.3 c 4.3 a 5.0 4.3 a CH2N2-treated cherries were sensed as firmer GA3 6.3 a 4.4 a 4.9 4.3 a than CaNH4NO3 and Control cherries in spite Year of there being no differences between the three 1998 6.0 a 3.8 c 4.8 4.0 b treatments in 1999 (Fig. 1A), a year of relatively 1999 5.0 b 4.0 b 5.1 3.9 b 2000 6.1 a 4.8 a 5.2 4.6 a softer fruit. There were no differences in sen- Color category soryfirmness between CaNH4NO3 and Control Salmon 6.0 a 2.0 d 6.5 a 2.2 d cherries. With respect to maturity, skin color Red 5.4 b 3.2 c 5.5 b 3.3 c has been considered a reliable maturity criterion Mahogany 5.4 b 5.1 b 4.3 c 5.0 b for ‘Bing’ (Clayton et al., 2003; Facteau et al., Dark mahogany 5.8 a 6.4 a 3.8 d 6.2 a 1983; Proebsting and Mills, 1981). However, Source sensory firmness was not consistently related Treatment (T) *** *** NS *to fruit skin color across years (Fig. 2A). Year (Y) *** *** NS *** Immature, salmon-colored cherries were sig- Color category (C) *** *** *** *** T × Y ** *** *** ** nificantly firmer than red, mahogany, and dark T × C NS NS NS NS mahogany cherries in 1998. Surprisingly, dark Y × C *** *** NS *** mahogany-colored cherries were rated firmer T × Y × C NS NS NS NS than red and mahogany colored cherries in 1998 zHigher sensory scores reflect increased sensory firmness, sweetness, tartness, and cherry flavor intensity, and 1999, yet measurements of ‘Bing’ cherries on a 1 to 10 hedonic scale. using a number of instruments have previously y CH2N2 = hydrogen cyanamide, CaNH4NO3 = calcium ammonium nitrate, Control = untreated, GA3 = revealed relatively strong correlations between gibberellic acid. skin color and fruit firmness with lighter col- xMean separation within columns and factors by LSD, P ” 0.05. ored fruit showing higher firmness (Mitcham NS,*,**,*** Nonsignificant or significant at P ” 0.05, 0.01, or 0.001, respectively. et al., 1998). The taste panelists did not detect differences in firmness between the four levels dark mahogany (6), the numbers correspond- ‘less’ at the origin of the horizontal line. of fruit maturity in 2000 (Fig. 2A).

ing to color category chips (Centre Technique Test two. Additional Control and GA3 fruit The supplemental firmness evaluation Interprofessional des Fruits et Legumes, Paris, were harvested on 29 May 2000. Unblemished conducted in 2000 (test 2) confirmed that GA3- France), and held overnight at 0 °C in unsealed ‘Bing’ cherries were sorted into the four treated cherries had higher sensory firmness plastic bags. skin-color categories as previously described than untreated cherries (Table 2). In contrast Each year a sensory panel of 20 persons and stored at 0 °C. The following day, fruit to previous studies (Facteau and Rowe, 1979; (university employees and students) un- were warmed to 20 °C and their firmness Facteau, 1982; Facteau et al., 1985; Looney derwent training to enhance their sensory measured nondestructively by force/deforma- and Lidster, 1980; Proebsting et al., 1973), perceptiveness of cherry fruit, to familiarize tion (g·mm–1) (FirmTech1, BioWorks Incorp., instrumental firmness of the exact same GA3- them with the selected taste quality traits, and Stillwater, Okla.). For each of the treatments, treated cherries was not significantly greater to specify sensory analysis protocols. Basker cherries were ranked by instrumental firmness than untreated cherries for reasons that were (1977) reported that 8 to 20 sensory panelists and divided by quartiles. One cherry within unclear. Correlation coefficients between could be used for a preliminary estimate of each firmness quartile of each treatment was sensory and instrumental firmness were rela- consumer response. Sensory panelists had no randomly assigned to each taste panelist. The tively strongly (P < 0.001) at 0.60 and 0.61 for knowledge of which preharvest treatment they order that fruit of each firmness category and untreated and GA3 cherries, respectively, with were evaluating, nor were they involved in the treatment was presented to the panelists was both correlations exhibiting strong parallelism study beyond the sensory evaluation. also randomized. Fruit were supplied to the (data not shown). These data further indicates Test one. The sensory flavor and texture panelists in individual cups under the illumina- that sensory firmness of GA3-treated fruit was traits evaluated were those also used by Cliff et tive conditions previously described whereby consistently greater than untreated fruit across al. (1996) and Dever et al. (1996), cherry flesh each sample was assessed for firmness based the full range of fruit maturities included in firmness, sweetness, tartness, and cherry flavor. on initial mastication of the fruit. Firmness this study. For each of the treatments, sensory evaluations assessment was recorded on a 10-cm line and On average, cherry sweetness for each were conducted the day after harvest with fruit quantified as previously described. treatment was perceived as relatively low in warmed from 0 to 20 °C. Tasting occurred in Analysis. Results were analyzed by analy- 1998 and 1999 with ratings of 3.8 and 4.0, individual booths illuminated by a dim green sis of variance whereby the main sources of respectively, out of a possible 10 (Table 1). light whereby the fruit were observed through variation for test one were treatment, year, and The weather before cherry harvest was cool- green lenses to obscure skin color (Crisosto fruit skin color category, and for test two was est in 1998 and warmest in 2000, and may 2002). Panelists were presented with three treatment. Panelist performance was not an have influenced seasonal variability in sugar groups (replications or blocks) of four numeri- objective and was not analyzed as a source of content. However, the apparent, extreme lack cally coded cups with each of the four cups variation for either test. Interactions between of sweetness detected in salmon and red col- containing a cherry randomly obtained from treatment, year, and color category for test one ored cherries disproportionately reduced the one of the four skin-color categories within a were analyzed for significance. mean sweetness scores. Cherries treated with block. Each cherry was assessed for sensory CH2N2 and CaNH4NO3 were perceived as being firmness (based on initial fruit mastication), Results and Discussion significantly less sweet than Control and GA3 sweetness, tartness, and cherry flavor, and the cherries. In contrast, the taste panelists did not assessment recorded with a vertical line drawn Fruit firmness, perceived by the taste detect differences in cherry tartness between on a 10-cm horizontal line anchored by the panel during initial mastication of the fruit, treatments combined across years or between

labels “less” and “more” for each trait. Nu- was higher in GA3-treated fruit as compared years themselves (Table 1). However, the in- merical values were assigned to each sensory with CH2N2, CaNH4NO3, and Control fruit teraction of treatment and year was significant assessment by measuring the distance from the (Table 1). While previous studies utilizing for sensory sweetness and tartness and revealed vertical assessment line to the anchor labeled instruments to measure firmness have con- that GA3 and Control fruit were considerably

746 HORTSCIENCE VOL. 41(3) JUNE 2006

JuneBook 746 4/4/06 11:03:31 AM Fig. 1. Sensory firmness (A), sweetness (B), tartness and a broad range in (C), and cherry flavor (D) of ‘Bing’ cherries aver- flavor/texture liking. Furthermore, there have Fig. 2. Sensory firmness (A), sweetness (B), tartness aged across panelists and skin color categories (C), and cherry flavor (D) of ‘Bing’ cherries aver- been conflicting reports as to the effects of GA3 following preharvest treatment with hydrogen on objective measurements of soluble solids aged across panelists and treatments following cyanamide (CH N ), calcium ammonium nitrate 2 2 content and titratable acidity of sweet cherry segregation of fruit into skin color categories of (CaNH4NO3), or gibberellic acid (GA3). Higher salmon, red, mahogany, and dark mahogany fruit. sensory scores reflect increased sensory firmness, (Facteau, 1982; Looney and Lidster, 1980; Pro- ebsting et al., 1973), suggesting the strong influ- Higher sensory scores reflect increased sensory sweetness, tartness, and cherry flavor intensity, firmness, sweetness, tartness, and flavor intensity, ence of factors other than GA interacting with as measured on a 1 to 10 hedonic scale. Mean 3 as measured on a 1 to 10 hedonic scale. Mean separation within quality attributes and years these attributes. Soluble solids content in sour separation within color categories and years by by LSD (P ” 0.05). cherry has also been shown to vary between LSD (P ” 0.05). years (Iezzoni, 1986). There were discrete

sweeter in 2000, but did not differ with CH2N2 differences between the four color categories fruit, with CaNH4NO3 fruit rated as having the and CaNH4NO3 fruit in 1998, while GA3 fruit for sweetness and tartness, as darker cherries lowest flavor when treatments were averaged were sweeter than all other treatments in 1999 of more advanced maturity were sweeter and across years. However, between years there

(Fig. 1B). CaNH4NO3 fruit was the most tart in less tart than lighter colored cherries (Table were few consistent differences between treat- 1999, yet CH2N2 and Control fruit were most 1). This effect was relatively consistent across ments (Fig. 1D). Cliff et al. (1996) also found tart in 1998, and differences between treat- years for sweetness (Fig. 2B) and tartness (Fig. a close association between cherry flavor and ments in 2000 were negligible (Fig. 1C). The 2C), although dark-mahogany fruit were not sensory sweetness in ‘Bing’ and other cherry inconsistency between treatments for sensory as proportionally sweet in 2000 as compared cultivars, while Dever et al. (1996) found a sweetness and tartness across the years is not with 1998 and 1999. good association between cherry flavor and well understood. Cliff et al. (1996) suggested Cherry flavor reflected similar scores to soluble solids content. Surprisingly, both various panelist-related reasons for a wide sensory sweetness when means were calculated studies showed a relatively weak association variation in certain sensory perceived texture/ for treatments, years, or skin color categories between soluble solids content and sensory flavor attributes including a lack of sensitivity (Table 1). As with sensory sweetness, cherry sweetness. Sensory panelists found that pro- to the attributes, inconsistency in preference, gressively darker colored fruit had significantly flavor was rated higher for GA3 and Control

HORTSCIENCE VOL. 41(3) JUNE 2006 747

JuneBook 747 4/4/06 11:03:36 AM Table 2. Sensory and instrumental firmness of GA z- 1996. Development and evaluation of multiple 3 CH2N2 cherries typically maintained or oc- and non-GA3-treated ‘Bing’ cherries. casionally rated lower compared to untreated regression models for prediction of sweet cherry x liking. Food Res. Intl. 28(6):583–589. Instrumental cherries. CaNH4NO3 cherries typically had Sensoryy firmness slightly less or occasionally similar ratings Crisosto, C.H., G.M. Crisosto, and M.A. Ritenour. Parameter firmness (N·mm–1) for sweetness and cherry flavor compared to 2002. Testing the reliability of skin color as an indicator of quality for early season ‘Brooks’ Treatmentx untreated cherries. (Prunus avium L.) cherry. Postharvest Biol. GA z 6.7 aw 1.91 3 The benefits of firmer cherries include Technol. 24(2):147–154. Non-GA 5.3 b 1.85 3 reduced susceptibility to decay organisms and Dever, M.C., R.A. MacDonald, M.A. Cliff, and W.D. Source of variation mechanical injury, enhanced storage potential, Lane. 1996. Sensory evaluation of sweet cherry. Treatment *** NS and increased appeal to consumers (Brown HortScience 31(1):150–153. z GA3 = gibberellic acid. Facteau, T.J. 1982. Levels of pectic substances and y and Bourne, 1988). Sweetness and cherry Higher sensory scores reflect increased sensory flavor intensity are also important attributes calcium in gibberellic acid-treated sweet cherry firmness on a 1 to 10 hedonic scale. fruit. J. Amer. Soc. Hort. Sci. 107(1):148–151. x for overall flavor preference, and increases in FirmTech1, BioWorks Incorp., Stillwater, Okla. Facteau, T.J., N.E. Chestnut, and K.E. Rowe. 1983. w both attributes resulted in increased consumer Mean separation within columns by LSD (P ” Relationship between fruit weight, firmness, and 0.05). preference (Cliff et al., 1996). In comparison leaf/fruit ratio in Lambert and Bing sweet cher- NS,*** Nonsignificant or significant at P ” 0.001, with untreated cherries, GA3-treated ‘Bing’ ries. Can. J. Plant Sci. 63:763–765. respectively. cherries can therefore be considered to have Facteau, T.J., K.E. Rowe, and N.E. Chestnut. 1985. improved sensory taste quality, evident by Firmness of sweet cherry fruit following multiple more cherry flavor than lighter colored fruit; improved firmness whilst, in the least, main- applications of gibberellic acid. J. Amer. Soc. however, the dark mahogany fruit in 2000 taining sweetness, cherry flavor, and tartness. Hort. Sci. 110(6):775–777. were not as proportionately flavorful as they Facteau, T.J. and K.E. Rowe. 1979. Factors associ- CH2N2-treated cherries had somewhat greater were in previous years (Fig. 2D), probably perceivedfirmness, but otherwise were similar ated with surface pitting of sweet cherry. J. Amer. resulting in the interaction of treatment and to untreated fruit in terms of sensory quality. Soc. Hort. Sci. 104(5):706–710. Iezzoni, A.F. 1986. Variance components and sam- year (Table 1). The quality of CaNH NO -treated cherries 4 3 pling procedures for fruit size and quality in sour The treatments appeared to have consis- often suffered compared to untreated cher- cherry. HortScience 21(4):1040–1042. tently impacted sensory firmness more so ries as evident from reduced sweetness and Kappel, F,, B. Fisher-Fleming, and E. Hogue. than sweetness, tartness, or cherry flavor. GA3 cherry flavor intensity in some years. These 1996. Fruit characteristics and sensory at- and CH2N2 fruit were consistently determined findings suggest that benefits may be realized tributes of an ideal sweet cherry. HortScience by panelists to be firmer than untreated and at retail markets supplied with GA -treated 31(3):443–446. 3 Looney, N.E. and P.D. Lidster. 1980. Some growth CaNH4NO3 fruit, although CH2N2 fruit were ‘Bing’ cherries due to increased consumer significantly firmer in only two of the three regulator effects on fruit quality, mesocarp satisfaction. CH2N2 may be useful to cherry years. Meanwhile, mature, dark mahogany producers for manipulating tree dormancy and composition, and susceptibility to postharvest cherries were periodically determined to have surface marking of sweet cherries. J. Amer. Soc. fruit maturation while not impacting negatively Hort. Sci. 105(1):130–134. higher sensory firmness than lighter red or on sensory quality of the fruit. CaNH4NO3 Mitcham, E.J., M. Clayton, and W.V. Biasi. 1998. mahogany cherries, contrasting with reports treatment of ‘Bing’ cherries was found, more Comparison of devices for measuring cherry fruit to the contrary when cherry firmness was often than not, to negatively impact sensory firmness. HortScience 33(4):723–727. measured objectively (Clayton et al., 2003; taste quality. Proebsting, E.L., G.H. Carter, and H.H. Mills. 1973. Mitcham et al., 1998). In spite of numerous Quality improvement in canned ‘Rainier’ cher- ries (P.avium L.) with gibberellic acid. J. Amer. studies reporting improved firmness in GA3- Literature Cited treated cherries, instrumental firmness of GA Soc. Hort. Sci. 98(4):334–336. 3 Basker, D. 1977. The number of assessors required Proebsting, E.L. and H.H. Mills. 1981. Effects of fruit in this study did not increase significantly for taste panels. Chemical Senses Flavor at the 5% probability level despite higher season and crop load on maturity characteris- 2:493–496. tics of ‘Bing’ cherry. J. Amer. Soc. Hort. Sci. sensory firmness for GA fruit. Nonetheless, 3 Brown, S.K. and M.C. Bourne. 1988. Assessment of 106(2):144–146. instrumental and sensory firmness correlated components of fruit firmness in selected sweet Weis, K.G., S.M. Southwick, J.T. Yeager, M.E. reasonably well and with similar strength for cherry genotypes. HortScience 23:902–904. Rupert, and W.W. Coates. 1998. Control of Ceponis, M.J. and J.E. Butterfield. 1981. Cull losses both GA3 and untreated cherries, concurring dormancy and budbreak in sweet cherry (Prunus with Kappel et al. (1996) who reported good in western sweet cherries at retail and consumer avium L.) cv ‘Bing’ with surfactant + calcium correlations between sensory and objective levels in metropolitan New York. HortScience ammonium nitrate and hydrogen cyanamide. firmness of untreated sweet cherries. Despite 16(3):324–326. HortScience 33(3):514. Clayton, M., W.V. Biasi, I.T. Agar, S.M. Southwick, Weis, K.G., S.M. Southwick, J.T. Yeager, M.E. considerable variability between treatments and E.J. Mitcham. 2003. Postharvest quality of for sweetness and cherry flavor across years, Rupert, R.E. Moran, J.A. Grant, and W.W. ‘Bing’ cherries following preharvest treatment Coates. 1999. Overcoming dormancy, advancing GA cherries either maintained or had margin- 3 with hydrogen cyanamide, calcium ammo- budbreak, and advancing fruit maturity in ‘Bing’ ally higher ratings for sweetness and cherry nium nitrate, or gibberellic acid. HortScience sweet cherry (Prunus avium L.): Surfactants/ flavor compared to untreated cherries while 38(3):407–411. dormant oils + calcium ammonium nitrate or Cliff, M.A., M.C. Dever, J.W. Hall, and B. Girard. hydrogen cyanamide. HortScience 34(3):525.

748 HORTSCIENCE VOL. 41(3) JUNE 2006

JuneBook 748 4/4/06 11:03:42 AM