Motivic Similarity and Form in Boulez's Anthèmes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Motivic Similarity and Form in Boulez's Anthèmes Motivic similarity and form in Boulez’s Anthèmes Cecilia Taher Music Theory Area, Department of Music Research Schulich School of Music, McGill University Montreal, Canada June 2016 A thesis submitted to McGill University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. © 2016 Cecilia Taher i To the memory of the musical genius Pierre Boulez (1925-2016), whose music and words inspired the very genesis of this research project. ii iii Abstract This dissertation combines theoretical, analytical, and empirical methods to study aspects of motivic similarity and form in Boulez’s Anthèmes. The concept of similarity is central to the definition of motives and musical form. Nevertheless, little is known about the perception of motivic relationships and form while listening to post-tonal music. The most traditional theoretical methods used for post-tonal analysis (such as pitch-class set theory) have tended to emphasize abstract similarity relations based on local pitch and interval structures, disregarding features other than pitch as well as larger formal levels. However, experimental research has demonstrated that the perception of these pitch relationships in post-tonal music is secondary and extremely difficult. According to the empirical evidence, the kinds of structures that are particularly salient during music listening (including post-tonal music listening) are defined by both local- and global-level relationships involving surface features such as articulation and rhythm. This marks a gap between theoretical and perceptual research agendas. Boulez seemed to be aware of this disconnect, as he explicitly intended to reconcile perception with compositional practice. He described the form of Anthèmes in terms of constantly changing yet clearly recognizable motives, with surface features acting as large-scale formal markers. The two versions of Anthèmes—Anthèmes 1 (1992) for violin and Anthèmes 2 (1997) for violin and electronics—can be understood as two different formal and timbral settings for the same motivic materials. This dissertation aims to begin filling the gap that separates theory and perception in the scholarship of post-tonal music. Chapter 1 introduces the topic of motivic similarity and its relationship to form, reviewing previous literature within the context of Boulez’s ideas about music. Chapter 2 investigates aspects of motivic categorization and similarity in Anthèmes out of the context of the piece. Two theoretical analyses of Anthèmes 1 combined with the results of three perceptual experiments show that (1) motivic materials can be grouped in families with different degrees of internal consistency, and (2) the perception of similarity relationships is highly dependent on the surface features of the materials and the conditions under which those materials are heard. Chapter 3 integrates formal and feature analyses with findings from two perceptual experiments to study the perception of motivic relationships while listening to Anthèmes 1 and Anthèmes 2. I propose that predictability, formal-unit blend (due to a lack of surface contrast), and elapsed musical time (resulting from the development and organization of materials) shift the listeners’ attention to different formal levels. A basic model of form perception is sketched based on these findings. Chapter 4 concludes the dissertation with a discussion about the effects of the musical context on motivic similarity perception, and an overview of the limitations and possibilities for future research on this topic. iv Résumé Cette thèse combine des méthodes théoriques, analytiques et empiriques afin d'étudier les aspects mis en œuvre dans Anthèmes de Boulez concernant la similitude motivique et la forme. Le concept de similitude est central dans la définition des motifs et de la forme musicale. Néanmoins, les connaissances sur la perception des relations motiviques et de la forme pendant l'écoute de la musique post-tonale sont encore très limitées. Les méthodes théoriques les plus traditionnellement employées pour l'analyse post-tonale (telle que la théorie des ensembles de classes de hauteurs) ont eu tendance à mettre l'emphase sur les relations de similitudes abstraites fondées sur les structures locales de hauteurs et d'intervalles, ignorant ainsi tous les paramètres autres que ceux liés à la hauteur et aux niveaux de forme à plus grande échelle. Cependant, la recherche expérimentale a mis en évidence que la perception de ces relations entre hauteurs dans la musique post-tonale était secondaire et extrêmement difficile. Les résultats empiriques révèlent que les types de structures les plus saillantes pendant l'écoute (y compris l'écoute de la musique post-tonale) sont définis à la fois par les relations locales et globales qui impliquent des traits de surface tels que l'articulation et le rythme. Ceci souligne l'existence d'un écart entre les programmes de recherche théorique et perceptif. Boulez semblait être conscient de cette coupure et cherchait en effet explicitement à réconcilier la perception avec sa pratique compositionnelle. Il décrivit la forme d'Anthèmes en termes de motifs changeant continuellement tout en restant clairement reconnaissables, les traits de surface agissant ainsi comme marqueurs de la forme à grande échelle. Les deux versions d'Anthèmes—Anthèmes 1 (1992) pour violon et Anthèmes 2 (1997) pour violon et électronique live—peuvent être interprétées comme deux manifestations de forme et de timbre des mêmes matériaux motiviques. Cette thèse a pour objectif de commencer à combler le vide séparant la théorie et la perception dans l'étude de la musique post-tonale. Le premier chapitre introduit le thème de la similitude motivique et de sa relation avec la forme au regard de la littérature existante et dans le contexte des idées de Boulez sur la musique. Le deuxième chapitre étudie différents aspects concernant la catégorisation et la similitude des motifs en dehors du contexte de l'œuvre. Deux analyses théoriques d'Anthèmes 1, combinées avec les résultats de trois expériences perceptives montrent que (1) les matériaux motiviques peuvent être classés en familles ayant des degrés de cohérence interne différents et (2) la perception des relations de similitude dépend fortement des traits de surface des matériaux ainsi que des conditions dans lesquelles ils sont écoutés. Le troisième chapitre intègre l'analyse de la forme et celle des traits avec les résultats de deux expériences perceptives afin d’étudier la perception des relations motiviques pendant l'écoute d'Anthèmes 1 et 2. Nous proposons que le caractère prévisible, l'intégration des unités de forme (due à un manque de contraste de la surface) et le temps musical écoulé (conséquence du développement et de l'organisation des matériaux) déplace l'attention de l'auditeur sur des niveaux de forme différents. Le quatrième chapitre conclut la thèse avec une discussion concernant les effets du contexte musical sur la perception de la similitude motivique, ainsi qu'avec un aperçu des limites et perspectives de recherches concernant ce sujet. v Acknowledgements This dissertation is only one humble mark of the many gifts I have received as a student at the Schulich School of Music. First and foremost, I thank my supervisors Robert Hasegawa and Stephen McAdams, for their constant dedication and support. Their brilliant work, commitment to excellence, and devotion to education are the main inspiration for this dissertation. Their example, their respect for my ideas, and their confidence in me are as invaluable for the completion of this project as they are for my integral professional development. Prof. Hasegawa has motivated me to rediscover my passion for post-tonal theory and analysis. Prof. McAdams has been my mentor since my arrival to McGill in the fall of 2012, accompanying me in every step and struggle. I admire their professionalism as much as I value the innumerable ways in which they have had an immensely positive impact on my career. Very special thanks to Bennett K. Smith, for programming the experiments and providing not only technical but also spiritual support. His unconditional desire to help was essential for finishing this work promptly. I am grateful to the members of the Music Perception and Cognition Lab, all of whom have contributed to this project in a number of ways, from participating in pilot experiments to suggesting improvements. It has been an honor to be part of a team that operates on the basis of restless mutual cooperation and support. Special thanks to Ira Braus, Song Hui Chon, Meghan Goodchild, Eddy Kazazis, Sven-Amin Lembke, Jason Noble, Joseph Plazak, David Sears, Kit Soden, Kai Siedenburg, Moe Touizrar, and Leigh VanHandel, for their encouragement and many fascinating conversations about music and perception, and to Dominique Beauregard Cazabon, Yinan Cao, Lorraine Chuen, Béatrice Copps, Shrinkhala Dawadi, Chelsea Douglas, Sarah Gates, Pierre Gianferrara, Kiray Jones-Mollerup, Emma Kast, Auroora Kiiski, Rachel Kim, Min-Jee Kim, Ian Lou, Sophia Metcalf, Jocelyn Micallef, Rasmus Nielsen, Kyra Parker, Channey Phung, Henri Rabalais, Jordana Saks, Jake Spitzer, Jack Tokarz, Ayla Tse, Tudor Vrinceanu, Grace Wang, Jamie Webber, Emily Wilson, Chris Wood, and Charlene Zhang, for the many unforgettable moments shared in the lab. I am indebted to Etienne Thoret for proofreading the French version of the abstract. Thanks to Darryl Cameron, for assisting with mechanical problems. I am also grateful for the continuous support of and
Recommended publications
  • Perceptual Interactions of Pitch and Timbre: an Experimental Study on Pitch-Interval Recognition with Analytical Applications
    Perceptual interactions of pitch and timbre: An experimental study on pitch-interval recognition with analytical applications SARAH GATES Music Theory Area Department of Music Research Schulich School of Music McGill University Montréal • Quebec • Canada August 2015 A thesis submitted to McGill University in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the degree of Master of Arts. Copyright © 2015 • Sarah Gates Contents List of Figures v List of Tables vi List of Examples vii Abstract ix Résumé xi Acknowledgements xiii Author Contributions xiv Introduction 1 Pitch, Timbre and their Interaction • Klangfarbenmelodie • Goals of the Current Project 1 Literature Review 7 Pitch-Timbre Interactions • Unanswered Questions • Resulting Goals and Hypotheses • Pitch-Interval Recognition 2 Experimental Investigation 19 2.1 Aims and Hypotheses of Current Experiment 19 2.2 Experiment 1: Timbre Selection on the Basis of Dissimilarity 20 A. Rationale 20 B. Methods 21 Participants • Stimuli • Apparatus • Procedure C. Results 23 2.3 Experiment 2: Interval Identification 26 A. Rationale 26 i B. Method 26 Participants • Stimuli • Apparatus • Procedure • Evaluation of Trials • Speech Errors and Evaluation Method C. Results 37 Accuracy • Response Time D. Discussion 51 2.4 Conclusions and Future Directions 55 3 Theoretical Investigation 58 3.1 Introduction 58 3.2 Auditory Scene Analysis 59 3.3 Carter Duets and Klangfarbenmelodie 62 Esprit Rude/Esprit Doux • Carter and Klangfarbenmelodie: Examples with Timbral Dissimilarity • Conclusions about Carter 3.4 Webern and Klangfarbenmelodie in Quartet op. 22 and Concerto op 24 83 Quartet op. 22 • Klangfarbenmelodie in Webern’s Concerto op. 24, mvt II: Timbre’s effect on Motivic and Formal Boundaries 3.5 Closing Remarks 110 4 Conclusions and Future Directions 112 Appendix 117 A.1,3,5,7,9,11,13 Confusion Matrices for each Timbre Pair A.2,4,6,8,10,12,14 Confusion Matrices by Direction for each Timbre Pair B.1 Response Times for Unisons by Timbre Pair References 122 ii List of Figures Fig.
    [Show full text]
  • Unified Music Theories for General Equal-Temperament Systems
    Unified Music Theories for General Equal-Temperament Systems Brandon Tingyeh Wu Research Assistant, Research Center for Information Technology Innovation, Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan ABSTRACT Why are white and black piano keys in an octave arranged as they are today? This article examines the relations between abstract algebra and key signature, scales, degrees, and keyboard configurations in general equal-temperament systems. Without confining the study to the twelve-tone equal-temperament (12-TET) system, we propose a set of basic axioms based on musical observations. The axioms may lead to scales that are reasonable both mathematically and musically in any equal- temperament system. We reexamine the mathematical understandings and interpretations of ideas in classical music theory, such as the circle of fifths, enharmonic equivalent, degrees such as the dominant and the subdominant, and the leading tone, and endow them with meaning outside of the 12-TET system. In the process of deriving scales, we create various kinds of sequences to describe facts in music theory, and we name these sequences systematically and unambiguously with the aim to facilitate future research. - 1 - 1. INTRODUCTION Keyboard configuration and combinatorics The concept of key signatures is based on keyboard-like instruments, such as the piano. If all twelve keys in an octave were white, accidentals and key signatures would be meaningless. Therefore, the arrangement of black and white keys is of crucial importance, and keyboard configuration directly affects scales, degrees, key signatures, and even music theory. To debate the key configuration of the twelve- tone equal-temperament (12-TET) system is of little value because the piano keyboard arrangement is considered the foundation of almost all classical music theories.
    [Show full text]
  • MTO 19.3: Nobile, Interval Permutations
    Volume 19, Number 3, September 2013 Copyright © 2013 Society for Music Theory Interval Permutations Drew F. Nobile NOTE: The examples for the (text-only) PDF version of this item are available online at: http://www.mtosmt.org/issues/mto.13.19.3/mto.13.19.3.nobile.php KEYWORDS: Interval permutations, intervals, post-tonal analysis, mathematical modeling, set-class, space, Schoenberg, The Book of the Hanging Gardens ABSTRACT: This paper presents a framework for analyzing the interval structure of pitch-class segments (ordered pitch-class sets). An “interval permutation” is a reordering of the intervals that arise between adjacent members of these pitch-class segments. Because pitch-class segments related by interval permutation are not necessarily members of the same set-class, this theory has the capability to demonstrate aurally significant relationships between sets that are not related by transposition or inversion. I begin with a theoretical investigation of interval permutations followed by a discussion of the relationship of interval permutations to traditional pitch-class set theory, specifically focusing on how various set-classes may be related by interval permutation. A final section applies these theories to analyses of several songs from Schoenberg’s op. 15 song cycle The Book of the Hanging Gardens. Received May 2013 [1] Example 1 shows the score to the opening measures of “Sprich nicht immer von dem Laub,” the fourteenth song from Schoenberg’s Book of the Hanging Gardens, op. 15. Upon first listen, one is drawn to the relationship between the piano’s gesture in the first measure and the vocal line in the second.
    [Show full text]
  • Chord Progressions
    Chord Progressions Part I 2 Chord Progressions The Best Free Chord Progression Lessons on the Web "The recipe for music is part melody, lyric, rhythm, and harmony (chord progressions). The term chord progression refers to a succession of tones or chords played in a particular order for a specified duration that harmonizes with the melody. Except for styles such as rap and free jazz, chord progressions are an essential building block of contemporary western music establishing the basic framework of a song. If you take a look at a large number of popular songs, you will find that certain combinations of chords are used repeatedly because the individual chords just simply sound good together. I call these popular chord progressions the money chords. These money chord patterns vary in length from one- or two-chord progressions to sequences lasting for the whole song such as the twelve-bar blues and thirty-two bar rhythm changes." (Excerpt from Chord Progressions For Songwriters © 2003 by Richard J. Scott) Every guitarist should have a working knowledge of how these chord progressions are created and used in popular music. Click below for the best in free chord progressions lessons available on the web. Ascending Augmented (I-I+-I6-I7) - - 4 Ascending Bass Lines - - 5 Basic Progressions (I-IV) - - 10 Basie Blues Changes - - 8 Blues Progressions (I-IV-I-V-I) - - 15 Blues With A Bridge - - 36 Bridge Progressions - - 37 Cadences - - 50 Canons - - 44 Circle Progressions -- 53 Classic Rock Progressions (I-bVII-IV) -- 74 Coltrane Changes -- 67 Combination
    [Show full text]
  • Surviving Set Theory: a Pedagogical Game and Cooperative Learning Approach to Undergraduate Post-Tonal Music Theory
    Surviving Set Theory: A Pedagogical Game and Cooperative Learning Approach to Undergraduate Post-Tonal Music Theory DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Angela N. Ripley, M.M. Graduate Program in Music The Ohio State University 2015 Dissertation Committee: David Clampitt, Advisor Anna Gawboy Johanna Devaney Copyright by Angela N. Ripley 2015 Abstract Undergraduate music students often experience a high learning curve when they first encounter pitch-class set theory, an analytical system very different from those they have studied previously. Students sometimes find the abstractions of integer notation and the mathematical orientation of set theory foreign or even frightening (Kleppinger 2010), and the dissonance of the atonal repertoire studied often engenders their resistance (Root 2010). Pedagogical games can help mitigate student resistance and trepidation. Table games like Bingo (Gillespie 2000) and Poker (Gingerich 1991) have been adapted to suit college-level classes in music theory. Familiar television shows provide another source of pedagogical games; for example, Berry (2008; 2015) adapts the show Survivor to frame a unit on theory fundamentals. However, none of these pedagogical games engage pitch- class set theory during a multi-week unit of study. In my dissertation, I adapt the show Survivor to frame a four-week unit on pitch- class set theory (introducing topics ranging from pitch-class sets to twelve-tone rows) during a sophomore-level theory course. As on the show, students of different achievement levels work together in small groups, or “tribes,” to complete worksheets called “challenges”; however, in an important modification to the structure of the show, no students are voted out of their tribes.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture Notes
    Notes on Chromatic Harmony © 2010 John Paul Ito Chromatic Modulation Enharmonic modulation Pivot-chord modulation in which the pivot chord is spelled differently in the two keys. Ger +6 (enharmonically equivalent to a dominant seventh) and diminished sevenths are the most popular options. See A/S 534-536 Common-tone modulation Usually a type of phrase modulation. The two keys are distantly related, but the tonic triads share a common tone. (E.g. direct modulation from C major to A-flat major.) In some cases, the common tone may be spelled using an enharmonic equivalent. See A/S 596-599. Common-tone modulations often involve… Chromatic mediant relationships Chromatic mediants were extremely popular with nineteenth-century composers, being used with increasing frequency and freedom as the century progressed. A chromatic mediant relationship is one in which the triadic roots (or tonal centers) are a third apart, and in which at least one of the potential common tones between the triads (or tonic triads of the keys) is not a common tone because of chromatic alterations. In the example above of C major going to A-flat major, the diatonic third relationship with C major would be A minor, which has two common tones (C and E) with C major. Because of the substitution of A-flat major (bVI relative to C) for A minor, there is only one common tone, C, as the C-major triad includes E and the A-flat-major triad contains E-flat. The contrast is even more jarring if A-flat minor is substituted; now there are no common tones.
    [Show full text]
  • When the Leading Tone Doesn't Lead: Musical Qualia in Context
    When the Leading Tone Doesn't Lead: Musical Qualia in Context Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Claire Arthur, B.Mus., M.A. Graduate Program in Music The Ohio State University 2016 Dissertation Committee: David Huron, Advisor David Clampitt Anna Gawboy c Copyright by Claire Arthur 2016 Abstract An empirical investigation is made of musical qualia in context. Specifically, scale-degree qualia are evaluated in relation to a local harmonic context, and rhythm qualia are evaluated in relation to a metrical context. After reviewing some of the philosophical background on qualia, and briefly reviewing some theories of musical qualia, three studies are presented. The first builds on Huron's (2006) theory of statistical or implicit learning and melodic probability as significant contributors to musical qualia. Prior statistical models of melodic expectation have focused on the distribution of pitches in melodies, or on their first-order likelihoods as predictors of melodic continuation. Since most Western music is non-monophonic, this first study investigates whether melodic probabilities are altered when the underlying harmonic accompaniment is taken into consideration. This project was carried out by building and analyzing a corpus of classical music containing harmonic analyses. Analysis of the data found that harmony was a significant predictor of scale-degree continuation. In addition, two experiments were carried out to test the perceptual effects of context on musical qualia. In the first experiment participants rated the perceived qualia of individual scale-degrees following various common four-chord progressions that each ended with a different harmony.
    [Show full text]
  • Chromatic Sequences
    CHAPTER 30 Melodic and Harmonic Symmetry Combine: Chromatic Sequences Distinctions Between Diatonic and Chromatic Sequences Sequences are paradoxical musical processes. At the surface level they provide rapid harmonic rhythm, yet at a deeper level they function to suspend tonal motion and prolong an underlying harmony. Tonal sequences move up and down the diatonic scale using scale degrees as stepping-stones. In this chapter, we will explore the consequences of transferring the sequential motions you learned in Chapter 17 from the asymmetry of the diatonic scale to the symmetrical tonal patterns of the nineteenth century. You will likely notice many similarities of behavior between these new chromatic sequences and the old diatonic ones, but there are differences as well. For instance, the stepping-stones for chromatic sequences are no longer the major and minor scales. Furthermore, the chord qualities of each individual harmony inside a chromatic sequence tend to exhibit more homogeneity. Whereas in the past you may have expected major, minor, and diminished chords to alternate inside a diatonic sequence, in the chromatic realm it is not uncommon for all the chords in a sequence to manifest the same quality. EXAMPLE 30.1 Comparison of Diatonic and Chromatic Forms of the D3 ("Pachelbel") Sequence A. 624 CHAPTER 30 MELODIC AND HARMONIC SYMMETRY COMBINE 625 B. Consider Example 30.1A, which contains the D3 ( -4/ +2)-or "descending 5-6"-sequence. The sequence is strongly goal directed (progressing to ii) and diatonic (its harmonies are diatonic to G major). Chord qualities and distances are not consistent, since they conform to the asymmetry of G major.
    [Show full text]
  • Generalized Tonnetze and Zeitnetz, and the Topology of Music Concepts
    June 25, 2019 Journal of Mathematics and Music tonnetzTopologyRev Submitted exclusively to the Journal of Mathematics and Music Last compiled on June 25, 2019 Generalized Tonnetze and Zeitnetz, and the Topology of Music Concepts Jason Yust∗ School of Music, Boston University () The music-theoretic idea of a Tonnetz can be generalized at different levels: as a network of chords relating by maximal intersection, a simplicial complex in which vertices represent notes and simplices represent chords, and as a triangulation of a manifold or other geomet- rical space. The geometrical construct is of particular interest, in that allows us to represent inherently topological aspects to important musical concepts. Two kinds of music-theoretical geometry have been proposed that can house Tonnetze: geometrical duals of voice-leading spaces, and Fourier phase spaces. Fourier phase spaces are particularly appropriate for Ton- netze in that their objects are pitch-class distributions (real-valued weightings of the twelve pitch classes) and proximity in these space relates to shared pitch-class content. They admit of a particularly general method of constructing a geometrical Tonnetz that allows for interval and chord duplications in a toroidal geometry. The present article examines how these du- plications can relate to important musical concepts such as key or pitch-height, and details a method of removing such redundancies and the resulting changes to the homology the space. The method also transfers to the rhythmic domain, defining Zeitnetze for cyclic rhythms. A number of possible Tonnetze are illustrated: on triads, seventh chords, ninth-chords, scalar tetrachords, scales, etc., as well as Zeitnetze on a common types of cyclic rhythms or time- lines.
    [Show full text]
  • Middleground Structure in the Cadenza to Boulez's Éclat
    Middleground Structure in the Cadenza to Boulez’s Éclat C. Catherine Losada NOTE: The examples for the (text-only) PDF version of this item are available online at: hp://www.mtosmt.org/issues/mto.19.25.1/mto.19.25.1.losada.php KEYWORDS: Boulez, Éclat, multiplication, middleground, transformational theory ABSTRACT: Through a transformational analysis of Boulez’s Éclat, this article extends previous understanding of Boulez’s compositional techniques by addressing issues of middleground structure and perception. Presenting a new perspective on this pivotal work, it also sheds light on the development of Boulez’s compositional style. Volume 25, Number 1, May 2019 Copyright © 2019 Society for Music Theory [1] Incorporating both elements of performer’s choice (mainly for the conductor) and an approach to temporality that subverts traditional notions of continuity by invoking the importance of the “moment,”(1) Boulez’s Éclat (1965) is a landmark work from the mid-1960s. It stems from a pivotal period within Boulez’s compositional career, following a time of intense application of novel serial techniques in works that cemented his reputation as a major figure of European modernism (such as Pli selon pli (1957–1962) and the Troisième Sonate (1955–63),(2) but preceding the marked simplification of the musical language that followed Rituel (1974).(3) Piencikowski (1993) has discussed the reliance of much of the central section of Éclat on material from the first version of Don (1960) which in turn is derived from the flute piece Strophes (1957) and from Orestie (1955), and has also thoroughly explained the pitch content of that central section.(4) The material from the framing cadenzas of Éclat, however, has received lile scholarly aention in published form, although Olivier Meston (2001) provides a description of abstract serial processes that could explain the pitch content.(5) One of Meston’s main claims is that the serial processes he presents are not perceptible (10, 16).
    [Show full text]
  • Harmonic Vocabulary in the Music of John Adams: a Hierarchical Approach Author(S): Timothy A
    Yale University Department of Music Harmonic Vocabulary in the Music of John Adams: A Hierarchical Approach Author(s): Timothy A. Johnson Source: Journal of Music Theory, Vol. 37, No. 1 (Spring, 1993), pp. 117-156 Published by: Duke University Press on behalf of the Yale University Department of Music Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/843946 Accessed: 06-07-2017 19:50 UTC JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://about.jstor.org/terms Yale University Department of Music, Duke University Press are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Music Theory This content downloaded from 198.199.32.254 on Thu, 06 Jul 2017 19:50:30 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms HARMONIC VOCABULARY IN THE MUSIC OF JOHN ADAMS: A HIERARCHICAL APPROACH Timothy A. Johnson Overview Following the minimalist tradition, much of John Adams's' music consists of long passages employing a single set of pitch classes (pcs) usually encompassed by one diatonic set.2 In many of these passages the pcs form a single diatonic triad or seventh chord with no additional pcs. In other passages textural and registral formations imply a single triad or seventh chord, but additional pcs obscure this chord to some degree.
    [Show full text]
  • Transfer Theory Placement Exam Guide (Pdf)
    2016-17 GRADUATE/ transfer THEORY PLACEMENT EXAM guide! Texas woman’s university ! ! 1 2016-17 GRADUATE/transferTHEORY PLACEMENTEXAMguide This! guide is meant to help graduate and transfer students prepare for the Graduate/ Transfer Theory Placement Exam. This evaluation is meant to ensure that students have competence in basic tonal harmony. There are two parts to the exam: written and aural. Part One: Written Part Two: Aural ‣ Four voice part-writing to a ‣ Melodic dictation of a given figured bass diatonic melody ‣ Harmonic analysis using ‣ Harmonic Dictation of a Roman numerals diatonic progression, ‣ Transpose a notated notating the soprano, bass, passage to a new key and Roman numerals ‣ Harmonization of a simple ‣ Sightsinging of a melody diatonic melody that contains some functional chromaticism ! Students must achieve a 75% on both the aural and written components of the exam. If a passing score is not received on one or both sections of the exam, the student may be !required to take remedial coursework. Recommended review materials include most of the commonly used undergraduate music theory texts such as: Tonal Harmony by Koska, Payne, and Almén, The Musician’s Guide to Theory and Analysis by Clendinning and Marvin, and Harmony in Context by Francoli. The exam is given prior to the beginning of both the Fall and Spring Semesters. Please check the TWU MUSIc website (www.twu.edu/music) ! for the exact date and time. ! For further information, contact: Dr. Paul Thomas Assistant Professor of Music Theory and Composition [email protected] 2 2016-17 ! ! ! ! table of Contents ! ! ! ! ! 04 Part-Writing ! ! ! ! ! 08 melody harmonization ! ! ! ! ! 13 transposition ! ! ! ! ! 17 Analysis ! ! ! ! ! 21 melodic dictation ! ! ! ! ! harmonic dictation ! 24 ! ! ! ! Sightsinging examples ! 28 ! ! ! 31 terms ! ! ! ! ! 32 online resources ! 3 PART-Writing Part-writing !Realize the following figured bass in four voices.
    [Show full text]