Interdisciplinary Research on Taxonomy and Nomenclature For

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Interdisciplinary Research on Taxonomy and Nomenclature For Article Progress in Physical Geography 1–21 ª The Author(s) 2015 Names matter: Interdisciplinary Reprints and permission: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav research on taxonomy DOI: 10.1177/0309133315589706 and nomenclature for ecosystem ppg.sagepub.com management Elizabeth S. Barron University of Wisconsin Oshkosh, USA Chris Sthultz Harvard University, USA Dale Hurley University of Wisconsin Oshkosh, USA Anne Pringle Harvard University, USA Abstract Local ecological knowledge (LEK) is increasingly used to provide insights into ecosystem dynamics and to promote stakeholder inclusion. However, research on how to incorporate LEK into ecosystem management rarely discusses taxonomy and nomenclature despite the fact that processes of naming are deeply implicated in what types of knowledge are validated and used. Too often, local names are vetted against and then subsumed under ‘true’ scientific names, producing an oversimplified understanding of local names and perpetuating stereotypes about communities that use them. Ongoing revisions in mycological taxonomy and widespread interest in wild edible fungi make mushrooms an excellent case study for addressing nomen- clature as an important part of multi-stakeholder research. We use morel mushrooms collected from the Mid-Atlantic United States to demonstrate a methodological approach to nomenclature – performative method – that focuses both on maintaining culturally meaningful aspects of local names and on recognizing culture and meaning behind scientific names. While recognizing the utility of the Linnaean nomenclatural system, we argue that acknowledging the contextual meanings of names avoids the unequal power relations inherent in integrating local knowledge into scientific discourse, and instead reframes knowledge production around shared interests in environmental questions and challenges. Keywords Critical physical geography, situated knowledges, local ecological knowledge, fungi, performative method I Introduction Corresponding author: Demonstrating the existence of traditional and Elizabeth S. Barron, University of Wisconsin Oshkosh, 800 local ecological knowledge has been a signifi- Algoma Blvd, Oshkosh, WI 54901, USA. cant tool for indigenous and community rights Email: [email protected] Downloaded from ppg.sagepub.com by guest on July 10, 2015 2 Progress in Physical Geography movements to enable greater participation in names are encountered as fixed and final. Sci- ecosystem management, and in many ways this entific names are considered more ‘‘correct’’ tool has worked. Local ecological knowledge or ‘‘true,’’ and thus more powerful and rele- (LEK) is increasingly discussed by scientists vant for use in environmental management. and used by policy makers to provide insights By extension, scientists’ input is considered into ecosystem dynamics and to promote stake- ultimately more valuable for ecosystem man- holder inclusion (Berkes et al., 2003; Gadgil agement decision-making than that of local et al., 2003; Miller et al., 2008; Raymond stakeholder communities, thus extending this et al., 2010; Rist and Dahdouh-Guebas, 2006; power imbalance to concerns of social equity Salick et al., 2002). In the majority of research and political economy. on LEK and ecosystem management, however, By giving attention to power relations, our taxonomy and nomenclature are rarely dis- interdisciplinary project on taxonomy and cussed. Too often, local names are vetted nomenclature is intended to highlight ethical against and then subsumed under ‘‘true’’ scien- concerns about power imbalances among stake- tific names, or sidelined in discussions focused holders by focusing on the production of local on relating knowledge to power. Many natural and scientific names, rather than the names and social scientists perceive scientific and themselves. Our case study examines local and local names as a necessary bottleneck on the scientific naming processes of morel mush- way to answering more interesting questions rooms found in the Mid-Atlantic region of the about ecology (Wolfe et al., 2012b), evolution- United States. Morels make a particularly inter- ary biogeography (Wolfe et al., 2012a), indi- esting case study because they are prized wild genous politics (Agrawal, 2002), or critical edibles among lay experts and their scientific analyses of community engagement (Brosius taxonomy and nomenclature, like those of many and Russell, 2003). While valuable in their own genera in mycology, has been destabilized in the rights, this ‘‘bottleneck mentality’’ on nomen- past few decades. Examining how nomencla- clature distracts from the ethical dimensions of tural data are produced, and under what condi- the naming process and the power dynamics tions they are used and change, shifts the focus inherent therein, perpetuating stereotypes about away from hierarchical comparison and knowl- use, meaning, and value of both scientific and edge integration, which has proven to be local names, and lay and scientific experts. ineffective in enabling real social change in Nomenclature includes metrics, mechanics, environmental governance and conservation and techniques that laypeople use in their every- frameworks (Brook and McLachlan, 2008; day lives (Bowker and Star, 1999) as well as Lave et al., 2014). those that scientists use as part of their work. We consider this work within the scope of Considering nomenclature is an important part the emerging field of critical physical geogra- of an integrated, multi-stakeholder approach phy (CPG) (Lave, 2012), where analyses of to ecosystem research because processes of biophysical systems are related to and informed naming are deeply implicated in what types of by accompanying analyses of cultural and insti- knowledge are validated and used (Bowker, tutional systems (Tadaki et al., 2012). Neither 2000). We argue there is a continuing need for scientific nor social processes are privileged in in-depth research on taxonomy and classifica- our analysis. Rather, in accordance with the tion in both the natural and social sciences, and CPG framework, we ‘‘produce critical biophysi- that these lines of research benefit from being cal and social explanations while also reflecting conducted together. This need stems from the on the conditions under which those explana- power imbalance present when local and scientific tions [were] produced’’ (Lave et al., 2014: 4). Downloaded from ppg.sagepub.com by guest on July 10, 2015 Barron et al. 3 In developing a CPG approach to taxonomy Performance, in short, seems to offer intriguing possi- and nomenclature, we consider claims from bilities for thinking about the constructedness of iden- bioscience and local knowledge to be equally tity, subjectivity, and agency. Our assumption [is] relevant to ecosystem management, and attend that a notion of performance is indeed crucial for criti- cal human geography concerned to understand the con- to the social power relations between them. struction of ...social power relations, and the way We emphasize ethical forms of producing space might articulate all of these. ...We agree that knowledge, rather than the development of both performance and performativity are important knowledge integration protocols. Focusing con- conceptual tools for a critical geography concerned to currently on environmental impacts, social denaturalize taken-for-granted social practices (Greg- equity, and political economic concerns gener- son and Rose, 2000: 434). ates an alternative ethical framework for future ecosystem management. With our focus on When taken together, these two concepts pro- ecology and critical nature discourses, our work vide a conceptual structure consistent with the extends the scope of CPG from the geomorpho- aims and goals of our project: to better under- logic to the biotic, and into dialogue with bios- stand the power relations inherent in the names cientific research. of organisms by seeing those names come into The paper proceeds in four parts. We begin being in specific places, at specific times, and section II with a review of a subset of the through shared experiences. We call our approach empirical literature on LEK in resource man- performative method: a form of self-ethnographic agement, primarily from the social-ecological data collection, analysis, and learning that pro- systems literature focusing on LEK integration. duces alternative ways of knowing nature. Our Our specific interests in ecosystem management aim with the concept of performative method and social equity are addressed more directly is to bring the methodological details of inter- there, although we also find insight in the ethno- disciplinary collaboration to the foreground, botanical and ethnoecological literature often with special attention to the social actions and associated with folk and biosystematics (Berlin, identities of local experts, scientific experts, 1992; Berlin et al., 1973; Rist and Dahdouh- and ourselves as an interdisciplinary research Guebas, 2006). We also include a review of for- team (Ellis and Waterton, 2005). The concep- mal classification in botany and mycology as a tual framework of situated knowledges and point of reference for those unfamiliar with performativity, in addition to our use of the these processes. social-ecological systems literature, further In section II we further include a discussion differentiates this project from classical ethno- of the concepts of ‘‘situated knowledges’’ and botanical work. ‘‘performativity,’’ which form the theoretical In section III, we briefly
Recommended publications
  • Individualism, Structuralism, and Climate Change
    1 2 3 Individualism, Structuralism, and Climate Change 4 5 Michael Brownstein 6 Alex Madva 7 Daniel Kelly 8 9 10 Abstract 11 12 Scholars and activists working on climate change often distinguish between “individual” and 13 “structural” approaches to decarbonization. The former concern behaviors and consumption 14 choices individual citizens can make to reduce their “personal carbon footprint” (e.g., eating less 15 meat). The latter concern institutions that shape collective action, focusing instead on state and 16 national laws, industrial policies, and international treaties. While the distinction between 17 individualism and structuralism—the latter of which we take to include “institutional”, “systemic”, 18 and “collectivist” approaches—is intuitive and ubiquitous, the two approaches are often portrayed 19 as oppositional, as if one or the other is the superior route to decarbonization. We argue instead for 20 a more symbiotic conception of structural and individual reform. 21 22 23 1. Introduction 24 Scholars and activists working on climate change often distinguish between “individual” and 25 “structural” approaches to decarbonization. The former concern behaviors and consumption 26 choices individual citizens can make to reduce their “personal carbon footprint” (e.g., eating less 27 meat). The latter concern institutions that shape collective action, focusing instead on state and 28 national laws, industrial policies, and international treaties. While the distinction between 29 individualism and structuralism—the latter of which we take to include “institutional”, “systemic”, 30 and “collectivist” approaches—is intuitive and ubiquitous, the two approaches are often portrayed 31 as oppositional, as if one or the other is the superior route to decarbonization.
    [Show full text]
  • Limits on Bilingualism Revisited: Stress 'Deafness' in Simultaneous French
    Cognition 114 (2010) 266–275 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT Limits on bilingualism revisited: Stress ‘deafness’ in simultaneous French–Spanish bilinguals Emmanuel Dupoux a,*, Sharon Peperkamp a,b, Núria Sebastián-Gallés c a Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, Ecole des Hautes Etudes en Sciences Sociales, Département d’Etudes Cognitives, Ecole Normale Supérieure, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, 29 Rue d’Ulm, 75005 Paris, France b Département des Sciences du Langage, Université de Paris VIII, Saint-Denis, France c Brain and Cognition Unit, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain article info abstract Article history: We probed simultaneous French–Spanish bilinguals for the perception of Spanish lexical Received 29 September 2008 stress using three tasks, two short-term memory encoding tasks and a speeded lexical deci- Revised 30 September 2009 sion. In all three tasks, the performance of the group of simultaneous bilinguals was inter- Accepted 1 October 2009 mediate between that of native speakers of Spanish on the one hand and French late learners of Spanish on the other hand. Using a composite stress ‘deafness’ index measure computed over the results of the three tasks, we found that the performance of the simul- Keywords: taneous bilinguals is best fitted by a bimodal distribution that corresponds to a mixture of Speech perception the performance distributions of the two control groups. Correlation analyses showed that Simultaneous bilingualism Phonology the variables explaining language dominance are linked to early language exposure. These Stress findings are discussed in light of theories of language processing in bilinguals. Ó 2009 Elsevier B.V.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Development Language Development
    Language Development rom their very first cries, human beings communicate with the world around them. Infants communicate through sounds (crying and cooing) and through body lan- guage (pointing and other gestures). However, sometime between 8 and 18 months Fof age, a major developmental milestone occurs when infants begin to use words to speak. Words are symbolic representations; that is, when a child says “table,” we understand that the word represents the object. Language can be defined as a system of symbols that is used to communicate. Although language is used to communicate with others, we may also talk to ourselves and use words in our thinking. The words we use can influence the way we think about and understand our experiences. After defining some basic aspects of language that we use throughout the chapter, we describe some of the theories that are used to explain the amazing process by which we Language9 A system of understand and produce language. We then look at the brain’s role in processing and pro- symbols that is used to ducing language. After a description of the stages of language development—from a baby’s communicate with others or first cries through the slang used by teenagers—we look at the topic of bilingualism. We in our thinking. examine how learning to speak more than one language affects a child’s language develop- ment and how our educational system is trying to accommodate the increasing number of bilingual children in the classroom. Finally, we end the chapter with information about disorders that can interfere with children’s language development.
    [Show full text]
  • Constructing Kallipolis: the Political Argument of Plato's Socratic Dialogues
    Constructing Kallipolis: The Political Argument of Plato's Socratic Dialogues The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Muller, Joe Pahl Williams. 2016. Constructing Kallipolis: The Political Argument of Plato's Socratic Dialogues. Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Graduate School of Arts & Sciences. Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:33493293 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Other Posted Material, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#LAA Constructing Kallipolis: The Political Argument of Plato’s Socratic Dialogues A dissertation presented by Joe Pahl Williams Muller to The Department of Government in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the subject of Political Science Harvard University Cambridge, Massachusetts April 2016 © 2016 Joe Pahl Williams Muller All rights reserved. Dissertation Advisors: Joe Pahl Williams Muller Professor Eric Nelson Professor Rusty Jones Constructing Kallipolis: The Political Argument of Plato’s Socratic Dialogues Abstract This dissertation examines the political argument of Plato’s Socratic dialogues. Common interpretations of these texts suggest, variously: (1) that Socrates does not offer much in the way of a political theory; (2) that Socrates does reflect on politics but ultimately rejects political in- stitutions as irrelevant to his ethical concerns; (3) that Socrates arrives at a political theory that either accepts or even celebrates free and demo- cratic political arrangements.
    [Show full text]
  • The Influence of Initial Exposure on Lexical
    Journal of Memory and Language Journal of Memory and Language 52 (2005) 240–255 www.elsevier.com/locate/jml The influence of initial exposure on lexical representation: Comparing early and simultaneous bilinguals Nu´ria Sebastia´n-Galle´s*, Sagrario Echeverrı´a, Laura Bosch Cognitive Neuroscience Group, Parc Cientı´fic UB-Hospital Sant Joan de De´u, Edifici Docent, c/ Sta. Rosa, 39-57 4a planta, 08950 Esplugues, Barcelona, Spain Received 8 March 2004; revision received 3 November 2004 Abstract The representation of L2 words and non-words was analysed in a series of three experiments. Catalan-Spanish bilinguals, differing in terms of their L1 and the age of exposure to their L2 (since birth—simultaneous bilinguals— or starting in early childhood—early sequential bilinguals), were asked to perform a lexical decision task on Catalan words and non-words. The non-words were based on real words, but with one vowel changed: critically, this vowel change could involve a Catalan contrast that Spanish natives find difficult to perceive. The results confirmed previous data indicating that in spite of early, intensive exposure, Spanish-Catalan bilinguals fail to perceive certain Catalan contrasts, and that this failure has consequences at the lexical level. Further, the results from simultaneous bilinguals show: (a) that even in the case of bilinguals who are exposed to both languages from birth, a dominant language prevails; and (b) that simultaneous bilinguals do not attain the same level of proficiency as early bilinguals in their first language. Ó 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Speech perception; Bilingualism; Spanish; Catalan; Vowel perception; Early exposure; Sensitive period; Lexicon How early must an individual learn a second lan- longer assume that there is a more or less sudden end guage to attain native performance? Popular wisdom to an individualÕs ability to master a second language, has it that puberty is the upper limit for ‘‘perfect’’ sec- but hold that the loss of the acquisition capacity is pro- ond language acquisition.
    [Show full text]
  • History of Structuralism. Vol. 2
    DJFHKJSD History of Structuralism Volume 2 This page intentionally left blank History of Structuralism Volume 2: The Sign Sets, 1967-Present Francois Dosse Translated by Deborah Glassman University of Minnesota Press Minneapolis London The University of Minnesota Press gratefully acknowledges financial assistance provided by the French Ministry of Culture for the translation of this book. Copyright 1997 by the Regents of the University of Minnesota Originally published as Histoire du structuralisme, 11. Le chant du cygne, de 1967 anos jour«; Copyright Editions La Decouverte, Paris, 1992. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior written permission of the publisher. Published by the University of Minnesota Press III Third Avenue South, Suite 290, Minneapolis, MN 554°1-2520 Printed in the United States of America on acid-free paper http://www.upress.umn.edu First paperback edition, 1998 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Dosse, Francois, 1950- [Histoire du structuralisme. English] History of structuralism I Francois Dosse ; translated by Deborah Glassman. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. Contents: v. 1. The rising sign, 1945-1966-v. 2. The sign sets, 1967-present. ISBN 0-8166-2239-6 (v. I: he: alk. paper}.-ISBN 0-8166-2241-8 (v. I: pbk. : alk. paper}.-ISBN 0-8166-2370-8 (v. 2: hc: alk. paper}.-ISBN 0-8166-2371-6 (v. 2: pbk. : alk. paper}.-ISBN 0-8166-2240-X (set: hc: alk. paper}.-ISBN 0-8166-2254-X (set: pbk.
    [Show full text]
  • Boas, Hunt, and the Ethnographic Silencing of First Nations Women
    University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Department of Anthropology Papers Department of Anthropology 2014 My Sisters Will Not Speak: Boas, Hunt, and the Ethnographic Silencing of First Nations Women Margaret Bruchac University of Pennsylvania, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/anthro_papers Part of the Archaeological Anthropology Commons, Indigenous Studies Commons, and the Other Languages, Societies, and Cultures Commons Recommended Citation Bruchac, M. (2014). My Sisters Will Not Speak: Boas, Hunt, and the Ethnographic Silencing of First Nations Women. Curator: The Museum Journal, 57 (2), 153-171. https://doi.org/10.1111/cura.12058 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/anthro_papers/170 For more information, please contact [email protected]. My Sisters Will Not Speak: Boas, Hunt, and the Ethnographic Silencing of First Nations Women Abstract First Nations women were instrumental to the collection of Northwest Coast Indigenous culture, yet their voices are nearly invisible in the published record. The contributions of George Hunt, the Tlingit/British culture broker who collaborated with anthropologist Franz Boas, overshadow the intellectual influence of his mother, Anislaga Mary Ebbetts, his sisters, and particularly his Kwakwaka'wakw wives, Lucy Homikanis and Tsukwani Francine. In his correspondence with Boas, Hunt admitted his dependence upon high-status Indigenous women, and he gave his female relatives visual prominence in film, photographs, and staged performances, but their voices are largely absent from anthropological texts. Hunt faced many unexpected challenges (disease, death, arrest, financial hardship, and the suspicions of his neighbors), yet he consistently placed Boas' demands, perspectives, and editorial choices foremost.
    [Show full text]
  • Speech Perception and Production by Sequential Bilingual Children: a Longitudinal Study of Voice Onset Time Acquisition
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by UCL Discovery Child Development, September/October 2014, Volume 85, Number 5, Pages 1965–1980 Speech Perception and Production by Sequential Bilingual Children: A Longitudinal Study of Voice Onset Time Acquisition Kathleen M. McCarthy, Merle Mahon, Stuart Rosen, and Bronwen G. Evans University College London The majority of bilingual speech research has focused on simultaneous bilinguals. Yet, in immigrant communi- ties, children are often initially exposed to their family language (L1), before becoming gradually immersed in the host country’s language (L2). This is typically referred to as sequential bilingualism. Using a longitudinal design, this study explored the perception and production of the English voicing contrast in 55 children (40 Sylheti-Eng- lish sequential bilinguals and 15 English monolinguals). Children were tested twice: when they were in nursery (52-month-olds) and 1 year later. Sequential bilinguals’ perception and production of English plosives were initially driven by their experience with their L1, but after starting school, changed to match that of their monolingual peers. To date the majority of speech development research Acquiring the Speech Sounds of Two Languages: has focused on monolingual infants. However, with Simultaneous and Sequential Bilinguals the increase in mobility and, in turn, immigration, It is well established that monolingual infants growing up bilingual is becoming the norm become attuned to the speech sounds of their (National Association for Language Development in ambient language within the 1st year of life (Kuhl, the Curriculum, 2013; Pupil Level Annual School 2004; Werker & Tees, 1984) and that these phonemic Census, 2013).
    [Show full text]
  • The Missing Link in Language Development of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children: Pragmatic Language Development
    The Missing Link in Language Development of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children: Pragmatic Language Development Dianne Goberis, M.A.,1 Dinah Beams, M.A.,2 Molly Dalpes, Au.D.,4 Amanda Abrisch, Au.D.,3 Rosalinda Baca, Ph.D.,5 and Christine Yoshinaga-Itano, Ph.D.5 ABSTRACT This article will provide information about the Pragmatics Checklist, which consists of 45 items and is scored as: (1) not present, (2) present but preverbal, (3) present with one to three words, and (4) present with complex language. Information for both children who are deaf or hard of hearing and those with normal hearing are presented. Children who are deaf or hard of hearing are significantly older when demonstrating skill with complex language than their normal hearing peers. In general, even at the age of 7 years, there are several items that are not mastered by 75% of the deaf or hard of hearing children. Additionally, the article will provide some suggestions of strategies that can be considered as a means to facilitate the development of these pragmatic language skills for children who are deaf or hard of hearing. KEYWORDS: Deaf, hard of hearing, pragmatic language development Learning Outcomes: As a result of this activity, the reader will be able to (1) identify the pragmatic language skills that children who are deaf or hard of hearing have the most difficulty mastering by 7 years of age; (2) identify the pragmatic language skills that are the latest developed with complex language for children with normal hearing; (3) differentiate the rate of pragmatic language development by age and degree of hearing loss; (4) compare and contrast the pragmatic language skills of children with normal hearing and children who are deaf or hard of hearing by age; and (5) develop teaching strategies for preschool-aged children who are deaf or hard of hearing to develop specific pragmatic language skills that are typically developed significantly This document was downloaded for personal use only.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Language? Learning Goals
    Introduc)on To Linguis)cs Adam Szczegielniak What is Language? Learning Goals • What do we know when we know a language • Creavity in language • Competence/Performance • What is Grammar • Language Universals • Language Development • Language and Thought What is Language? • The ability to use language, perhaps more than any other attribute, distinguishes humans from other animals • But what does it mean to know a language? Linguistic Knowledge • When you know a language, you can speak (or sign) and be understood by others who know that language • Five-year-olds already know their first language(s) • The ability to use a language requires profound knowledge that most speakers don’t know that they know Cant help but use language • Try to not understand what I am saying • Just as impossible as trying not to be fooled by op)cal illusions • You cannot stop blinking or having peripheral vision • For example: Rotang Snakes – the circular snakes appear to rotate spontaneously in your peripheral vision as a result of the peripheral driM illusion. No)ce how the movement stops when you look directly at a collec)on of concentric circles. The illusion is probably caused by unconscious rapid eye movements and blinking Knowledge of the Sound System • When we know a language, we know what sounds (or signs) are used in the language and which sounds (or signs) are not • This also includes knowing how the sounds of the language can be combined – Which sounds may start a word – Which sounds may end a word – Which sounds may follow each other within a word
    [Show full text]
  • Online Journal in Public Archaeology
    ISSN: 2171-6315 Volume 7 - 2017 Editors: Jaime Almansa Sánchez & Elena Papagiannopoulou www.arqueologiapublica.es AP: Online Journal in Public Archaeology AP: Online Journal in Public Archaeology is edited by JAS Arqueología S.L.U. AP: Online Journal in Public Archaeology Volume 7 - 2017 p. 143-148 REVIEWS Alexandra ION Collecting, Ordering, Governing. Anthropology Museums and Liberal Government [by Tony Bennet et al.] Duke University Press ISBN: 9780822362531 360 pages “In their bodies is the record of their brotherhood” (Bennett et al. 2017, p. 256) This collective volume is an addition to a recent large body of literature which discusses the legacy of colonial encounters, and the ways in which anthropological practices of collecting have been embedded in political or scientific power relations (e.g. Baker 2010; Conklin 2013; Edwards 2012; Redman 2016). In this case, the analysis is placed at the cross-roads of museum studies and history of anthropology, with case studies spanning around 50 years. The reader is taken from the 1898-99 Torres Strait Island expedition, through the displays at Musée de l’Homme, or the Maori Ethnology Gallery (Dominion Museum, 1936), to the mid-century ‘The Race Question’ declaration on the fallacies of the race concept — and the subsequent critical reactions. The seven authors of this book try to develop an original perspective in exposing the specificities of the ‘museum phase of anthropology’, by understanding how in these different settings the cultural conceptions of difference have been articulated as a result of particular power configurations. By moving the focus between anthropologists in the field, to exhibitions, between biopolitical framings of local groups to international scientific networks, from photographs to texts, the authors propose 144 - Alexandra ION - Review: Collecting, Ordening, Governing a rethinking of the role of anthropology in ‘mediating the relations between the collecting practices of fieldwork, the ordering practices of museums, and the practices of social governance’ (p.
    [Show full text]
  • Redalyc.AUTONOMY in SIMULTANEOUS BILINGUALISM
    Revista Káñina ISSN: 0378-0473 [email protected] Universidad de Costa Rica Costa Rica Vásquez Carranza, Luz Marina AUTONOMY IN SIMULTANEOUS BILINGUALISM: EVIDENCE FROM AN ENGLISH- SPANISH BILINGUAL CHILD Revista Káñina, vol. XXXI, núm. 2, 2007, pp. 97-134 Universidad de Costa Rica San José, Costa Rica Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=44248781006 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative Káñina, Rev. Artes y Letras, Univ. Costa Rica. Vol. XXXI (2), pág. 97-134, 2007 / ISSN: 0378-0473 AUTONOMY IN SIMULTANEOUS BILINGUALISM: EVIDENCE FROM AN ENGLISH-SPANISH BILINGUAL CHILD Luz Marina Vásquez Carranza* ABSTRACT This study examines the simultaneous bilingual acquisition of English and Spanish by a boy from age 1;2.0 to 2;3.3. Transcripts and diary recordings were examined in order to establish the way in which phonological, lexical, mor- phological, syntactic, and pragmatic skills developed in the child’s speech. The child’s development was contrasted to that in monolingual English and Spanish-speaking children of comparable ages, and the results evidenced an analogous progress at the phonological and morpho-syntactic levels, which suggests language autonomy. The child’s rate of translation equivalents was also analyzed, as well as his rate of mixing and whether these were grammatically constrained. The boy started making use of translation equivalents as soon as these were available in his speech, and by and large, his mixing resulted from lack of equivalents in the two languages.
    [Show full text]