A Theory of Phenomenal Geometry and Its Applications
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Longitudinal Investigation of Disparity Vergence in Young Adult Convergence Insufficiency Patients
New Jersey Institute of Technology Digital Commons @ NJIT Theses Electronic Theses and Dissertations Summer 2019 Longitudinal investigation of disparity vergence in young adult convergence insufficiency patients Patrick C. Crincoli New Jersey Institute of Technology Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.njit.edu/theses Part of the Biomedical Engineering and Bioengineering Commons Recommended Citation Crincoli, Patrick C., "Longitudinal investigation of disparity vergence in young adult convergence insufficiency patients" (2019). Theses. 1683. https://digitalcommons.njit.edu/theses/1683 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Electronic Theses and Dissertations at Digital Commons @ NJIT. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ NJIT. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Copyright Warning & Restrictions The copyright law of the United States (Title 17, United States Code) governs the making of photocopies or other reproductions of copyrighted material. Under certain conditions specified in the law, libraries and archives are authorized to furnish a photocopy or other reproduction. One of these specified conditions is that the photocopy or reproduction is not to be “used for any purpose other than private study, scholarship, or research.” If a, user makes a request for, or later uses, a photocopy or reproduction for purposes in excess of “fair use” that user may be liable for copyright infringement, This institution -
Eye-Movement Studies of Visual Face Perception
Eye-movement studies of visual face perception Joseph Arizpe A dissertation submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy of the University College London Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience University College London 2015 1 Declaration I, Joseph Arizpe, confirm that the work presented in this thesis is my own. Where information has been derived from other sources, I confirm that this has been indicated in the thesis. 2 Abstract This thesis investigates factors influencing eye-movement patterns during face perception, the relationship of eye-movement patterns to facial recognition performance, and methodological considerations impacting the detection of differences in eye-movement patterns. In particular, in the first study (chapter 2), in which the basis of the other-race effect was investigated, differences in eye- movement patterns during recognition of own- versus other-race (African, Chinese) faces were found for Caucasian participants. However, these eye- movement differences were subtle and analysis-dependent, indicating that the discrepancy in prior reports regarding the presence or absence of such differences are due to variability in statistical sensitivity of analysis methods across studies. The second and third studies (chapters 3 and 4) characterized visuomotor factors, specifically pre-stimulus start position and distance, which strongly influence subsequent eye-movement patterns during face perception. An overall bias in fixation patterns to the opposite side of the face induced by start position and an increasing undershoot of the first ordinal fixation with increasing start distance were found. These visuomotor influences were not specific to faces and did not depend on the predictability of the location of the upcoming stimulus. -
The Figure Is in the Brain of the Beholder: Neural Correlates of Individual Percepts in The
The Figure is in the Brain of the Beholder: Neural Correlates of Individual Percepts in the Bistable Face-Vase Image A Thesis Presented to The Division of Philosophy, Religion, Psychology, and Linguistics Reed College In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Bachelor of Arts Phoebe Bauer May 2015 Approved for the Division (Psychology) Michael Pitts Acknowledgments I think some people experience a degree of unease when being taken care of, so they only let certain people do it, or they feel guilty when it happens. I don’t really have that. I love being taken care of. Here is a list of people who need to be explicitly thanked because they have done it so frequently and are so good at it: Chris: thank you for being my support system across so many contexts, for spinning with me, for constantly reminding me what I’m capable of both in and out of the lab. Thank you for validating and often mirroring my emotions, and for never leaving a conflict unresolved. Rennie: thank you for being totally different from me and yet somehow understanding the depths of my opinions and thought experiments. Thank you for being able to talk about magic. Thank you for being my biggest ego boost and accepting when I internalize it. Ben: thank you for taking the most important classes with me so that I could get even more out of them by sharing. Thank you for keeping track of priorities (quality dining: yes, emotional explanations: yes, fretting about appearances: nu-uh). #AshHatchtag & Stella & Master Tran: thank you for being a ceaseless source of cheer and laughter and color and love this year. -
UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society
UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society Title Voluntary versus Involuntary Perceptual Switching: Mechanistic Differences in Viewing an Ambiguous Figure Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/333348w4 Journal Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 27(27) ISSN 1069-7977 Authors Rambusch, Jana Ziemke, Tom Publication Date 2005 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Voluntary versus Involuntary Perceptual Switching: Mechanistic Differences in Viewing an Ambiguous Figure Michelle Umali ([email protected]) Center for Neurobiology & Behavior, Columbia University 1051 Riverside Drive, New York, NY, 10032, USA Marc Pomplun ([email protected]) Department of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts at Boston 100 Morrissey Blvd., Boston, MA 02125, USA Abstract frequency, blink frequency, and pupil size, which have been robustly correlated with cognitive function (see Rayner, Here we demonstrate the mechanistic differences between 1998, for a review). Investigators utilizing this method have voluntary and involuntary switching of the perception of an examined the regions within ambiguous figures that receive ambiguous figure. In our experiment, participants viewed a attention during a specific interpretation, as well as changes 3D ambiguous figure, the Necker cube, and were asked to maintain one of two possible interpretations across four in eye movement parameters that may specify the time of different conditions of varying cognitive load. These switch. conditions differed in the instruction to freely view, make For example, Ellis and Stark (1978) reported that guided saccades, or fixate on a central cross. In the fourth prolonged fixation duration occurs at the time of perceptual condition, subjects were instructed to make guided saccades switching. -
Hershenson (1992) Size-Distance Invariance. Kinetic Invariance Is
Perception & Psychophysics 1992, 51 (6), 541-548 Size-distance invariance: Kinetic invariance is different from static invariance MAURICE HERSHENSON Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts The static form of the size-distance invariance hypothesisasserts that a given proximal stimu- lus size (visual angle) determines a unique and constant ratio of perceived-object size to perceived object distance. A proposed kinetic invariance hypothesis asserts that a changing proximal stim- ulus size (an expanding or contracting solid visual angle) produces a constant perceived size and a changing perceived distance such that the instantaneous ratio of perceived size to perceived distance is determined by the instantaneous value of visual angle. The kinetic invariance hy- pothesis requires a new concept, an operating constraint, to mediate between the proximal ex- pansion or contraction pattern and the perception of rigid object motion in depth. As a conse- quence of the operating constraint, expansion and contraction patterns are automatically represented in consciousness as rigid objects. In certain static situations, the operation of this constraint produces the anomalous perceived-size-perceived-distance relations called the size- distance paradox. The size-distance invariance hypothesis (SDIH) asserts STATIC INVARIANCE RELATIONSHIP that a given proximal stimulus size (visual angle) deter- mines a unique constant ratio of perceived object size to Traditional Formulation perceived object distance (Epstein, 1977; Epstein, Park, The traditional form of the SDIH is derived from the & Casey, 1961; Kilpatrick & Ittelson, 1953). Is it possible analysis of stationary objects (Epstein, 1977; Gilinsky, to explain the moon illusion and retain this form of the 1951; Ittelson, 1951a; Johansson, 1977; Kilpatrick & It- SDIH? To do so, Kaufman and Rock (1962, 1989; Rock telson, 1953; Weintraub & Gardner, 1970). -
Frontoparietal Activity and Its Structural Connectivity in Binocular Rivalry
Author's personal copy BRAIN RESEARCH 1305 (2009) 96– 107 available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Frontoparietal activity and its structural connectivity in binocular rivalry Juliane C. Wilckea,b,⁎, Robert P. O'Sheac,d, Richard Wattsb,e aDepartment of Psychology, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand bDepartment of Physics and Astronomy, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand cDepartment of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand dPsychology, School of Health and Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Coffs Harbour, NSW, Australia eVan der Veer Institute for Parkinson's and Brain Research, Christchurch, New Zealand ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Article history: To understand the brain areas associated with visual awareness and their anatomical Accepted 20 September 2009 interconnections, we studied binocular rivalry with functional magnetic resonance imaging Available online 25 September 2009 (fMRI) and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). Binocular rivalry occurs when one image is viewed by one eye and a different image by the other; it is experienced as perceptual alternations Keywords: between the two images. Our first experiment addressed problems with a popular Visual awareness comparison condition, namely permanentsuppression,bycomparingrivalrywith Conscious perception binocular fusion instead. We found an increased fMRI signal in right frontal, parietal, and Binocular rivalry occipital regions during rivalry viewing. The pattern of neural activity differed from findings Binocular fusion of permanent suppression comparisons, except for adjacent activity in the right superior fMRI parietal lobule. This location was near fMRI signal changes related to reported rivalry DTI tractography alternations in our second experiment, indicating that neighbouring areas in the right parietal cortex may be involved in different components of rivalry. -
M Pathway and Areas 44 and 45 Are Involved in Stereoscopic Recognition Based on Binocular Disparity
Japanese Journal of Physiology, 52, 191–198, 2002 M Pathway and Areas 44 and 45 Are Involved in Stereoscopic Recognition Based on Binocular Disparity Tsuneo NEGAWA, Shinji MIZUNO*, Tomoya HAHASHI, Hiromi KUWATA†, Mihoko TOMIDA, Hiroaki HOSHI*, Seiichi ERA, and Kazuo KUWATA Departments of Physiology, * Radiology, and † Nursing Course, Gifu University School of Medicine, Gifu, 500–8705 Japan Abstract: We characterized the visual path- was reported that these regions were inactive ways involved in the stereoscopic recognition of during the monocular stereopsis. To separate the the random dot stereogram based on the binocu- specific responses directly caused by the stereo- lar disparity employing a functional magnetic res- scopic recognition process from the nonspecific onance imaging (fMRI). The V2, V3, V4, V5, in- ones caused by the memory load or the inten- traparietal sulcus (IPS) and the superior temporal tion, we designed a novel frequency labeled sulcus (STS) were significantly activated during tasks (FLT) sequence. The functional MRI using the binocular stereopsis, but the inferotemporal the FLT indicated that the activation of areas 44 gyrus (ITG) was not activated. Thus a human M and 45 is correlated with the stereoscopic recog- pathway may be part of a network involved in the nition based on the binocular disparity but not stereoscopic processing based on the binocular with the intention artifacts, suggesting that areas disparity. It is intriguing that areas 44 (Broca’s 44 and 45 play an essential role in the binocular area) and 45 in the left hemisphere were also ac- disparity. [Japanese Journal of Physiology, 52, tive during the binocular stereopsis. -
Symmetric Networks with Geometric Constraints As Models of Visual Illusions
S S symmetry Article Symmetric Networks with Geometric Constraints as Models of Visual Illusions Ian Stewart 1,*,† and Martin Golubitsky 2,† 1 Mathematics Institute, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK 2 Department of Mathematics, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] † These authors contributed equally to this work. Received: 17 May 2019; Accepted: 13 June 2019; Published: 16 June 2019 Abstract: Multistable illusions occur when the visual system interprets the same image in two different ways. We model illusions using dynamic systems based on Wilson networks, which detect combinations of levels of attributes of the image. In most examples presented here, the network has symmetry, which is vital to the analysis of the dynamics. We assume that the visual system has previously learned that certain combinations are geometrically consistent or inconsistent, and model this knowledge by adding suitable excitatory and inhibitory connections between attribute levels. We first discuss 4-node networks for the Necker cube and the rabbit/duck illusion. The main results analyze a more elaborate model for the Necker cube, a 16-node Wilson network whose nodes represent alternative orientations of specific segments of the image. Symmetric Hopf bifurcation is used to show that a small list of natural local geometric consistency conditions leads to alternation between two global percepts: cubes in two different orientations. The model also predicts brief transitional states in which the percept involves impossible rectangles analogous to the Penrose triangle. A tristable illusion generalizing the Necker cube is modelled in a similar manner. -
The Perception of Transparent Three-Dimensional Objects (Vision͞illusion͞visual Learning͞association)
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 94, pp. 6517–6522, June 1997 Neurobiology The perception of transparent three-dimensional objects (visionyillusionyvisual learningyassociation) DALE PURVES* AND TIMOTHY J. ANDREWS Department of Neurobiology, Box 3209, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC 27710 Contributed by Dale Purves, April 9, 1997 ABSTRACT When the proximal and distal elements of orientation, the cube appears to be balanced on its distal– wire-frame cubes are conflated, observers perceive illusory inferior vertex, with the surface on which it actually rests rising structures that no longer behave veridically. These phenom- from the balance point (see Figs. 1 and 2). (Illusory, in this ena suggest that what we normally see depends on visual case, means an interpretation of the stimulus that does not associations generated by experience. The necessity of such accord with the configuration of the object determined by learning may explain why the mammalian visual system is direct measurement.) In short, the observer no longer judges subject to a prolonged period of plasticity in early life, when the object to be a cube, despite the unchanged retinal image, novel circuits are made in enormous numbers. knowledge of its actual structure, and the immediately pre- ceding perception of a cube in top-down view. Information generated by the eyes is ambiguous. Everyday we A first order explanation of these phenomena follows from have to make decisions (about the size and distance of objects, the geometry of the situation. Because of their greater dis- their form, and whether they are moving) based on retinal tance, the angles subtended on the retina by the distal elements images that can have two or more meanings (1–4). -
Seeing Black and White
Seeing Black and White Alan Gilchrist OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Seeing Black and White This page intentionally left blank SEEING BLACK AND WHITE Alan Gilchrist 1 2006 1 Oxford University Press, Inc., publishes works that further Oxford University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education. Oxford New York Auckland Cape Town Dar es Salaam Hong Kong Karachi Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi New Delhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto With offices in Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam Copyright ᭧ 2006 by Oxford University Press, Inc. Published by Oxford University Press, Inc. 198 Madison Avenue, New York, New York 10016 www.oup.com Oxford is a registered trademark of Oxford University Press All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior permission of Oxford University Press. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Gilchrist, Alan. Seeing black and white / by Alan Gilchrist. p. cm. (Oxford psychology series; no. 40) Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-19-518716-4 ISBN-13 978-0-19-518716-8 1. Imagery (Psychology). I. Title. II. Series. BF241.L54 2006 153—dc22 2006006283 987654321 Printed in the United States of America on acid-free paper This book is dedicated to the memory of Irvin Rock, my wonderful mentor and warm friend. This page intentionally left blank Foreword And God said let Gilchrist be and all was light. -
Vision and Mind: Selected Readings in the Philosophy of Perception
ision and Mind Selected Readings in the Philosophy of Perception edited by Alva Noë and Evan Thompson Vision and Mind This Page Intentionally Left Blank Vision and Mind Selected Readings in the Philosophy of Perception edited by Alva Noë and Evan Thompson A Bradford Book The MIT Press Cambridge, Massachusetts London, England © 2002 Massachusetts Institute of Technology All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form by any electronic or mechanical means (including photocopying, recording, or information storage and retrieval) without permission in writing from the publisher. This book was set in Sabon by SNP Best-set Typesetter Ltd., Hong Kong. Printed and bound in the United States of America. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Vision and mind : selected readings in the philosophy of perception / edited by Alva Noë and Evan Thompson. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-262-14078-0 (alk. paper)—ISBN 0-262-64047-3 (pbk. : alk. paper) 1. Perception (Philosophy) I. Noë, Alva. II. Thompson, Evan. B828.45 .V565 2002 121¢.34—dc21 2002023533 Contents Preface vii Sources ix 1 Introduction 1 Alva Noë and Evan Thompson 2 Selections from Phenomenology of Perception 15 Maurice Merleau-Ponty 3 Some Remarks about the Senses 35 H. P. Grice 4 The Intentionality of Sensation: A Grammatical Feature 55 G. E. M. Anscombe 5 A Theory of Direct Visual Perception 77 James J. Gibson 6 Perception and Its Objects 91 P. F. Strawson 7 Perceptions as Hypotheses 111 Richard L. Gregory 8 Veridical Hallucination and Prosthetic Vision 135 David Lewis 9 Perception, Vision and Causation 151 Paul Snowdon 10 How Direct Is Visual Perception?: Some Reflections on Gibson’s “Ecological Approach” 167 Jerry A. -
Brain Networks Underlying Bistable Perception
This is a repository copy of Brain networks underlying bistable perception. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/88383/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Baker, Daniel Hart orcid.org/0000-0002-0161-443X, Karapanagiotidis, Theodoros orcid.org/0000-0002-0813-1019, Coggan, David et al. (2 more authors) (2015) Brain networks underlying bistable perception. Neuroimage. pp. 229-234. ISSN 1053-8119 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.053 Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ Baker, Karapanagiotidis, Coggan, Wailes-Newson & Smallwood (2015) Neuroimage, 119: 229-234 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.053 Brain networks underlying bistable perception Daniel H. Baker, Theodoros Karapanagiotidis, David D. Coggan, Kirstie Wailes-Newson & Jonathan Smallwood Department of Psychology, University of York, YO10 5DD, UK email: [email protected] Abstract Bistable stimuli, such as the Necker Cube, demonstrate that experience can change in the absence of changes in the environment.