John F. Eisenberg Papers, Circa 1960-1982 and Undated

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

John F. Eisenberg Papers, Circa 1960-1982 and Undated John F. Eisenberg Papers, circa 1960-1982 and undated Finding aid prepared by Smithsonian Institution Archives Smithsonian Institution Archives Washington, D.C. Contact us at [email protected] Table of Contents Collection Overview ........................................................................................................ 1 Administrative Information .............................................................................................. 1 Historical Note.................................................................................................................. 1 Introduction....................................................................................................................... 2 Descriptive Entry.............................................................................................................. 2 Names and Subjects ...................................................................................................... 3 Container Listing ............................................................................................................. 5 John F. Eisenberg Papers http://siarchives.si.edu/collections/siris_arc_217566 Collection Overview Repository: Smithsonian Institution Archives, Washington, D.C., [email protected] Title: John F. Eisenberg Papers Identifier: Record Unit 7411 Date: circa 1960-1982 and undated Extent: 18 cu. ft. (18 record storage boxes) Creator:: Eisenberg, John Frederick, 1935- Language: Language of Materials: English Administrative Information Prefered Citation Smithsonian Institution Archives, Record Unit 7411, John F. Eisenberg Papers Historical Note John Eisenberg was born June 20, 1935 in Everett, Washington. He received a B.S. at Washington State University in 1957 and an M.A. from the University of California, Berkeley, in 1959. He received a Ph.D. in zoology from the University of California, Berkeley, in 1962 and went to the University of British Columbia, where he stayed from 1962 to 1964 as an assistant professor of zoology. In 1964 he accepted a post at the University of Maryland, College Park, as assistant professor of zoology and in 1965 was made research associate professor of zoology. In 1972 he became a research professor at the University of Maryland. In 1973 he was made an associate of the Department of Mental Hygiene at Johns Hopkins University, and he held this title until 1978. In 1964 the National Zoological Park (NZP) organized a division to do research. On September 1, 1965 Eisenberg was appointed to the post of resident scientist in the Division of Research at the National Zoological Park. He worked with Edwin Gould of John's Hopkins University on an ecological and behavioral study of the tenrecoid insectivores of Madagascar from January 1966 to April 1966. In January 1967 he left for Ceylon to initiate an elephant research project. At the end of January he returned to Madagascar to continue his studies of the tenrecoid. In April he returned to Washington, D.C., then flew back to Ceylon in May for field inspections of the elephant project. He started a year-long residency in Ceylon June 10, 1968, and during October did another field inspection as well as teaching a course to Ceylonese personnel on immobilizing wild elephants. In January he met with Dr. Paul Leyhausen of the Max Planck Institute. By 1970 the fieldwork on the Ceylon Elephant Project was complete. In 1971 the research division, under his direction, started the captive breeding of papcaranas and a research project on sloths in Panama. In March he was the acting director for the Zoological Program; he went back to being resident scientist in 1972 because the Zoological Program was dissolved. Page 1 of 67 John F. Eisenberg Papers http://siarchives.si.edu/collections/siris_arc_217566 The study on the sloth continued in Panama during 1972. He became president of the Animal Behavior Society in 1973, and the scientist in charge of the office of Scientific Research at the National Zoological Park. The sloth study continued in Panama, led by Dr. G. C. Montgomery. During 1974 progress was made on 24 research projects that included: the sloth study, a study of the behavior scoring of female mammals in heat, and reproduction in caviomorph rodents. He was also involved with the Thirteenth International Congress of Ethology that took place in Washington, D.C. In January 1975 he left for Venezuela to do herpetological and mammalian studies, and research in Venezuela continued until June. The Venezuelan field projects at Guatopo National Park and the ranch of Sr. Tomas Blohm started in earnest in 1976. During 1977 the Venezuelan projects continued, and a new project on the vocal communication in cogeneric wrens started in Panama. These projects continued through 1978, and a new project studying the toque monkey in Ceylon started as well. In 1979 Dr. Eisenberg was made the assistant director for animal programs at the National Zoological Park. He became responsible for all animal and educational programs run by the National Zoological Park. Vertebrate Ecology in the Northern Neotropics, which he edited, was published in 1980 by the Smithsonian Press. In 1981 he did a field study of the Cuban solendon in Cuba and visited various national parks in India. In April he spent three weeks in China discussing the possibility of setting a series of research projects based in a national park. In September his monograph, The Mammalian Radiations, was published. He also received the prestigious C. Hart Merriam award from the American Society of Mammalogists in 1981. He continued in the post of assistant director of animal and education programs until August 21, 1982, when he resigned to become Ordway professor of ecosystem conservation at the University of Florida in Gainesville. Introduction This finding aid was digitized with funds generously provided by the Smithsonian Institution Women's Committee. Descriptive Entry The Venezuela Project (1974-1982) was one of three big projects that Dr. Eisenberg directed during his time at the National Zoological Park. The papers documenting this project include grant information and progress reports, correspondence between Sr. Tomas Blohm (research took place on his ranch), information on personnel who participated in the project, financial records connected to the project, background information and publicity, and other correspondence. Included are photographs and newspaper clippings. The Ceylon Project included research on elephants (1967-1976) and primates (1968-1982). Papers connected to this project include a research agreement with the Ceylonese government, grant information and progress reports, financial records, correspondence with Dittus Wolfgang, George McKay, and other researchers in Ceylon, and information on elephant immobilization techniques learned from the Ringling Brothers' elephant handlers. Angela Daugharty writes an interesting letter connected to the elephant project. Suzanne Ripley, co-investigator, was an integral part of the research done in Ceylon, but none of her correspondence is found here. Oddly enough the correspondence between Eisenberg and Ripley is found in box 16. Papers document research on pregnant elephants and dugongs. Correspondence from contacts within the Ceylonese infrastructure is included. Page 2 of 67 John F. Eisenberg Papers http://siarchives.si.edu/collections/siris_arc_217566 The Panama project was the third large, long-term project that Dr. Eisenberg was involved in. In Panama research centered on sloths, howler monkeys, iguanas, anteaters, and various plant studies done in conjunction with the two-toed sloths. All of these are represented to some degree in the Panama files. Grant and financial records, logistics paperwork, progress reports, and several manuscripts are also included. Correspondence is mainly from Dr. Montgomery, who led the research effort in Panama. Dr. Eisenberg, in his capacity as head of the research office at the National Zoological Park, received information from many sources on many subjects. The bulk of this paperwork can be found in his subject files. Information in these files comes from research projects as well as events within the Zoological Park. Subjects touched upon include a memorandum listing possible projects for the 1976 Bicentennial at the National Zoological Park, a memorandum on carcasses, correspondence on the Deer Project, various research reports, and reports on the Sleep Project. Information from the Smithsonian Foreign Currency Program is also included. Like the subject files, the general correspondence files cover a broad spectrum of people and activities, and document Eisenberg's career within and outside the Smithsonian. Correspondence comes from educators, graduate students, Smithsonian staff, research scientists, members of professional societies, and curators. Many of these contacts are personal and some are international; they include discussion of possible drugs to immobilize wolves, letters from people inquiring about the white tigers, letters on speaking engagements, references provided by Eisenberg, and even a Christmas card. Some correspondents also sent reports dealing with research in the zoological field. Eisenberg was a member of many scientific societies and published in many scientific journals. He was often invited to attend conferences and to review articles by other scientists in his field; this component of his professional life is documented in this collection. Papers include book reviews by Eisenberg, letters from Elsevier Scientific
Recommended publications
  • Biography (Modified, After Festetics 1983)
    Konrad Lorenz’s Biography (modified, after Festetics 1983) 1903: Konrad Zacharias Lorenz (KL) was born in Altenberg /Austria on Nov. 7 as the last of three children of Emma Lorenz and Dr. Adolf Lorenz, professor for orthopedics at the Medical branch of the University of Vienna. In the same year the representative and spacious Altenberg family home was finished. 1907: KL starts keeping animals, such as spotted newts in aquaria, raises some ducklings and is not pleased by his first experiences with a dachshound. Niko Tinbergen, his lifelong colleague and friend, is born on April 15 in Den Haag, The Netherlands. 1909: KL enters elementary school and engages in systematic studies in crustaceans. 1910: Oskar Heinroth, biologist and founder of "Vergleichende Verhaltensforschung" (comparative ethology) from Berlin and fatherlike scientific mentor of the young KL publishes his classical paper on the ethology of ducks. 1915: KL enters highschool (Schottengymnasium Wien), keeps and breeds songbirds. 1918: Wallace Craig publishes the comparative ethology of Columbidae (pigeons), a classics of late US biologist Charles O. Whitman, who was like O. Heinroth, a founding father of comparative ethology. 1921: KL excels in his final exams. Together with friend Bernhard Hellmann, he observes and experiments with aggression in a cichlid (Herichthys cyanoguttatum). This was the base for KL's psychohydraulic model of motivation. 1922: Father Adolf sends KL to New York to take 2 semesters of medicine courses at the ColumbiaUniversity, but mainly to interrupt the relationship of KL with longterm girlfriend Gretl Gebhart, his later wife. This paternal attempt to influence the mate choice of KL failed.
    [Show full text]
  • Konrad Lorenz, NL Nobel Laureate in Physiology Or Medicine-1973
    Glossary on Kalinga Prize Laureates UNESCO – Kalinga Prize Winner – 1974 Konrad Lorenz, NL Nobel Laureate in Physiology or Medicine-1973 Great Zoologist and Ethologist [Birth : 7th November 1903 in Vienna, Austria Death : 27th February 1989, Vienna] Truth in Science can be defined as the working hypothesis best suited to open the way to the next better one. …Konrad Lonenz It is a good morning exercise for a research scientist to discard a Pet hypothesis every day before breakfast. It keeps him young. …Konrad Lonenz 1 Glossary on Kalinga Prize Laureates Konrad Lorenz Biography Ethology – Imprinting Konrad Lorenz (Konard Zacharisa Lorenz) was born on November 7, 1903 in Vienna, Austria. As a little boy, he loved animals and had a collection that include fish, dogs, monkeys, insects, ducks, and geese. His interest in animal behaviour was intense.When he was 10 years old, Lorenz became aware of the existence of the Theory of Evolution through reading a book by Wilhelm Bölsche in which he was fascinated by a picture of an Archaeopteryx. Evolution gave him insight-his father had explained that the word “insect” was derived from the notches, the “incisions” between the segments-if reptiles could become birds, annelid worms could develop into insects. As he grew towards adulthood he wanted to become a paleontologist, however he reluctantly followed his father’s wishes, and studied medicine at the University of Vienna and at Columbia University. He later regarded this compliance to have been in his own best interests as one of his teachers of anatomy, Ferdinand Hochstetter, proved to be a brilliant comparative anatomist and embryologist and a dedicated teacher of the comparative method.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Humaneness Canine? Email-Version
    WMS-NEWVEE98.pdf Schleidt, W. M. 1998 Is humaneness canine? Human Ethology Bulletin 13(4):1-4. In recent years, various fields of science-- neurobiology, sociobiology, behavioral ecology and game theory, to name but a few--have opened new vistas on human origins and on the question of what makes humans such special animals. Much attention has been focused on the importance of brain, kinship, competition, and evolutionary stable strategies. Even Machiavellian intelligence has been accepted as an old primate heritage1. Given this new scientific wisdom one may wonder how traits of humaneness--true altruism, idealism or human kindness--could have evolved and become a recurrent phenomenon in various human populations. How could some clever but rather beastly Anatomically Modern Humans (AMHs) have turned toward humaneness? How could AMHs have invented forms of cooperation, communication, society culminating in individual sacrifice unrivaled by any other mammal? AMH is the only primate that has evolved the capacity for "true friendship": loyalty beyond kinship. Konrad Lorenz once stated very bluntly: "of all creatures the one nearest to man in the fineness of its perceptions and in its capacity to render true friendship is a bitch."2 There is something in the bond among wolves and dogs and humans that can reach beyond what even our closest primate relatives, chimpanzees, do. I am NOT talking about brain power now, but about what we poetically associate with "kindness of heart." Jane Goodall, commenting on Konrad Lorenz´s statement, writes: Schleidt, W. M. 1998 Is humaneness canine? Human Ethology Bulletin 13(4):1-4. page 1 of 7 WMS-NEWVEE98.pdf "Dogs have been domesticated for a very long time.
    [Show full text]
  • Konrad Lorenz 1985 My Family and Other Animals In: D.A.Dewsbury (Ed.) Leaders in the Study of Animal Behavior: Autobiographical Perspectives
    Konrad Lorenz 1985 My family and other animals In: D.A.Dewsbury (ed.) Leaders in the Study of Animal Behavior: autobiographical perspectives. Lewisburg, PA: Bucknell Univ. Press. pp. 258-287 [OCR by Konrad Lorenz Haus Altenberg – http://klha.at] Seitenumbrüche und -zahlen wie im Original. K. Lorenz 1985 My family and other animals 258 My family and other animals Konrad Lorenz watching well-fed, large Zanclus canescens, a species difficult to keep in the middle of Europe. K. Lorenz 1985 My family and other animals 259 When, as a scientist, one tries to get insight into the deeper roots of one's own life interest, it is quite instructive to delve into earliest childhood memories. As a very little boy, I loved owls and I was quite determined to become an owl. In this choice of profession I was swayed by the consideration that an owl was not put to bed as early as I was, but was allowed to roam freely throughout the night. I learned to swim very early and when I realized that owls could not swim, they lost my esteem. My yearning for universality drove me to want to become an animal that could fly and swim and sit on trees. A photograph of a Hawaiian goose sitting on a branch induced me to choose a Sandwich goose as my life's ideal. I was not yet quite six years old when I was hit by the impact of Selma Lagerlöf's immortal book The Journey of Little Nils Holgerson with the Wild Geese. Consequently I wanted to become the sort of wild goose idealized by the Swedish poet.
    [Show full text]
  • Cambridge.University.Press-How.Well.Do.Facts.Travel.2010.Retail.Ebook.Pdf
    This page intentionally left blank HOW WELL DO FACTS TRAVEL? Why write about facts? Facts are everywhere. They litter the utterances of public life as much as the private conversations of individuals. They frequent the humanities and the sciences in equal measure. But their very ubiquity tells us not only why it is difficult to form general but sensible answers in response to seemingly simple questions about facts, but also why it is important to do so. This book discusses how facts travel, and when and why they sometimes travel well enough to acquire an independent life of their own. Whether or not facts travel in this manner depends not only on their character and ability to play useful roles elsewhere, but also on the labels, packaging, vehicles, and company that take them across difficult terrains and over disciplinary boundar- ies. These diverse stories of travelling facts, ranging from architecture to nano- technology and from romance fiction to climate science, change the way we see the nature of facts. Facts are far from the bland and rather boring but useful objects that scientists and humanists produce and fit together to make narra- tives, arguments, and evidence. Rather, their extraordinary abilities to travel well – and to fly flags of many different colours in the process – show when, how, and why facts can be used to build further knowledge beyond and away from their sites of original production and intended use. Peter Howlett is an expert on the economic history of World Wars I and II, and contributed the text for the official history Fighting with Figures (1995).
    [Show full text]
  • The Hawk/Goose Story: the Classical Ethological Experiments of Lorenz and Tinbergen, Revisited
    Journal of Comparative Psychology © 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 125, No. 2, 121–133 0735-7036/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0022068 The Hawk/Goose Story: The Classical Ethological Experiments of Lorenz and Tinbergen, Revisited Wolfgang Schleidt Michael D. Shalter University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria Independent Practice, Castres, France Humberto Moura-Neto Independent Practice, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil We present a historical account of the story behind the famous hawk/goose experiments of Lorenz and Tinbergen in a wider context of cognitive ethology. We discuss their significance, for ethological experimentation in general, and specifically for understanding innate constraints on cognition. As examples of the continuing significance of the hawk/goose paradigm of selective habituation, we discuss its relation to “exposure therapy” of human phobias and the use of hawk silhouettes as deterrents for songbirds. Finally we rephrase Uexku¨ll’s thesis of taxon-specific worlds (“Umwelten”) as a “Theory of World.” Keywords: selective habituation, hawk/goose experiments, cognitive ethology, theory of world, phobias, predator-prey Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022068.supp Based largely on the involvement of the senior author of this scientists, who were already well set on their individual courses. For paper in his 1961 attempt to replicate the famous 1937 hawk/goose Lorenz and Tinbergen, however, it proved pivotal . The sympo- experiments (Figure 1) of Lorenz and Tinbergen (Lorenz, 1939; sium’s significance was that it provided the occasion for Lorenz and Tinbergen, 1939; Schleidt, 1961a, 1961b, English translation, Tinbergen to meet one another. Each of them had the chance to begin Schleidt, 1974) we present a historical account of the story behind to recognize how well the other’s strength complemented his own.
    [Show full text]
  • Edited by Walter Bock
    REVIEWS Edited by Walter Bock The Audubon Society Field Guide to North American Birds. Eastern Region.--John Bull and John Farrand, Jr. 775 pp., 584 colorphotographs. Western Region.--Miklos D. F. Udvardy. 850 pp., 62? colorphotographs. 1977. New York, Alfred A. Knopf; producedby ChanticleerPress, Inc., N.Y. $7.95 each.--Over four decadeshave passed since Roger Tory Peterson,welding brilliant visual insight onto the pioneeringframework of suchauthors as Ralph Hoffman, producedthe first practical field guide. Later, exceptional workers like Pough and Eckelberry, Robbins and Singer, etc. created variations on this principle through different approachesin format and style, but their efforts were always anchored in the belief that there can be few short-cutsto the learning process. Now, in bland disregardof yearsof accumulatedwisdom, the publisherand editorsof this currentset of booksare presentingwhat purportsto be a "revolutionary"method of field identification,one destinedto supplant outmodedconcepts. So pervasivelyeffective has been the promotion and distribution of these booksthat it couldonly be the disinterestedperson who is unacquaintedwith the generalformat. Therefore, this review will not take up spacedescribing such treatment in detail. It is only necessaryto state that this "new" methodis predicatedupon severalmisconceptions: that photographyis somehowmore realistic,less interpretivethan painting, and that the arbitrary assignmentof speciesinto categoriesof shapeor color(in the picturesection), and habitat(in the text) is both morespecific and moreinformative than the traditional arrangement reflecting actual relationships. To addressthe first issue,the questionmust be askedwhether photographscan be substitutedfor precise, diagnosticillustrations in a field guide. There is certainlyan abundanceof excellentcolor photography on displayhere, especially among the waterfowl,Laridae, shorebirds,flying raptors, and woodpeckers, almost all of it in faultlessreproduction.
    [Show full text]
  • Quo Vadis, Behavioural Biology? Past, Present, and Future of an Evolving Science
    Quo Vadis, Behavioural Biology? Past, Present, and Future of an Evolving Science NOVA ACTA LEOPOLDINA Abhandlungen der Deutschen Akademie der Naturforscher Leopoldina Herausgegeben von Jörg HACKER, Präsident der Akademie NEUE FOLGE NUMMER 380 BAND 111 Quo Vadis, Behavioural Biology? Past, Present, and Future of an Evolving Science International Symposium of the Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin and the German National Academy of Sciences Leopoldina Berlin April 30 to May 4, 2009 Editors: Andreas WESSEL (Berlin) Randolf MENZEL (Berlin) Member of the Academy Günter TEMBROCK (†) Member of the Academy With 139 Figures and 2 Tables Deutsche Akademie der Naturforscher Leopoldina – Nationale Akademie der Wissenschaften, Halle (Saale) 2013 Wissenschaftliche Verlagsgesellschaft Stuttgart Redaktion: Dr. Michael KAASCH und Dr. Joachim KAASCH Die Schriftenreihe Nova Acta Leopoldina erscheint bei der Wissenschaftlichen Verlagsgesellschaft Stutt- gart, Birkenwaldstraße 44, 70191 Stuttgart, Bundesrepublik Deutschland. Jedes Heft ist einzeln käuflich. Die Schriftenreihe wird gefördert durch das Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung sowie das Ministe- rium für Wissenschaft und Wirtschaft des Landes Sachsen-Anhalt. Wir danken der Deutschen Forschungsgemeinschaft und der Humboldt-Universitäts-Gesellschaft für die finan- zielle Unterstützung der Veranstaltung. Cover: The three foxes represent fundamentally different aspects of behaviour. The first fox stands for motivation and orientation towards a goal at a distance. The second fox signifies activation of a motor pattern with identification of the spatial position of the goal. The third fox marks the end of the sequence with consummatory acts, such as killing and eating. Bibliografische Information der Deutschen Nationalbibliothek Die Deutsche Nationalbibliothek verzeichnet diese Publikation in der Deutschen Nationalbibliografie; detaillierte bibliografische Daten sind im Internet über http//dnb.ddb.de abrufbar.
    [Show full text]