<<

University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln

Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology, Department of

June 2006

Two Sides of the Same Coin? The Relations between Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors

Meredith McGinley University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected]

Gustavo Carlo University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/psychfacpub

Part of the Psychiatry and Psychology Commons

McGinley, Meredith and Carlo, Gustavo, "Two Sides of the Same Coin? The Relations between Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors" (2006). Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology. 57. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/psychfacpub/57

This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Psychology, Department of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications, Department of Psychology by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Published in Journal of Youth and Adolescence: A Multidisciplinary Research Publication (2006). Copyright © Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006. Used by permission. DOI 10.1007/s10964-006-9095-9

Two Sides of the Same Coin? The Relations between Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors Meredith McGinley * and Gustavo Carlo †

* Meredith McGinley is a graduate student in the Developmental Psychology program, Department of Psychology, University of Ne- braska–Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska 68588. Her research interests include positive social and moral behaviors in children and adolescents and methodology. Email: [email protected] † Professor of Developmental Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska 68588. Dr. Carlo’s main interests include the individual, , and cultural correlates of positive social and moral behaviors in children and adolescents.

Submitted May 2005; accepted July 2005; published online July 21, 2006

Abstract: The direct and indirect relations between six types of prosocial behavior and physical were examined. Data were gathered from 252 college students (M age = 21.67 years; 184 women) who completed mea- sures of , prosocial behavior, and physical aggression. Structural equation modeling revealed that sympa- thy fully mediated the relations between compliant prosocial behaviors and physical aggression, and partially medi- ated the relations between and physical aggression and public prosocial behaviors and physical aggression. The fi ndings suggest that the relations between prosocial behaviors and aggression are complex and that prosocial behavior should not be treated as a unitary construct. Keywords: Prosocial behaviors, Sympathy, Aggression, Adolescence, Methodology

Humans have the capacity to act both aggressively is considerable evidence for the existence of selfl essly and altruistically, and sometimes can engage in both be- motivated prosocial behaviors (e.g., Batson et al., 2002; haviors simultaneously (Feshbach and Feshbach, 1986; Eisenberg, 2003); though some support for egoistically Zahn-Waxler et al., 1986). For example, witnessing a motivated prosocial behaviors has also been presented person experience a distressing event might incite both (e.g., Cialdini et al., 1987). Because aggression is con- anger at the aggressor (fueling desires to aggress, or to ceptualized as a selfi shly motivated , fur- become hostile towards another) and sympathy for the ther evidence that lends credence to the selfl essly mo- victim (fueling helping desires; Vitaglione and Barnett, tivated argument is that some prior researchers have 2003). This authentic concern for another’s plight, or al- shown negative relations between aggression and pro- truism, is one motivator of prosocial behavior, which social behaviors. However, the evidence on the relations can be defi ned as any behavior someone engages in that between aggression and prosocial behavior is not clear. benefi ts or helps another (Eisenberg, 2003; Zahn-Waxler For example, scholars using cluster analyses have et al., 1986). shown that in certain groups of children, high levels of However, there is considerable research on whether prosocial behavior coexist with aggressiveness (Haapas- altruism is truly motivated by selfl ess motives. The crux alo et al., 2000; Pulkkinen and Tremblay, 1992). Re- of this debate is whether prosocial behaviors are driven searchers examining peer relationships have additional- by egoistic motives or whether they are driven by self- ly reported that most children exhibit at least some level less motives (Batson, 1998). In addition, some scholars of both prosocial and aggressive behaviors (Coie and have argued that some types of prosocial behaviors may Kupersmidt, 1983; Dodge, 1983). Aggressive children, be egoistically motivated and other types may be self- moreover, do not always exhibit discernible differences lessly motivated (e.g., Carlo and Randall, 2001). There in showing concern for or helping others when compared

1 2 McGinley & Carlo in Journal of Youth & Adolescence (2006) to nonviolent peers. For example, Crick and Grotpeter sensitive to the troubles of others. In turn, this increase (1995) reported no mean differences in peer nominations in feeling sorrow for another, or sympathy, might pre- of prosocial behavior between relationally nonaggres- vent the individual from engaging in aggressive behav- sive and aggressive children. These aggressive children iors (Bandura et al., 2001). Several researchers have fur- sometimes display a greater concern towards strangers thermore agreed that engaging in prosocial behavior is at younger ages because of their poorer impulse control an important buffer that may protect against the devel- and emotion regulation (Feshbach and Feshbach, 1986; opment of aggressive or antisocial behavior in children Gill and Calkins, 2003; Hastings et al., 2000). Howev- as they become older (Eron and Huesmann, 1984; Kuc- er, Miller and Eisenberg (1988) in their meta-analysis re- zynski and Kochanska, 1995; Haapasalo et al., 2000; ported overall negative relations between and Hastings et al., 2000; Tremblay et al., 1992; Vitaro et al., externalizing or aggressive behavior. 1990). The present paper will thus examine the possibili- Several investigators have furthermore theorized that ty that engaging in prosocial behavior has an indirect ef- measures of prosocial and aggressive behaviors are or- fect on physical aggression via sympathy. thogonal (Pulkkinen, 1984). Empirical research has supported this notion. Caprara et al. (2001), for exam- Defi ning types of prosocial behavior ple, reported that the correlations between self, teacher, and peer-reported prosocial and aggressive behavior at Many studies examining prosocial behavior implement seven different time points (from ages 7–13, inclusive- global assessments to capture this construct. Global as- ly) were generally nonsignifi cant. These constructs have sessments measure the likelihood of engaging in a proso- emerged as separate factors after conducting explorato- cial behavior across situations and personal motivations. ry factor analyses (Crick and Grotpeter, 1995; Harris At times, these global measures can include aspects of et al., 1996) and seem to maintain their orthogonal na- a broader construct that subsumes prosocial behavior, ture after taking into account measurement unreliabili- namely social competence. However, the usefulness of ty, responder bias (a tendency to acquiesce) and general global measures might be limited as subtypes of proso- level of social interaction (Krueger et al., 2001; Radke- cial behaviors have unique correlates that are otherwise Yarrow et al., 1976). masked when implementing such measures. Instead, Given that the existing literature suggests that proso- it might be benefi cial to categorize prosocial behaviors cial and aggressive behaviors can co-exist and have lit- as either a product of the situation or personal motives tle or no direct relation with each other, it makes sense (Carlo and Randall, 2001). For example, Persson (2005) to contend they are not two sides of the same coin. observed three types of aggression and two motives of However, stating that prosocial and aggressive behav- observed prosocial behavior: altruistic and “acting out” ior have little or no infl uence on one another might be altruism, sometimes with an egocentric intention, over a premature conclusion for two reasons. The fi rst con- the course of three years. While the measures of the al- cerns how prosocial behavior has been defi ned in the truistic motive were signifi cantly and negatively related literature. Unlike aggression, when studying prosocial to measures of reactive and proactive hostile aggression, behavior researchers have generally employed glob- the more egocentric or “acting out” prosocial measures al measures (Carlo and Randall, 2002), although sev- were positively related to measures of reactive and pro- eral scholars have recently begun to examine different active instrumental aggression (all correlations were types and contexts of prosocial behavior as well as the controlled for level of sociability). A situational measure, unique correlates of these measures (Boxer et al., 2004; prosocial helping at the request of others, was generally Eberly and Montemayor, 1998; Hawley, 2003a, 2003b; not related to any measures of aggression. Iannotti, 1985; Persson, 2005). Consequently, a critical Carlo and his colleagues (Carlo et al., 2003; Carlo and examination of different types of prosocial behavior in Randall, 2001, 2002) have identifi ed six types of proso- conjunction with aggression might help to elucidate the cial behaviors based on either the situation or personal relations between these two behaviors. motives: altruism (selfl ess helping, usually motivated by A second reason to suggest that prosocial behavior and sympathy), public (helping in front of an audience, usu- aggression might be interrelated is that both constructs ally motivated by wanting to gain approval, respect from are theoretically and empirically linked to empathy (i.e. others, and self-worth), compliant (helping because it understanding another’s emotions and perspective) and has been requested by another), emotional (helping un- sympathy (i.e., feelings of concern or sorrow towards an- der emotionally evocative circumstances), dire (helping other; Feshbach and Feshbach, 1986; Hill, 2004; Miller in emergency situations), and anonymous (helping with- and Eisenberg, 1988; Strayer and Roberts, 2004). While out the receiver knowing the identity of the helper). empathy and sympathy have been thought to be precur- The importance of identifying these different proso- sors to prosocial behavior, it is also plausible that be- cial behaviors is demonstrated by the fact that each be- ing prosocial can make an individual more attentive and havior is characterized by its unique relations with oth- Two Sides of the Same Coin? Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors 3 er variables. Altruistic prosocial behaviors, for example, that these relations are bidirectional in nature. In other have been found to be signifi cantly and positively re- words, being prosocial might very well increase one’s lated to perspective taking, sympathy, and internalized tendency to be sympathetic. Successfully helping anoth- moral reasoning, whereas public prosocial behaviors er person, for example, can show someone that he or she have been found to be signifi cantly and negatively cor- can be effective in relieving another’s distress. This re- related with these same measures (Carlo et al., 2003). alization might consequently shift focus away from the Aggression has also been found to be negatively related self and instead towards others’ feelings and emotional to altruistic prosocial behaviors and positively related experiences. A shift in focus from the self to others has to prosocial behaviors that benefi t the self (Carlo et al., also been linked to less aggression (Manning and Bear, 2003; Persson, 2005). Compliant helping is another pro- 2002; Mussen and Eisenberg, 2001). Prosocial behav- social behavior that is positively related to perspective ior has been demonstrated in the past to affect cognitive taking, sympathy, and internalized moral reasoning and components associated with aggression. For example, negatively related to aggression, but these relations with Bandura et al. (2001) reported that prosocial behavior in aggression have been less conclusive. This might be be- adolescent girls led to avoiding ruminating about events cause, in contrast to altruism, compliant helping is not which incite anger, and less ruminating in turn was pre- motivated completely by selfl ess motives and instead dictive of engaging in fewer transgressive behaviors. by the demands of the social situation (Eisenberg and Empirical evidence supporting that prosocial be- Miller, 1987). Thus, there might be different motives havior affects sympathy (and perspective taking) stems for compliant prosocial behavior as compared to other from a study conducted by Eisenberg et al. (1999) Us- forms of prosocial behaviors. ing a longitudinal design, it was found that spontaneous sharing behavior observed at preschool age (4–5 years) Sympathy and prosocial behavior was signifi cantly correlated with self-reported sympathy at ages 13–14, 15–16, 17–18, and 19–20, signifi cantly As previously mentioned, mechanisms that can help ex- correlated with self-reported sympathy and friend-re- plicate the relation between prosocial and aggressive be- ported sympathy at ages 21–22, and nearly signifi cantly havior are empathy and sympathy (Feshbach and Fesh- correlated with friend-reported sympathy at ages 23–24. bach, 1986). In its most nascent form, empathy occurs It is notable that no other observed behavior (spontane- when one experiences and understands another’s affec- ous helping, compliant sharing and compliant helping) tive and cognitive state. Once the self becomes differen- had any associations with sympathy. While the criteria tiated from others, empathy can manifest itself through for spontaneous helping included acting without the re- either personal distress or sympathetic distress (hereaf- quest of another, this defi nition of helping involved of- ter referred to as sympathy). Personal distress is char- fering something without any physical “cost” to the acterized by a focus on relieving distress within the self child. Spontaneous sharing, conversely, was defi ned by (egocentric responding) due to the negative emotions giving an item in one’s possession to another due to the one is vicariously experiencing with another. Sympa- child’s own desire to share. Thus, only observations of thy, conversely, arises when one feels a true concern, spontaneous sharing might have tapped into the selfl ess pity, or sorrow for another’s plight. Resulting from this nature of these preschoolers. compassion is a shift in the focus of relieving distress from the self to the victim. It is then sympathy that is Sympathy and aggression the more proximal precursor of altruistic tendencies (Carlo and Randall, 2002; Hill, 2004; Hoffman, 1987). A tendency to feel sorry for someone else’s situation In addition to sympathy, perspective taking has been re- might moreover attenuate the likelihood that one will garded as another key component of prosocial respond- respond to a situation in an aggressive or antisocial ing; the cognitive ability to understand another’s affect, manner. Feshbach and Feshbach (1986) theorize that the thoughts, and even visual perspective has been positive- more affectively sympathetic an observer or instigator ly related to helping behaviors (Eisenberg and Fabes, of an aggressive act is, the more likely this person will 1998). Elaborated role taking has been hypothesized to vicariously experience the painful consequences of this lead to a deeper processing of empathic emotions which aggressive act. This vicarious experience will in turn de- can result in sympathetic or personal distress, depending ter this person from engaging in the same aggressive act on person characteristics (e.g., ability to cope with emo- in the future. In regards to the cognitive components of tions) as well as situational characteristics (e.g. feelings sympathy, the more advanced one is in perspective-tak- towards the other person; Eisenberg et al., 1991). ing, the less likely this person will fi nd him/herself in While many theorists have conceptualized and vali- aggressive confl icts stemming from misunderstandings. dated sympathy to be an important precursor of proso- Many researchers have reported negative relations cial behaviors (Eisenberg and Fabes, 1998; Hoffman, between sympathy and physical, verbal, and indirect 1987; Staub, 1986), it is conceivable to hypothesize aggression and antisocial behavior (Carlo et al., 1998; 4 McGinley & Carlo in Journal of Youth & Adolescence (2006)

Hughes et al., 2000; Kaukianen et al., 1999; Strayer less aggression into young adulthood (Coie and Dodge, and Roberts, 2004). Research on intervention studies 1998; Lahey et al., 2000). The present study also ex- which promote sympathy provides evidence that these plored whether age is signifi cantly related to prosocial techniques are effective in decreasing aggressive be- behavior, sympathy, and physical aggression in a sam- havior. For example, college-aged men, after watching ple of young adults. videotaped testimonials of other men who have com- mitted rape, were more sympathetic and less relation- Hypotheses ally aggressive following the treatment (O’Donohue et al., 2003). Programs using methods to teach children This study had two main goals: to examine how different how to be aware of other people’s feelings have also de- types of prosocial behaviors were related to physical ag- creased aggressive behavior in the home and in school gression, and to examine whether or not the relations be- (Frey et al., 2000; Webster-Stratton and Reid, 2003). tween prosocial behaviors and physical aggression were mediated by sympathy. Based on prior research, altruism Gender and age was expected to be signifi cantly and negatively related to physical aggression, and public prosocial behavior was ex- Gender socialization theorists have noted that, due to pected to be signifi cantly and positively related to physical gender specifi c socialization and experience, gender dif- aggression. Because prior research has been mixed in re- ferences in prosocial and aggressive behaviors consoli- gards to the relation between compliant prosocial behav- date and emerge by adolescence (Maccoby and Jacklin, ior and physical aggression, it is unknown whether or not 1974). Scholars examining prosocial behaviors and ag- these two behaviors will be related. Furthermore, due to gression have also reported many gender differences in lack of prior research, no a priori hypotheses were made on these behaviors. Females tend to engage in more pro- the relations between dire, emotional, and anonymous pro- social behaviors, show more perspective taking and be social behaviors and physical aggression. more empathic, sympathetic, and nurturing than males, Based on theory and empirical research, it was also whereas males have been found to be more physically hypothesized that altruism and compliant, dire, emo- aggressive and engage more risky and instrumental forms tional, and anonymous prosocial behaviors will be sig- of prosocial behaviors (Eagly and Crowley, 1986; Eisen- nifi cantly and positively related to sympathy. In con- berg, 2003; Carlo et al., 1999; Carlo and Randall, 2002; trast, since public prosocial behaviors are focused on Knight et al., 1996; Ostrov and Keating, 2004). Gender benefi ting the self, these behaviors were expected to be differences in aggression may be especially pronounced signifi cantly and negatively related to sympathy. Addi- in emotionally evocative situations (Knight et al., 2002). tionally, sympathy will be signifi cantly and negatively However, it has also been suggested the reasons for these related to physical aggression. differences in aggression may be a result of gender dif- To directly examine the mediating role of sympathy on ferences in empathy, perspective taking and sympathy. the relations between prosocial behaviors and physical Sympathy, for example, has been found to mediate the aggression, structural equation modeling analyses were relationship between gender and aggression (Carlo et al., conducted. Because sympathy and selfi sh motives defi ne 1999). The present study examined gender differences in altruism and public prosocial behaviors, respectively, it six types of prosocial behaviors, sympathy, and physi- was expected that a direct path between both altruism cal aggression in addition to examining whether or not and public prosocial behaviors and physical aggression the proposed model explaining the relation among these would exist. Additionally, an indirect path via sympathy measures would differ for men and women. is also expected to be found. However, due to lack of Cognitive developmental theorists have posited that prior research, mediation analyses for compliant, dire, increases in adolescents’ moral reasoning, prosocial be- emotional, and anonymous prosocial behaviors would haviors, and sympathy can be attributed to growths in be conducted only if these prosocial behaviors were sig- sociocognitive skills such as attentional processes and nifi cantly related to physical aggression and sympathy. perspective taking (Eisenberg and Fabes, 1998). More- Because of theory and prior empirical evidence, gen- over, with age comes increased opportunities to en- der differences were also anticipated. Women were ex- gage in prosocial acts, as well as life experience that pected to engage in more altruism and compliant, dire, may provide one with the tools and abilities to help oth- emotional and anonymous prosocial behaviors and be ers. Scholars have accordingly found positive relations more sympathetic than men, while men were expected among age and prosocial behavior, especially among to be more physically aggressive and engage in more early to middle adolescents (but not into young adult- public prosocial behaviors than women. However, while hood) and there are reported age increases in sympathy gender disparities were expected on these measures, the through young adulthood (Carlo et al., 1992; Eisenberg path models proposed examining these three constructs and Fabes, 1998; Fabes et al., 1999). These sociocog- may be gender-invariant given that sympathy has been nitive advancements may help explain a trend towards found to mediate the relationship between gender and Two Sides of the Same Coin? Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors 5

aggression in previous studies. Finally, based on the pri- two scales were averaged to form an overall sympathy or empirical evidence, older individuals were expect- scale (14 items; Cronbach’s α=.85). ed to report more sympathy than younger individuals; however, given the lack of prior evidence, no a priori Prosocial Tendencies Measure (PTM) hypotheses were made regarding age differences in the Additionally, students completed a 22-item version (one different types of prosocial behaviors and physical ag- item was inadvertently left off) of the PTM, composed gression in young adulthood. of 6 subscales: altruism (4 items, Cronbach’s α =.60), public (4 items, Cronbach’s α =.87), emotional (4 items, Methods Cronbach’s α =.81), dire (3 items, Cronbach’s α =.70), anonymous (5 items, Cronbach’s α =.82) and compliant Participants and procedure (2 items, Cronbach’s α=.81). Participants were asked Participants were 252 college students (68 males, 184 to rate the extent to which statements (sample items: “I females; M age=21.67 years, SD =3.35) who were re- tend to help people who are hurt badly”, “Helping oth- cruited from the subject pool at a Pacifi c-coast state ers when I am in the spotlight is when I work best”) de- university. All were enrolled in Introductory Psycholo- scribed themselves on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 gy courses. A slight majority of the sample was White (does not describe me at all) to 5 (describes me greatly). (37%), while 35% was Asian/Middle Eastern, 18% was Although the reliability of the subscales is moderate to Hispanic, 3% was Black, and 8% was classifi ed as “oth- strong, the PTM has been found to have adequate reli- er.” Mothers’ educational status included elementary/ ability and validity in prior samples (Carlo and Randall, junior high (10%), high school (13%), some college/2- 2002; Carlo et al., 2003; Hardy and Carlo, 2005). year college (30%), 4-year college (25%), and postgrad- uate studies (23%). Fathers’ educational status included Physical aggression elementary/junior high (6.5%), high school (13%), some To assess both trait and state physical aggression, par- college/2 year college (24%), 4-year college (22%), and ticipants completed the Suppression of Aggression sub- postgraduate studies (34%). A survey packet includ- scale of the Weinberger Adjustment Inventory (Wein- ing the Davis Interpersonal Reactivity Index, the Pro- berger, 1991) and three behavioral fi ghting items (one social Tendencies Measure, the Suppression of Aggres- fi ghting item was later dropped due to extremely low sion subscale of the Weinberger Adjustment Inventory, variability). The Suppression of Aggression subscale of and three behavioral fi ghting items was administered by the Weinberger Adjustment Inventory was a fi ve-item the researchers to the participants in a large classroom. scale designed to assess aggressive behaviors (sample The participants took approximately 45 minutes to com- item “If someone tries to hurt me, I make sure I get even plete the survey packet. Upon completion, the partici- with them”) on a fi ve point scale (1 = does not describe pants were given course credit, debriefed, and thanked me well through 5= describe me very well). The behav- for their participation. ioral items included: “During the past year, how many times were you in a physical fi ght in which no weapons Measures were present?” (M = 1.13, SD = .51, ordered catego- ries with a range from 1 to 4) and “During the past year, Sympathy how many times did you provoke a physical fi ght?” (M Students completed the empathic concern and perspec- = 1.11, SD =.43, ordered categories with a range from 1 tive taking subscales from the Interpersonal Reactivi- to 5). Both the Suppression of Aggression scale (Cron- ty Index (Davis, 1983). Both the empathic concern sub- bach’s α = .82) and the two fi ghting items (Cronbach’s α scale (Cronbach’s α=.76; sample item: “I often have = .88) were converted to z-scores and averaged to form tender, concerned feelings for people less fortunate a seven-item index of physical aggression (Cronbach’s than me”) and the perspective taking scale (Cronbach’s α = .79). Weinberger and colleagues have reported ade- α=.78; sample item: “I sometimes fi nd it diffi cult to see quate validity and reliability for the Suppression of Ag- things from the ‘other person’s’ point of view”) con- gression subscale in college samples (Weinberger, 1995; sisted of seven items. Items were rated on a fi ve-point Weinberger and Gomes, 1995). scale ranging from “does not describe me” to “describes me very well.” Adequate reliability and validity for this Results measure has been reported elsewhere (e.g. Davis and Franzoi, 1991; Laible et al., 2000). Because perspective Preliminary confi rmatory factor analyses taking and empathic concern are theoretically and em- A preliminary confi rmatory factor analysis was conduct- pirically related (Davis, 1983; Eisenberg, 1986) and be- ed to examine the psychometric properties of the PTM. cause preliminary analysis indicated that the two scales All factor loadings and path coeffi cients presented are were signifi cantly correlated, r(250) = .53, p < .001, the 6 McGinley & Carlo in Journal of Youth & Adolescence (2006)

standardized values. To determine if model parameters Tests of gender and age differences in social behaviors were statistically signifi cant, signifi cance levels of .05 were used. Prior to all other analyses, a CFA was con- To examine anticipated gender differences, t-tests were ducted in order to determine the factorial validity of the conducted. Results (using a Bonferonni correction) six PTM latent variables. This model fi t well according showed that there were signifi cant gender differences to descriptive fi t indices, χ2 (194, N = 252) = 344.64, p among the variables. Men had a lower mean sympa- < .01, CFI = .93, SRMR = .06 (Hu and Bentler, 1999). thy score than women (men M = 3.44, SD = .46, wom- The factor loadings for all six factors and item names en M = 3.87, SD = .86), t(250 )= −5.44, p < .001. Ad- can be found in Table 1. These loadings, with the ex- ditionally, men had a higher physical aggression score ception of one item that weakly loaded onto the altruism than women (men M = .40, SD =.86, women M = factor (recognition), were large and positive. −.14, SD = .51), t(250) = 6.07, p < .001. Gender dif- ferences were found for three types of prosocial be- Univariate statistics and correlations haviors. Men had a higher mean score than women for public prosocial behaviors (men M = 7.63, SD =3.05, Means, standard deviations, and correlations among sym- women M = 6.25, SD =2.62), t(246) = 3.54, p < .001. pathy, physical aggression, and the six factors of the PTM Men had a lower mean score than women on altruism (created by using factor loadings obtained from the pre- (men M = 8.26, SD = 1.69, female M = 8.91, SD = liminary CFA of these factors) can be found in Table 2. 1.70), t(244) = −2.655, p < .01, and compliant proso- Sympathy was signifi cantly and positively correlated to cial behaviors (men M = 5.53, SD = 1.43, female M = altruism and compliant, emotional, dire, and anonymous 6.33, SD = 1.64), t(249) = −3.529, p < .01. Men tend- prosocial behaviors and signifi cantly and negatively cor- ed to have lower mean scores than women on emotion- related to public prosocial behaviors and physical aggres- al prosocial behaviors (men M = 10.53, SD = 2.35, fe- sion. Physical aggression was signifi cantly and positively male M = 11.51, SD = 2.75), but this difference was correlated to public prosocial behaviors and signifi cant- marginally signifi cant, t(246) = −2.58, p < .05. Zero- ly and negatively correlated to altruism and compliant order correlations were also conducted in order to ex- prosocial behaviors. Altruism was signifi cantly and pos- amine the relations among age and the six types of itively correlated with compliant prosocial behaviors prosocial behavior, sympathy, and physical aggression. and signifi cantly and negatively correlated with the pub- Age was not signifi cantly correlated with any of these lic prosocial behaviors. Compliant, emotional, dire, and social behaviors. anonymous prosocial behaviors were all signifi cantly and positively correlated with one another. Two Sides of the Same Coin? Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors 7

Structural equation modeling analyses Multigroup analyses

Once the six latent variables were established, a mod- Multigroup analyses for gender were performed to ex- el examining the mediation of sympathy between each amine whether or not constraining the three regres- latent prosocial variable that was signifi cantly correlat- sion coeffi cients would yield a signifi cant drop in χ2 in ed with sympathy and physical aggression (altruism, each of the three models (altruism, compliant, public) compliant, public) and the observed physical aggression tested. A signifi cant drop in χ2 from the full to the re- variable was tested by constructing direct paths from (a) duced model would suggest signifi cantly worse fi t for each prosocial behavior to the observed sympathy vari- the reduced model, or the model where the regression able and (b) from the observed sympathy variable to the coeffi cients have been constrained to be equal for men observed physical aggression variable. and women. The multigroup results for altruism mod- The altruism model (see Fig. 1) had adequate fi t, χ2(8, el (Δχ2(3) = 7.60, p = .06), the compliant model (Δχ2(3) N = 252) = 10.55, p = .23, CFI = .99, SRMR = .03. This = 5.85, p = .12), and the public model (Δχ2(3) = 7.11, p model had a signifi cant and positive path from altruism = .07) showed that the χ2 difference test approached, but to sympathy (R 2 = .163) and a signifi cant and negative did not reach signifi cance. Since the regression coeffi - path from sympathy to physical aggression. Additional- cients for all three models were found to be invariant for ly, the direct path from altruism to physical aggression males and females, mediation tests were performed only (R 2 =.272) was signifi cant and negative. The compliant on the full-group models. model (see Fig. 2) had adequate fi t, χ2(1, N = 252) = .04, p = .84, CFI=1.00, SRMR < .01. Similar to the al- Tests of mediation truism model, this model had a signifi cant and positive path from compliant prosocial behavior to sympathy (R2 Standard errors for all signifi cance tests of indirect ef- = .270) and a signifi cant and negative path from sym- fects were computed using the bootstrap procedure im- pathy to physical aggression. However, the direct path plemented in Mplus 3.10 (MacKinnon et al., 2002). Re- between the compliant prosocial behavior and physical sults showed that the 95% confi dence intervals for the aggression (R2 = .202) was nonsignifi cant. Finally, the standard error estimates of the indirect effects fell out- public model (see Fig. 3) also had adequate fi t, χ 2(8, N side of zero for the altruism (−.133, −.039), compliant = 252) = 23.92, p < .05, CFI = .97, SRMR = .03. Un- (−.258, −.108), and public (.032, .143) models. Since like fi rst two models, this model had a signifi cant and each confi dence interval fell outside of zero, sympathy negative path from public prosocial behavior to sympa- was therefore a signifi cant mediator of the relation be- thy (R2 = .112) in addition to a signifi cant and negative tween prosocial behavior and physical aggression for all path from sympathy to physical aggression. The direct three models tested. path from public prosocial behavior to physical aggres- sion (R2 = .215) was signifi cant and positive. 8 McGinley & Carlo in Journal of Youth & Adolescence (2006)

In order to statistically control for social desirability, Discussion a shortened ten-item version of a measure of social de- sirability (Crowne and Marlowe, 1964; Cronbach’s al- This study had two main goals: to examine how differ- pha was .59 after deleting one low loading item) was ent types of prosocial behaviors were related to physical also administered. When social desirability was includ- aggression, and to examine whether or not the relations ed as a covariate in the mediation analyses, the results between prosocial behaviors and physical aggression were virtually identical in all three models except the di- were mediated by sympathy. Altruism and compliant rect path between public prosocial behavior and physi- and public prosocial behaviors were related to physical cal aggression became nonsignifi cant in the public mod- aggression in expected directions; altruism and compli- el. Sympathy remained a signifi cant mediator for all ant prosocial behaviors were negatively related to phys- three models tested. ical aggression, while the public prosocial behaviors Two Sides of the Same Coin? Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors 9

were positively related to physical aggression. As expect- son vicariously feels the pain of another or understands ed, results showed that sympathy was negatively corre- the perspective of a person in need for altruistic helping lated with public prosocial behaviors and positively cor- to occur (Eisenberg and Miller, 1987; Staub, 2005). related with the other fi ve types of prosocial behaviors Contrary to the fi nding concerning altruism, the di- (altruism, compliant, dire, emotional and anonymous). rect relation between the compliant prosocial behavior In turn, sympathy was negatively correlated with physi- and physical aggression did not remain signifi cant once cal aggression. Results from the mediation analyses con- sympathy was included as a mediator. Because compli- ducted showed that sympathy partially mediated the rela- ant prosocial behavior was not theorized to be related to tion between altruism and physical aggression, as well as sympathy (Carlo and Randall, 2002) and that previous between public prosocial behaviors and physical aggres- research has suggested that compliant prosocial behav- sion. Sympathy fully mediated the relation between com- iors are not related to sympathy (Eisenberg et al., 1999), pliant prosocial behaviors and physical aggression. this relation between compliant prosocial behaviors and Consistent with previous research, sympathy was physical aggression warrants further attention. Similar to positively related to altruism and compliant, dire, emo- the present fi nding, Carlo et al. (2003) reported a signif- tional, and anonymous prosocial behaviors and neg- icant and negative relation between compliant prosocial atively related to public prosocial behaviors (Carlo behaviors and aggression in early adolescence. Howev- and Randall, 2002; Eisenberg and Fabes, 1998; Fesh- er, this same relation was nonsignifi cant in middle ado- bach and Feshbach, 1986; Miller and Eisenberg, 1988). lescence. One potential reason for fi nding these different Scholars have noted that sympathy is a primary motive relations between compliant prosocial behavior and ag- for prosocial behavior, especially prosocial behaviors gression over time may be the nature of the relationship that consider the perspective of needy others and are the helper has with the requester. Several researchers linked to strong internalized moral principles (e.g. Hoff- have suggested that the nature of this relationship has an man, 1987). Because altruism and compliant, dire, emo- impact on helping behavior. For example, Staub (1986) tional, and anonymous prosocial behaviors frequently theorized that the more a person engages in helpful be- evoke cues of distress and need, and because strong in- haviors, the more likely that person will see him or her- ternalized moral principles are relevant (particularly for self as prosocial, but only when the requester is not co- altruism) to these types of prosocial behaviors, it was ercive. Eisenberg et al. (1985) reported that there was not surprising that sympathy was related positively to a different rationale for helping when a peer versus an these prosocial behaviors. In contrast, the negative asso- adult requested help in their study of compliant behav- ciation between sympathy and public prosocial behavior iors with preschoolers. Helping peers was dependent on refl ects the notion that public prosocial behavior might whether or not the child liked the peer, whereas helping be primarily motivated by the need to gain the approval adults was justifi ed with the fact that the adult was an of others–a more self-enhancing motivated form of pro- authority fi gure, and the child would be punished if he social behavior (Carlo and Randall, 2002). Moreover, or she did not comply with the request. Finally, a child’s sympathy and physical aggression had a strong nega- willing with a parent appears to be depen- tive relation with each other, giving support to the the- dent on the reciprocal nature of the relationship. Charac- ory that the ability to vicariously experience another’s teristics of reciprocal relationships include whether the suffering reduces the likelihood to engage in aggressive parent is responsive to a child’s needs, whether the child acts (Feshbach and Feshbach, 1986). This fi nding was comes to expect that his or her parent will be respon- consistent with previous empirical fi ndings regarding sive to these needs, and whether both the parent and the sympathy and physical aggression (Carlo et al., 1998; child take pleasure when interacting with one another O’Donohue et al., 2003). (Grusec et al., 2000; Kochanska, 1997; Kochanska and When the six types of prosocial behaviors of the Aksan, 1995; Parpal and Maccoby, 1985). One explana- PTM were examined, only two had signifi cant and neg- tion for the current fi nding might then be that college- ative relations with physical aggression: altruism and aged young adults increasingly help out with requests as compliant prosocial behaviors. It was expected that al- their obligations to mentors, peers, and their own fami- truism would have a negative relation with physical ag- lies become more important to them. gression due to the notion that altruism is characterized Mediation analyses also revealed that, consistent by strong, selfl essly-oriented motives as well as previ- with the correlational fi ndings, public prosocial behav- ous empirical research. However, while sympathy me- iors were negatively related with sympathy, and sympa- diated this relationship, altruism still had a direct nega- thy in turn was negatively related to physical aggression. tive relation with aggression. This fi nding is consistent However, inspection of the direct relations between pub- with the notion that altruistic acts are motivated by a lic prosocial behaviors and physical aggression revealed prosocial personality or internalized values in addition that the two behaviors had a signifi cant and positive re- to sympathy. Thus, it is not always necessary that a per- lation after taking the mediating effects of sympathy 10 McGinley & Carlo in Journal of Youth & Adolescence (2006) into account. This fi nding is consistent with the hypothe- ture research should keep in mind that encouraging the sis and the empirical work of Persson (2005) who found development of sympathy alone might not be enough to that selfi sh prosocial acts were positively related to con- effectively increase the number of prosocial behaviors current and future measures of hostile aggressive behav- someone engages in, depending on the type of prosocial ior. However, given the contemporaneous nature of this behavior being measured. study, it is not known whether (or how) helping publicly As stated earlier, strong interpretation of the results contributes to the development of physically aggressive from the path analysis is not warranted, especially be- behavior in addition to its negative relation with sympa- cause the measures from this sample were collected thy, or whether this type of prosocial behavior is instead concurrently. Because sympathy is a well-established the by-product of socialization that also promotes physi- precursor to prosocial behaviors according to many cally aggressive behavior. theorists (Eisenberg and Fabes, 1998; Hoffman, 1987; Consistent with previous research, dire, emotional, Staub, 1986), it is plausible that an alternative model us- and anonymous prosocial behaviors had no signifi cant ing sympathy as a predictor of prosocial behavior, which relations with physical aggression (Carlo et al., 2003). in turn predicts physical aggression, would also ex- Consequently, no mediation tests were conducted. It is plain the data. However, given that an alternative mod- possible that engaging in these behaviors is not related to el would fi t the data just as well, and that both models physical aggression due to the unique nature of these be- are supported by theory and empirical research, there is haviors. A person who helps under emotionally evocative no way to discern the specifi c direction of causality. Un- circumstances or in emergency situations is not neces- derstanding which model explains the data best would sarily a person who is nonaggressive; the cues of distress require future research utilizing longitudinal measures and need are clear and strong, consequently overriding of sympathy, prosocial behavior, and aggression. Oth- individual differences in prosocial behaviors. This expla- er research (Persson, 2005) suggests that such an inves- nation is consistent with Snyder and Ickes’ (1985) con- tigation would be worthwhile. Persson found that (af- tention that “strong” situation contexts pull for specifi c ter controlling for level of sociability) altruistic acts in behaviors; thus, attenuating individual differences. With the fi rst year of observing preschoolers were related to regards to anonymous helping, this behavior might not aggression in years two and three, and altruistic acts in be related to physical aggression because, unlike physi- year two were related to aggression in year three. Self- cal aggression, this type of helping does not directly in- ish helpful acts at year one were also related to year two volve interacting with another person. aggression. However, only concurrent measures of com- As expected, gender differences were found in sym- pliant helpful acts and aggression at year three yielded pathy, prosocial behaviors, and physical aggression. a signifi cant correlation, again suggesting that this con- Consistent with prior research, women were more sym- struct warrants further attention. pathetic and engaged in more altruistic, compliant, and While the focus of this paper was on distinguishing emotional prosocial behaviors, whereas men were more unique types of prosocial behavior from one another, physically aggressive and engaged in more public pro- only one type of aggression was studied: physical ag- social behaviors (Eagly and Crowley, 1986; Eisen- gression. How these prosocial behaviors would differen- berg, 2003; Carlo et al., 1999; Carlo and Randall, 2002; tially relate to verbal or relational aggression is a ques- Knight et al., 1996; Ostrov and Keating, 2004). How- tion yet to be addressed by current research. Persson ever, group analyses did not indicate signifi cant differ- (2005) did distinguish between three types of aggressive ences in the model paths for men and women. Because behavior in her observation of preschoolers: reactive ag- it has been suggested that differences in aggression gression, proactive instrumental aggression, and pro- might be a result of gender differences in sympathy, active hostile aggression. Each type of aggression was and that sympathy has been found to mediate the rela- unique in terms of its correlates. For example, proactive tion between gender and aggression (Carlo et al., 1999), instrumental aggression was generally not related to al- this is not a surprising fi nding. Following this notion, it truism, whereas reactive and proactive hostile aggres- might be the case that the gender differences found in sion had signifi cant correlations with altruism. Thus, a sympathy also explain the differences found between more complete picture of how prosocial behavior and males and females on the public, altruistic, compliant, aggression relate must include different measures of ag- and emotional PTM subscales. It is interesting that other gression. Another concern is the reliance on self report gender differences were not found on the dire and anon- measures of the constructs, making the fi ndings prone ymous PTM subscales; perhaps other variables are just to shared method variance. It would be desirable to rep- as important as, or are more important than sympathy licate present fi ndings with multiple methods and/or re- when explaining why someone helps in emergency sit- porters. Finally, although the sample used in this study uations or helps without the knowledge of others. Fu- was heterogeneous in terms of ethnicity, it was relative- Two Sides of the Same Coin? Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors 11 ly limited in level of education, age, gender, and fam- hart AC, Stipek DJ (eds) Constructive & Destructive Behav- ily background. Future studies should focus on exam- ior: Implications for family, school, & society (pp. 187–203). ining a more diverse population in order to more fully American Psychological Association, Washington, DC understand the relation between aggression and proso- Carlo G, Eisenberg N, Knight GP (1992) An objective measure of adolescents’ prosocial moral reasoning. J Res Adolesc cial behavior. 2:331–349 While the present study comes with several caveats, Carlo G, Hausmann A, Christiansen S, Randall BA (2003) Socio- the fi ndings suggest that the relation between prosocial cognitive and behavioral correlates of a measure of prosocial behavior and physical aggression is dependent on the tendencies for adolescents. J Early Adolesc 23:107–134 specifi c form of prosocial behavior. Consequently, it is Carlo G, Raffaelli M, Laible DJ, Meyer KA (1999) Why are girls important to consider the potential unique developmen- less physically aggressive than boys? Personality and parent- tal trajectories of different forms of prosocial behav- ing mediators of physical aggression. Sex Roles 40:711–729 iors and the ongoing interplay between prosocial behav- Carlo G, Randall BA (2001) Are all prosocial behaviors equal? iors and physical aggression. For example, the results of A socioecological developmental conception of prosocial this study suggest that program developers need to fo- behavior. In Columbus F (ed) Advances in psychology re- search, vol. III (pp 151–170). Nova Science, Hauppauge, cus on how to promote altruism as its negative relation NY with physical aggression remained strong in light of the Carlo G, Randall BA (2002) The development of a measure of mediating effects of sympathy. Moreover, promoting prosocial behaviors for late adolescents. J Youth Adolesc compliant prosocial behavior might also be effective as 31:31–44 the fi ndings of the present study indicated that helping Carlo G, Roesch SC, Melby J (1998) The multiplicative relations at the request of others does tend to promote sympathy, of parenting and temperament to prosocial and antisocial be- which in turn was related to less physical aggression. At haviors in adolescence. J Early Adolesc 18:266–290 the same time, actively discouraging young adults from Cialdini RB, Schller M, Houlohan D, Arps K, Fultz J, Eaman AL participating in public prosocial behaviors may also pro- (1987) Empathy-based helping: Is it selfl essly or selfi shly motivated? J Pers Soc Psychol 52:749–758 mote more sympathy and less aggression. Finally, given that selfl ess forms of prosocial behavior were negative- Coie J, Dodge KA (1998) Aggression and antisocial behavior. In Damon W (Series ed), Eisenberg N (Vol. ed), Handbook of ly related to physical aggression and that selfi sh forms child psychology, Vol. 3. Social emotional and personality of prosocial behavior were positively related to physi- development (5th ed., pp. 779–862). New York: Wiley cal aggression, the present fi ndings support the notion Coie J, Kupersmidt JB (1983) A behavioral analysis of emerging that not all forms of prosocial behavior are motivated by social status in boys’ groups. Child Dev 54:1400–1416 egoistical drives. Crick NR, Grotpeter JK (1995) Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Dev 66:710–722 Acknowledgements: This research has been sponsored Crowne DP Marlowe D (1964) The approval motive. Wiley, New by grants from the Offi ce of the Research Council, In- York stitute for Ethnic Studies, and a Human Rights and Hu- Davis MH (1983) Measuring individual differences in empathy: man Diversity Grant-In-Aid awarded to Gustavo Carlo Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psy- and Marcela Raffaelli. The authors would like to thank chol 44:113–126 Scott Roesch for his assistance in the data collection and Davis MH, Franzoi SL (1991) Stability and change in adolescent self-consciousness and empathy. J Res Pers 25:70–87 Craig Enders for his input on the data analysis. Dodge KA (1983) Behavioral antecedents of peer social status. Child Dev. 54:1386–1399 Eagly AH, Crowley M (1986) Gender and helping behavior: A References meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychol Bull 100:283–308 Bandura A, Caprara GV, Barbaranelli C, Pastorelli C, Regalia C Eberly MB Montemayor R (1998) Doing good deeds: An exami- (2001) Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms govern- nation of adolescent prosocial behavior in the context of par- ing transgressive behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 80:125–135 ent-adolescent relationships. J Adolesc Res 13:403–432 Batson CD (1998) Altruism and prosocial behavior. In Gilbert DT, Eisenberg N (1986) Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior. Fiske ST, Lindzey G (eds) Handbook of : Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ Vol 2 (4th ed., pp. 282–316). McGraw Hill, New York Eisenberg N (2003) Prosocial behavior, empathy, and sympathy. Batson CD, Ahmad N, Tsang J (2002) Four motives for communi- In Bornstein MH, Davidson L, Keyes CLM, Moore KA (eds) ty involvement. J Soc Issues 58:429–445 Well-being: Positive development across the life course (pp. Boxer P, Tisak MS, Goldstein SE (2004) Is it bad to be good? An 253–265). Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ exploration of aggressive and prosocial behavior subtypes in Eisenberg N, Fabes RA (1998) Prosocial development. In Damon adolescence. J Youth Adolesc 33:91–100 W (Series ed) & Eisenberg N (Vol. ed), Handbook of child Caprara GV, Barbaranelli C, Pastorelli C (2001) Prosocial behav- psychology, Vol. 3. Social emotional and personality devel- ior and aggression in childhood and pre-adolescence. In Bo- opment (5th ed., pp. 701–778). New York: Wiley 12 McGinley & Carlo in Journal of Youth & Adolescence (2006)

Eisenberg N, Guthrie IK, Murphy BC, Shepard SA, Cumberland Hoffman ML (1987) The contribution of empathy to justice and A, Carlo G (1999) Consistency and development of prosocial moral judgment. In Eisenberg N, Strayer J (eds) Empa- dispositions: A longitudinal study. Child Dev 70:1360–1372 thy and its development (pp. 47–80). Cambridge Universi- Eisenberg N, Lundy T, Shell R, Roth K (1985) Children’s justifi - ty Press, New York cations for their adult and peer-directed compliant (prosocial Hu L, Bentler PM (1999) Cutoff criteria for fi t indices in covariance and nonprosocial) behaviors. Deve Psychol 21:325–331 structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alterna- Eisenberg N, Miller PA (1987) The relation of empathy to proso- tives. Struct Equ Model An Interdisciplinary Journal 6:1–55 cial and related behaviors. Psycholo Bull 101:91–119 Hughes C, White A, Sharpen J, Dunn J (2000) Antisocial, angry, Eisenberg N, Shea CL, Carlo G, Knight GP (1991) Empathy-re- and unsympathetic: “Hard-to-manage” preschoolers’ peer lated responding and cognition: A “Chicken and the Egg” problems and possible cognitive infl uences. J Child Psychol dilemma. In Kurtines WM, Gewirtz JL (eds) Handbook of Psychiatr 41:169–179 Moral Behavior and Development (pp. 63–88). Erlbaum, Iannotti RJ (1985) Naturalistic and structured assessments of pro- Hillsdale, NJ social behavior in preschool children: The infl uence of em- Eron LD, Huesmann LR (1984) The relation of prosocial behav- pathy and perspective taking. Dev Psycholo 21:46–55 ior to the development of aggression and psychopathology. Kaukianen A, Björkqvist K, Lagerspetz K, Österman K, Salmival- Aggressive Behav 10:201–211 li C, Rothberg S, et al. (1999) The relationships between so- Fabes RA, Carlo G, Kupanoff K, Laible D (1999) Early adoles- cial intelligence, empathy, and three types of aggression. Ag- cence and prosocial/moral behavior. 1. The role of individual gressive Behav 25:81–89 processes. J Early Adolesc 198:5–16 Knight GP, Fabes RA, Higgins DA (1996) Concerns about draw- Feshbach S, Feshbach ND (1986) Aggression and altruism: A per- ing causal inferences from meta-analyses: An example in sonality perspective. In Zahn-Waxler C, Cummings EM, Ian- the study of gender differences in aggression. Psycholo Bull notti R (eds) Altruism and aggression: Biological and social 119:410–421 origins (pp. 135–164). Cambridge University Press, Cam- Kochanska G (1997) Mutually responsive orientation between bridge, MA mothers and their young children: Implications for early so- Frey KS, Hirschstein MK, Guzzo BA (2000) Second Step: Pre- cialization. Child Dev 68:94–112 venting aggression by promoting social competence. J Emo- Kochanska G, Aksan N (1995) Mother-child mutually positive af- tional Behav Disord 8:102–112 fect, the quality of child compliance to requests and prohibi- Gill KL, Calkins SD (2003) Do aggressive/destructive toddlers tions, and maternal control as correlates of early internaliza- lack concern for others? Behavioral and physiological indi- tion. Child Development 66:236–254 cators of empathic responding in 2-year-old children. Dev Krueger RF, Hicks BM, McGue M (2001) Altruism and antisocial Psychopathol 15:55–71 behavior: Independent tendencies, unique personality corre- Grusec JE, Goodnow JJ, Cohen L (1996) Household work lates, distinct etiologies. Psycholo Sci 12:397–402 and the development of concern for others. Dev Psychol Lahey BB, Schwab-Stone M, Goodman SH, Waldman ID, Canino 32:999–1007 G, Rathouz PJ, et al. (2000) Age and gender differences in Grusec JE, Goodnow JJ, Kuczynski L (2000) New directions in oppositional behavior and conduct problems: A cross-sec- analyses of parenting contributions to children’s acquisition tional household study of middle childhood and adolescence. of values. Child Dev 71:205–211 J Abnorm Psychol 109:488–503 Haapasalo J, Tremblay RE, Boulerice B, Vitaro F (2000) Rela- Laible DJ, Carlo G, Raffaelli M (2000) The differential relations tive advantages of person- and variable-based approaches of parent and peer attachment to adolescent adjustment. J for predicting problem behaviors from kindergarten assess- Youth Adolesc 29:45–59 ments. J Quant Criminol 16:145–168 Maccoby EE, Jacklin CN (1974) The psychology of sex differenc- Hardy SA (2005) Religiosity and prosocial behaviors in adoles- es. Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA cence: The mediating role of prosocial values. J Moral Edu MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V 34:231–249. (2002) A comparison of methods to test mediation and other Harris JA, Rushton JP, Hampson E, Jackson DN (1996) Salivary intervening variable effects. Psychol Methods 7:83–104 testosterone and self-report aggressive and pro-social per- Manning MA, Bear GG (2002) Are children’s concerns about sonality characteristics in men and women. Aggressive Be- punishment related to their aggression? J Sch Psychol hav 22:321–331 40:523–539 Hastings PD, Zahn-Waxler C, Robinson J, Usher B, Bridges D Miller PA, Eisenberg N (1988) The relation of empathy to ag- (2000) The development of concern for others in children gressive and externalizing/ antisocial behavior. Psychol Bull with behavior problems. Dev Psycholo 36:531–546 103:324–344 Hawley PH (2003a). Prosocial and coercive confi gurations of re- Mussen P, Eisenberg N (2001) Prosocial development in con- source control in early adolescence: A case for the well-adapt- text. In Bohart AC, Stipek DJ (eds) Constructive & destruc- ed machiavellian. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 49:279–309 tive behavior: Implications for family, school, & society (pp. Hawley PH (2003b). Strategies of control, aggression and morali- 103–126). American Psychological Association, Washing- ty in preschoolers: An evolutionary perspective. J Exp Child ton, D.C. Psychol 85:213–235 O’Donohue W, Yeater EA, Fanetti M (2003) Rape prevention with Hill CE (2004) Helping skills: Facilitating exploration, insight, college males: The roles of rape myth acceptance, victim and action (2nd ed.). American Psychological Association, empathy, and outcome expectancies. Journal of Interperson- Washington, DC al Violence 18:513–531 Two Sides of the Same Coin? Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors 13

Ostrov JM, Keating CF (2004) Gender differences in preschool Strayer J, Roberts W (2004) Empathy and observed anger and ag- aggression during free play and structured interactions: An gression in fi ve-year-olds. Soc. Dev. 13:1–13 observational study. Soc Dev 13:255–277 Tremblay RE, Vitaro F, Gagnon C, Piché C, Royer N (1992) A Parpal M, Maccoby EE (1985) Maternal responsiveness and sub- prosocial scale for the Preschool Behaviour Questionnaire: sequent child compliance. Child Dev 56:1326–1334 Concurrent and predictive correlates. Int. J. Behav. Dev. Persson GEB (2005) Developmental perspectives on prosocial 15:227–245 and aggressive motives in preschoolers’ peer interactions. Int Vitaglione GD, Barnett MA (2003) Assessing a new dimension of J Behav Dev 29:80–91 empathy: Empathic anger as a predictor of helping and pun- Pulkkinen L (1984) The inhibition and control of aggression. Ag- ishing desires. Motivation & Emotion 27:301–324 gressive Behav 10:221–225 Vitaro F, Gagnon C, Tremblay RE (1990) Predicting stable peer Pulkkinen L, Tremblay RE (1992) Patterns of boys’ social adjust- rejection from kindergarten to grade one. J Clin Child Psy- ment in two cultures and at different ages: A longitudinal chol 19:257–264 perspective. Int J Behav Dev. 15:527–553 Webster-Stratton C, Reid MJ (2003) Treating conduct problems Radke-Yarrow M, Zahn-Waxler C, Barrett D, Darby J, King R, and strengthening social and emotional competence in young Pickett M, Smith J (1976) Dimensions and correlates of pro- children: The Dina Dinosaur treatment program. J Emotional social behavior in young behavior. Child Dev. 47:118–125 Behav Disord. 11:130–143 Snyder M, Ickes W (1985) Personality and social behavior. In Weinberger DA (1995) The construct validity of the repressive Lindzey G, Aronson E (eds) Handbook of social psycholo- coping style. In Singer JL (ed) Repression and dissociation: gy: vol. 2 (3rd ed., pp. 883–947). Random House, New York Implications for personality theory, psychopathology, and health, pp 337–386. University of Chicago Press, Chicago Staub E (1986) A conception of the determinants and develop- ment of altruism and aggression: Motives, the self, and the Weinberger DA, Gomes ME (1995) Changes in daily mood and environment. In Zahn-Waxler C, Cummings EM, Iannotti R self-restraint among undercontrolled preadolescents: A time- (eds) Altruism and aggression: Biological and social origins. series analysis of “acting out”. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 135–164 Psychiatr 34:1473–1482 Staub E (2005) The roots of goodness: The fulfi llment of basic Zahn-Waxler C, Cummings EM, Iannotti R (1986) Introduction: human needs and the development of caring, helping in non- Altruism and aggression: Problems and progress in research. aggression, inclusive caring, moral courage, active bystand- In Zahn-Waxler C, Cummings EM, Iannotti R (eds) Altruism ership, and altruism born of suffering. In Carlo G, Edwards and aggression: Biological and social origins, pp 1–15. Cam- CP (eds) 51st Annual Symposium on Motivation: Moral de- bridge University Press, Cambridge, MA velopment across the lifespan. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, NE 14 McGinley & Carlo in Journal of Youth & Adolescence (2006) Two Sides of the Same Coin? Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors 15 16 McGinley & Carlo in Journal of Youth & Adolescence (2006) Two Sides of the Same Coin? Prosocial and Physically Aggressive Behaviors 17