Reconsidering Evidence for the Suppression Model of the Octave Illusion

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Reconsidering Evidence for the Suppression Model of the Octave Illusion Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (4), 642-666 Reconsidering evidence for the suppression model of the octave illusion CHRISTOPHER D. CHAMBERS and JASON B. MATTINGLEY University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia and SIMON A. MOSS Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia The octave illusion is elicited by a sequence of tones presented to each ear that continuously alter- nate in frequency by one octave, but with high and low frequencies always in different ears. The per- cept for most listeners is a high pitch in one ear, alternating with a low pitch in the other ear. The in- fluential suppression model of the illusion proposed by Deutsch and Roll (1976) carries three postulates: first, that listeners perceive only the pitch of the tones presented to their dominant ear; sec- ond, that this pitch is heard in whichever ear received the higher frequency tone; and third, that this ap- parent dissociation between what and where mechanisms arises from sequential interactions between the tones. In the present article, we reappraise evidence for the suppression model and demonstrate (1) the incompatibility of the theory with the existing literature on pitch perception, sound localization, and ear dominance and (2) methodological limitations in studies that have claimed to provide support for the suppression model. We conclude by proposing an alternative theory of the octave illusion that is based on established principles of fusion, rather than suppression, between ears. The octave illusion is a compelling perceptual phe- high-frequency dominance for sound localization, com- nomenon that arises when each ear is presented with an bined with ear dominance for pitch in which the domi- alternating sequence of tones separated by one octave, nant ear “exercises a steady suppression on the other but with the high- and the low-frequency tones in differ- [ear], so that only the frequencies arriving at one ear are ent ears (Figure 1). The majority of listeners are unable to heard” (p. 24). According to this theory, the interplay of accurately describe these stimuli and, instead, report a ear dominance for pitch and high-frequency dominance high pitch in one ear alternating with a low pitch in the for localization results in conflict when the high-frequency opposite ear. Deutsch (1974) noted that, when presented tone is presented to the nondominant ear. Under this con- with these stimuli via headphones, most listeners heard dition, Deutsch and Roll suggested that listeners perceive the higher pitch toward the right and the lower pitch to- the pitch presented to their dominant ear but localize this ward the left. This trend was particularly salient for right- percept in the opposite ear. The authors thus concluded handed listeners, but not for left-handed listeners. Deutsch that the octave illusion reveals separate neural mecha- (1974) suggested that this handedness effect indexed the nisms governing the what and where of auditory percep- lateralization of the higher pitch “toward the side produc- tion, which may be placed in conflict under appropriate ing the most effective input to the dominant hemisphere” conditions. Deutsch (1978, 1980a, 1988) later included a (p. 308). third tenet to the suppression model, suggesting that this The unique and influential suppression model of this unique division of object- and location-based auditory phenomenon was later proposed by Deutsch and Roll mechanisms was facilitated by sequential interactions (1976). From an analysis of subjective reports, the au- between the tones—specifically, the repeated alternation thors suggested that the octave illusion arises from a of the same frequencies between the ears. A recent study by Chambers, Mattingley, and Moss (2002) has challenged the validity of the suppression model. From a psychophysical investigation, their results Parts of this research were presented at the 28th Annual Australian Experimental Psychology Conference, April 2001. We thank Pierre Di- suggest instead that (1) the pitch variation during the oc- venyi and three anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an ear- tave illusion may arise from established mechanisms of lier draft of this manuscript. We are also grateful to Dexter Irvine, Mel harmonic fusion, (2) the high-frequency dominance for Brown, Bruno Repp, Bob Carlyon, and Chris Darwin for valuable dis- localization proposed by the suppression model regularly cussions. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to C. D. Chambers, Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of fails to emerge, and (3) both the pitch differences be- 1 Psychology, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC 3010, Australia tween alternate dichotic octaves and the apparent later- (e-mail: [email protected]). alization of single dichotic octaves can occur indepen- Copyright 2004 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 642 OCTAVE ILLUSION REVIEW 643 of pitch. Section 2 shows that the suppression model is inconsistent with explanations of similar binaural phe- nomena that involve illusions of auditory localization. Sec- tion 3 demonstrates that the suppression model is incon- sistent with a related body of literature on ear dominance. Section 4 reviews three landmark investigations that ap- pear to provide evidence for suppression and sequential in- teractions but that relied on subjective report data and may, therefore, have been compromised by response bias. In section 5, we consider the contribution to this debate by two recent electrophysiological studies. Finally, in section 6, we propose an alternative theory of the octave illusion that reconciles the phenomenon with evidence for pitch perception based on harmonic fusion. 1. The Suppression Model Is Inconsistent With Theories of Pitch Perception The suppression model proposed by Deutsch and Roll (1976) suggests that during the octave illusion, listeners perceive only the frequencies presented to their dominant ear. Therefore, if the illusion is elicited with alternating se- quences of 400- and 800-Hz tones, the perceived pitch is predicted to alternate between 400 and 800 Hz. This tenet contrasts with theories of pitch perception, which predict that a dichotic complex of 400 and 800 Hz should either be perceptually segregated into two distinct pitches or be fused into a single percept with a pitch corresponding to the overall repetition rate of the complex (known as the fundamental frequency, F0). In the present section, this discrepancy will be explored through a brief review of pitch theories—specifically, those theories that deal with the pitch perception of complex tones. One of the oldest and most robust observations in psy- Figure 1. The octave illusion arises from an alternating se- choacoustics is that the pitch of harmonic complex tones quence of dichotic octaves presented via headphones (A). Most approximates F0, even when spectral energy at F0 is ab- listeners perceive these stimuli as a single tone shifting between a sent or masked, and even when harmonics are presented high pitch in one ear and a low pitch in the other ear (B). The suppression model (C) suggests that listeners perceive the pitch to different ears (Arehart & Burns, 1999; Demany & presented to their dominant ear (italicized) but localize this pitch Semal, 1988; Hermann, 1890; Houstma & Fleuren, 1991; in the ear receiving the higher frequency tone (white border). The Houstma & Goldstein, 1972; Licklider, 1954; Patterson, predicted percept according to suppression (D) thus comprises 1969; Seebeck, 1841; Small, 1955; Small & Campbell, an octave pitch alternation between 400 and 800 Hz. 1961; Thurlow & Small, 1955). This phenomenon is often referred to as virtual pitch, because the auditory system synthesizes a pitch sensation that is not physically pres- dently of sequential interactions. This interpretation con- ent in the stimulus. The perception of virtual pitch is trasts with the theoretical explanation of the illusion that thought to arise either from analysis of periodicity (e.g., has dominated opinion for the past quarter century. In Schouten, 1938, 1940a, 1940b, 1940c) or through a the present article, we consider this conflict from a the- pattern-matching mechanism that assigns pitch to the oretical standpoint and review evidence for the three most likely F0 (e.g., Terhardt, 1974). tenets of the suppression model. The purpose of this A distinction may also be drawn between virtual pitch analysis is to point out several inconsistencies between and spectral pitch. Spectral pitch arises when listeners per- the suppression model and previous research, in addition ceptually segregate a complex tone into distinct pitches, to identifying methodological limitations in past studies each relating closely to the frequency of the resolved sinu- that have been used to support a suppression hypothesis. soid (Terhardt, 1974). Thus, for a dichotic octave of 400 Section 1 of the present article considers the discrep- and 800 Hz, listeners should perceive either a single virtual ancy between the suppression model and evidence demon- pitch at F0 (400 Hz) or separate spectral pitches at 400 and strating fusion, rather than suppression, in the perception 800 Hz. Terhardt’s (1974) influential pattern-matching the- 644 CHAMBERS, MATTINGLEY, AND MOSS ory of pitch perception suggests that spectral and virtual The performance of listeners in a discrimination task pitches compete during pitch extraction, with the final has provided further evidence of harmonic fusion during percept determined by the listener’s
Recommended publications
  • Virtual Pitch and Pitch Shifts in Church Bells
    Open Journal of Acoustics, 2017, 7, 52-68 http://www.scirp.org/journal/oja ISSN Online: 2162-5794 ISSN Print: 2162-5786 Virtual Pitch and Pitch Shifts in Church Bells William A. Hibbert, Shahram Taherzadeh, David B. Sharp School of Engineering and Innovation, Open University, Milton Keynes, UK How to cite this paper: Hibbert, W.A., Abstract Taherzadeh, S. and Sharp, D.B. (2017) Virtual Pitch and Pitch Shifts in Church It is well established that musical sounds comprising multiple partials with Bells. Open Journal of Acoustics, 7, 52-68. frequencies approximately in the ratio of small integers give rise to a strong https://doi.org/10.4236/oja.2017.73006 sensation of pitch even if the lowest or fundamental partial is missing—the so-called virtual pitch effect. Experiments on thirty test subjects demonstrate Received: August 3, 2017 Accepted: September 5, 2017 that this virtual pitch is shifted significantly by changes in the spacing of the Published: September 8, 2017 constituent partials. The experiments measured pitch by comparison of sounds of similar timbre and were automated so that they could be performed Copyright © 2017 by authors and remotely across the Internet. Analysis of the test sounds used shows that the Scientific Research Publishing Inc. This work is licensed under the Creative pitch shifts are not predicted by Terhardt’s classic model of virtual pitch. The Commons Attribution International test sounds used were modelled on the sounds of church bells, but a further License (CC BY 4.0). experiment on seventeen test subjects showed that changes in partial ampli- http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ tude only had a minor effect on the pitch shifts observed, and that a pitch shift Open Access was still observed when two of the lowest frequency partials were removed, so that the effects reported are of general interest.
    [Show full text]
  • The Perception of Melodic Consonance: an Acoustical And
    The perception of melodic consonance: an acoustical and neurophysiological explanation based on the overtone series Jared E. Anderson University of Pittsburgh Department of Mathematics Pittsburgh, PA, USA Abstract The melodic consonance of a sequence of tones is explained using the overtone series: the overtones form “flow lines” that link the tones melodically; the strength of these flow lines determines the melodic consonance. This hypothesis admits of psychoacoustical and neurophysiological interpretations that fit well with the place theory of pitch perception. The hypothesis is used to create a model for how the auditory system judges melodic consonance, which is used to algorithmically construct melodic sequences of tones. Keywords: auditory cortex, auditory system, algorithmic composition, automated com- position, consonance, dissonance, harmonics, Helmholtz, melodic consonance, melody, musical acoustics, neuroacoustics, neurophysiology, overtones, pitch perception, psy- choacoustics, tonotopy. 1. Introduction Consonance and dissonance are a basic aspect of the perception of tones, commonly de- scribed by words such as ‘pleasant/unpleasant’, ‘smooth/rough’, ‘euphonious/cacophonous’, or ‘stable/unstable’. This is just as for other aspects of the perception of tones: pitch is described by ‘high/low’; timbre by ‘brassy/reedy/percussive/etc.’; loudness by ‘loud/soft’. But consonance is a trickier concept than pitch, timbre, or loudness for three reasons: First, the single term consonance has been used to refer to different perceptions. The usual convention for distinguishing between these is to add an adjective specifying what sort arXiv:q-bio/0403031v1 [q-bio.NC] 22 Mar 2004 is being discussed. But there is not widespread agreement as to which adjectives should be used or exactly which perceptions they are supposed to refer to, because it is difficult to put complex perceptions into unambiguous language.
    [Show full text]
  • The Composition and Performance of Spatial Music
    The Composition and Performance of Spatial Music A dissertation submitted to the University of Dublin for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Enda Bates Trinity College Dublin, August 2009. Department of Music & Department of Electronic and Electrical Engineering Trinity College Dublin Declaration I hereby declare that this thesis has not been submitted as an exercise for a degree at this or any other University and that it is entirely my own work. I agree that the Library may lend or copy this thesis upon request. Signed, ___________________ Enda Bates ii Summary The use of space as a musical parameter is a complex issue which involves a number of different, yet interrelated factors. The technical means of performance, the sonic material, and the overall musical aesthetic must all work in tandem to produce a spatial impression in the listener which is in some way musically significant. Performances of spatial music typically involve a distributed audience and often take place in an acoustically reverberant space. This situation is quite different from the case of a single listener at home, or the composer in the studio. As a result, spatial strategies which are effective in this context may not be perceived correctly when transferred to a performance venue. This thesis examines these complex issues in terms of both the technical means of spatialization, and the compositional approach to the use of space as a musical parameter. Particular attention will be paid to the effectiveness of different spatialization techniques in a performance context, and what this implies for compositional strategies which use space as a musical parameter.
    [Show full text]
  • Major Heading
    THE APPLICATION OF ILLUSIONS AND PSYCHOACOUSTICS TO SMALL LOUDSPEAKER CONFIGURATIONS RONALD M. AARTS Philips Research Europe, HTC 36 (WO 02) Eindhoven, The Netherlands An overview of some auditory illusions is given, two of which will be considered in more detail for the application of small loudspeaker configurations. The requirements for a good sound reproduction system generally conflict with those of consumer products regarding both size and price. A possible solution lies in enhancing listener perception and reproduction of sound by exploiting a combination of psychoacoustics, loudspeaker configurations and digital signal processing. The first example is based on the missing fundamental concept, the second on the combination of frequency mapping and a special driver. INTRODUCTION applications of even smaller size this lower limit can A brief overview of some auditory illusions is given easily be as high as several hundred hertz. The bass which serves merely as a ‘catalogue’, rather than a portion of an audio signal contributes significantly to lengthy discussion. A related topic to auditory illusions the sound ‘impact’, and depending on the bass quality, is the interaction between different sensory modalities, the overall sound quality will shift up or down. e.g. sound and vision, a famous example is the Therefore a good low-frequency reproduction is McGurk effect (‘Hearing lips and seeing voices’) [1]. essential. An auditory-visual overview is given in [2], a more general multisensory product perception in [3], and on ILLUSIONS spatial orientation in [4]. The influence of video quality An illusion is a distortion of a sensory perception, on perceived audio quality is discussed in [5].
    [Show full text]
  • Low Frequency Spatialization in Electro-Acoustic Music and Tel (02) 9528 4362 Performance: Composition Meets Perception Fax (02) 9589 0547 Roger T
    not ROCKET SCIENCE but REAL SCIENCE Pyrotek Noise Control recognises that the effective specification of a product needs reliable data. As such we are working with some of the world’s leading testing and certification organisations to help support our materials with clear unbiased test data giving a specifier the facts about our products’ performance. With an ongoing research and testing budget we have some interesting data to share. To keep up to date with all the additions to testing we are making, simply contact us for a personal visit or review our website www.pyroteknc.com and sign up for our ‘PRODUCT UPDATE’ emails and we will keep you up to date with our developments. you can hear a pin drop www.pyroteknc.com manufacturing quietness testing_april2014.indd 1 19/03/2014 3:02:43 PM Acoustics Australia EDITORIAL COMMITTEE: Vol. 42, No. 2 August 2014 Marion Burgess, From the Guest Editor.....................................................75 Truda King, Tracy Gowen From the President . .77 From the Chief Editor .....................................................78 PAPERS Acoustics Australia All Editorial Matters Special Issue: AUDITORY PERCEPTION (articles, reports, news, book reviews, new products, etc) Native and Non-Native Speech Perception The Editor, Acoustics Australia Daniel Williams and Paola Escudero. .........................................79 c/o Marion Burgess In Thrall to the Vocabulary [email protected] www.acoustics.asn.au Anne Cutler. ............................................................84 Active Listening: Speech Intelligibility in Noisy Environments General Business Simon Carlile . ...........................................................90 (subscriptions, extra copies, back issues, advertising, etc.) Auditory Grammar Mrs Leigh Wallbank Yoshitaka Nakajima, Takayuki Sasaki, Kazuo Ueda and Gerard B. Remijn. 97 P O Box 70 OYSTER BAY NSW 2225 Low Frequency Spatialization in Electro-Acoustic Music and Tel (02) 9528 4362 Performance: Composition Meets Perception Fax (02) 9589 0547 Roger T.
    [Show full text]
  • Music: Broken Symmetry, Geometry, and Complexity Gary W
    Music: Broken Symmetry, Geometry, and Complexity Gary W. Don, Karyn K. Muir, Gordon B. Volk, James S. Walker he relation between mathematics and Melody contains both pitch and rhythm. Is • music has a long and rich history, in- it possible to objectively describe their con- cluding: Pythagorean harmonic theory, nection? fundamentals and overtones, frequency Is it possible to objectively describe the com- • Tand pitch, and mathematical group the- plexity of musical rhythm? ory in musical scores [7, 47, 56, 15]. This article In discussing these and other questions, we shall is part of a special issue on the theme of math- outline the mathematical methods we use and ematics, creativity, and the arts. We shall explore provide some illustrative examples from a wide some of the ways that mathematics can aid in variety of music. creativity and understanding artistic expression The paper is organized as follows. We first sum- in the realm of the musical arts. In particular, we marize the mathematical method of Gabor trans- hope to provide some intriguing new insights on forms (also known as short-time Fourier trans- such questions as: forms, or spectrograms). This summary empha- sizes the use of a discrete Gabor frame to perform Does Louis Armstrong’s voice sound like his • the analysis. The section that follows illustrates trumpet? the value of spectrograms in providing objec- What do Ludwig van Beethoven, Ben- • tive descriptions of musical performance and the ny Goodman, and Jimi Hendrix have in geometric time-frequency structure of recorded common? musical sound. Our examples cover a wide range How does the brain fool us sometimes • of musical genres and interpretation styles, in- when listening to music? And how have cluding: Pavarotti singing an aria by Puccini [17], composers used such illusions? the 1982 Atlanta Symphony Orchestra recording How can mathematics help us create new of Copland’s Appalachian Spring symphony [5], • music? the 1950 Louis Armstrong recording of “La Vie en Rose” [64], the 1970 rock music introduction to Gary W.
    [Show full text]
  • City Research Online
    City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Cross, I. (1989). The cognitive organisation of musical pitch. (Unpublished Doctoral thesis, City University London) This is the accepted version of the paper. This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link: https://openaccess.city.ac.uk/id/eprint/7663/ Link to published version: Copyright: City Research Online aims to make research outputs of City, University of London available to a wider audience. Copyright and Moral Rights remain with the author(s) and/or copyright holders. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to. Reuse: Copies of full items can be used for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge. Provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. City Research Online: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/ [email protected] The Cognitive Organisation of Musical Pitch A dissertation submitted in fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in The City University, London by Ian Cross September1989 ABSTRACT This thesis takes as its initial Premise the idea that the rationales for the forms of pitch organisation employed within tonal music which have been adopted by music theorists have strongly affected those theorists` conceptions of music, and that it is of critical importance to music theory to investigate the potential origination of such rationales within the human sciences.
    [Show full text]
  • The Octave Illusion and Auditory Perceptual Integration
    The Octave Illusion and Auditory Perceptual Integration DIANA DEUTSCH University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California I. Introduction . 1 II. The Octave Illusion . 2 A. The Basic Effect . 2 B. Handedness Correlates . 4 C. Further Complexities: Ears or Auditory Space? . 6 D. Dependence of the Illusion on Sequential Interactions . 6 III. Parametric Studies of Ear Dominance . 7 A. Apparatus . 7 B. Experiment 1 . 7 C. Experiment 2 . 9 D. Experiment 3 . 10 E. Experiment 4 . .11 F. Hypothesized Basis for Ear Dominance . 13 G. Discussion . 13 IV. Parametric Studies of Lateralization by Frequency . .15 A. Experiment 1 . 15 B. Experiment 2. 16 C. Experiment 3 . 16 D. Experiment 4 . 16 E. Discussion . 18 V. The What–Where Connection . 18 Discussion . 19 VI. Conclusion . 19 References . 20 I. INTRODUCTION hemisphere damage to Brodmann’s areas 39 and 40 has been found to produce deficits in visual perceptual clas- A philosophical doctrine stemming from the empiri- sification (Warrington and Taylor, 1973). Further, vari- cists of the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries is that ous studies on human and subhuman species point to objects manifest themselves simply as bundles of attrib- an anatomical separation between the pathways medi- ute values. This doctrine has had a profound influence ating pattern discrimination on the one hand and local- on the thinking of sensory psychologists and neurophys- ization on the other (Ingle et al., 1967-1968). For exam- iologists. For example, it is assumed that when we see an ple, Schneider found that ablation of visual cortex in object, we separately appreciate its color, its shape, its hamsters led to an inability to discriminate visual pat- location in the visual field, and so on.
    [Show full text]
  • The Shepard–Risset Glissando: Music That Moves
    University of Wollongong Research Online Faculty of Social Sciences - Papers Faculty of Social Sciences 2017 The hepS ard–Risset glissando: music that moves you Rebecca Mursic University of Wollongong, [email protected] B Riecke Simon Fraser University Deborah M. Apthorp Australian National University, [email protected] Stephen Palmisano University of Wollongong, [email protected] Publication Details Mursic, R., Riecke, B., Apthorp, D. & Palmisano, S. (2017). The heS pard–Risset glissando: music that moves you. Experimental Brain Research, 235 (10), 3111-3127. Research Online is the open access institutional repository for the University of Wollongong. For further information contact the UOW Library: [email protected] The hepS ard–Risset glissando: music that moves you Abstract Sounds are thought to contribute to the perceptions of self-motion, often via higher-level, cognitive mechanisms. This study examined whether illusory self-motion (i.e. vection) could be induced by auditory metaphorical motion stimulation (without providing any spatialized or low-level sensory information consistent with self-motion). Five different types of auditory stimuli were presented in mono to our 20 blindfolded, stationary participants (via a loud speaker array): (1) an ascending Shepard–Risset glissando; (2) a descending Shepard–Risset glissando; (3) a combined Shepard–Risset glissando; (4) a combined-adjusted (loudness-controlled) Shepard–Risset glissando; and (5) a white-noise control stimulus. We found that auditory vection was consistently induced by all four Shepard–Risset glissandi compared to the white-noise control. This metaphorical auditory vection appeared similar in strength to the vection induced by the visual reference stimulus simulating vertical self-motion.
    [Show full text]
  • (Autumn 2004) Pitch Perception: Place Theory, Temporal Theory, and Beyond
    EE 391 Special Report (Autumn 2004) Pitch Perception: Place Theory, Temporal Theory, and Beyond Advisor: Professor Julius Smith Kyogu Lee Center for Computer Research in Music and Acoustics (CCRMA) Music Department, Stanford University [email protected] Abstract to explain are easily explained by a temporal theory, and vice versa. Hence it would be fairer to call them complementary than competing, which may be proved by later hybrid models. Two competing theories on pitch perception are reviewed with brief history and several significant works that contributed to building such theories. Fundamental concepts behind these 2 Place Theory two theories - place theory and temporal theory - are briefly described, and the further steps their successors took are pre- The place theory has a long history which may hark back sented, followed by possible future directions. to the days of Helmholtz (von Helmholtz 1954) although most ideas can be traced back far beyond him.(de Cheveigne´ 2004). According to his resonance-place theory of hearing, the inner 1 Introduction ear acts like a frequency analyzer, and the stimulus reach- ing our ear is decomposed into many sinusoidal components, Pitch is one of the most important attributes of audio sig- each of which excites different places along the basilar mem- nals such as speech and music. In speech, pitch can greatly brane, where hair cells with distinct characteristic frequencies improve speech intelligibility and thus can be very useful are linked with neurones. He also suggested that the pitch of in speech recognition systems. Sound source separation is a stimulus is related to the pattern of the excitation produced another application where pitch information is critical espe- by the stimulus along the basilar membrane.
    [Show full text]
  • Music Perception 13
    Music Perception 13 LEARNING OBJECTIVES ISLE EXERCISES 13.1 Kurdish Music Example 13.1 Explain how frequency is related to pitch, chroma, and the octave. 13.2 Javanese Gamelan Music Example 13.2 Summarize the basic neuroscience of music, including how training and experience can affect the representation of music in the brain. 13.3 Ancient Greek Music 13.4 35,000-Year-Old Flute 13.3 Discuss how learning and culture affect music perception. distribute 13.5 Is This Music? 13.6 The Octave and or Tone Similarity INTRODUCTION 13.7 Pentatonic Music 13.8 Meter and Beat Wherever you travel, you will find music. It may sound very different from the music you are accustomed to hearing, but you will recognize it instantly as music. In Kurdistan, we 13.9 Bolero Clip find a unique culture of music featuring such instrumentspost, as the tanbur (a fretted string 13.10 Attack and Decay instrument), the qernête (a double-reed wind instrument), and the şimşal (a flutelike 13.11 Examples of Melody instrument) (Figure 13.1). Although most of you may never have heard of these instru- ments and may never have heard Kurdish music before, you would instantly recognize 13.12 Types of Scales them as musical instruments, and you might even like Kurdish music (see ISLE 13.1 for 13.13 Gestalt Principles an example of Kurdish music). Review copy, © Aurora Photos/Alamy 13.14 Gestalt Principle: Proximity: Bach’s Partita No. 3 in E major not 13.15 A Shave and a Haircut 13.16 Cross-Modal Matchings as a Simulation of Synesthesia Do 13.17.
    [Show full text]
  • Notices of the American Mathematical
    ISSN 0002-9920 Notices of the American Mathematical Society AMERICAN MATHEMATICAL SOCIETY Graduate Studies in Mathematics Series The volumes in the GSM series are specifically designed as graduate studies texts, but are also suitable for recommended and/or supplemental course reading. With appeal to both students and professors, these texts make ideal independent study resources. The breadth and depth of the series’ coverage make it an ideal acquisition for all academic libraries that of the American Mathematical Society support mathematics programs. al January 2010 Volume 57, Number 1 Training Manual Optimal Control of Partial on Transport Differential Equations and Fluids Theory, Methods and Applications John C. Neu FROM THE GSM SERIES... Fredi Tro˝ltzsch NEW Graduate Studies Graduate Studies in Mathematics in Mathematics Volume 109 Manifolds and Differential Geometry Volume 112 ocietty American Mathematical Society Jeffrey M. Lee, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, American Mathematical Society TX Volume 107; 2009; 671 pages; Hardcover; ISBN: 978-0-8218- 4815-9; List US$89; AMS members US$71; Order code GSM/107 Differential Algebraic Topology From Stratifolds to Exotic Spheres Mapping Degree Theory Matthias Kreck, Hausdorff Research Institute for Enrique Outerelo and Jesús M. Ruiz, Mathematics, Bonn, Germany Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Spain Volume 110; 2010; approximately 215 pages; Hardcover; A co-publication of the AMS and Real Sociedad Matemática ISBN: 978-0-8218-4898-2; List US$55; AMS members US$44; Española (RSME). Order code GSM/110 Volume 108; 2009; 244 pages; Hardcover; ISBN: 978-0-8218- 4915-6; List US$62; AMS members US$50; Ricci Flow and the Sphere Theorem The Art of Order code GSM/108 Simon Brendle, Stanford University, CA Mathematics Volume 111; 2010; 176 pages; Hardcover; ISBN: 978-0-8218- page 8 Training Manual on Transport 4938-5; List US$47; AMS members US$38; and Fluids Order code GSM/111 John C.
    [Show full text]