<<

The Uralic

PIRKKO SUIHKONEN

Suihkonen, Pirkko (2002). The . Fennia 180: 1–2, pp. 165– 176. Helsinki. ISSN 0015-0010.

This paper deals with the Uralic languages, their regional distribution and re- lationship with one another. The Uralic languages are spoken in a large area in North and Central . Most of the Uralic languages are seriously en- dangered minority languages – only Finnish, Hungarian, and Estonian are prin- cipal national languages spoken in independent countries. Despite being rel- atives, the Uralic languages differ remarkably from one another. In the west, the Uralic languages have had most intensive relationships with Indo-- an languages, and in the east, with . The differences within the group carry information regarding these contacts. In the research of the Uralic languages, the proto-languages and the original home of the peo- ples speaking these languages have attracted particular interest. Comparative and historical methods and archaeology have been important in the research of the history of the Uralic languages.

Pirkko Suihkonen, Department of General , P. O. Box 9, FIN-00014 University of Helsinki, . -mail: [email protected]

Introduction guages) based on the information available from the daughter languages. The so-called Proto-- The Uralic languages, their regional distribution, ic dates back at least 5,000–4,000 years before and relationship with one another are examined contemporary era (B.C.E.) (cf. Koivulehto 1999a). in this . In the first section, the Uralic lan- Excluding Hungarian, Estonian, Finnish, Livo- guages and peoples belonging to the ethnic group nian, and most of the Saami languages, the main identified by the name of the language are pre- areas where the Uralic languages are spoken are sented within the framework of statistical infor- located in (Fig. 1). The North-Samoyedic mation. The section also contains a short overview languages are spoken in Eurasia’s northernmost of the investigations on the origin and the history areas: the Nenets live in the Nenets, Yamalo of the Uralic languages and their contemporary Nenets, and Taymyr Autonomous Areas, the Enets situation. In the second section of the article, the in the Taymyr Autonomous Area, and the Ngana- Uralic languages are described within the frame- sans in the Taymyr Autonomous Area and the work of language typology. In the last section, Krasnoyarsk Territory. The Nenets can also be some focal points in the study of the Uralic lan- found in the -Mansiysk Autonomous Area. guages will be summarized. The Selkup dialects are spoken in the Khanty- Mansiysk Autonomous Area and in the Tomsk Re- gion. In addition to their national areas, speakers The regional distribution of the of Mari, Udmurt, Komi, and especially Mordvin Uralic languages are scattered across Russia. As a consequence of the relocation of peoples during the Soviet peri- The concept Uralic languages is defined on the od in Russia, Estonian is also spoken, e.g., in the basis of their genetic classification, which is car- Krasnoyarsk Territory in Central . The Kare- ried out according to the historical and compara- lian Republic, the , Murmansk and Leningrad tive methods used in . These Regions, and St. Petersburg are the principal are- methods are used to reconstruct a common pro- as where live. The Veps live in the Kare- to-language (the parent of the contemporary lan- lian Republic and in the Vologda and Leningrad 166Pirkko Suihkonen FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002)

Fig. 1. Peoples and groups of people belonging to the Uralic (Rikkinen et al. 1999: 49; Nacionalnyj… 1990; Census of Finland 1999; information on the speakers of the Saami languages is given by Ellen Näkkäläjärvi. Note that linguistic or ethnic data are usually not included in the census information in western European countries. Information on the Hungarian people in the Czech Republic was given by Michaela Kholova (Information Services Unit, CSO), and in Austria, by Ruth Hügelsberger, Statistics Austria, Bundesanstalt Statistik, Österreich). FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002)The Uralic languages 167

Regions, and the Ingrians and the Vods are found The degree of endangerment in the Leningrad Region. The few remaining Livo- nian-speakers live in northern . Most of the The contemporary Uralic languages can be divid- people belonging to Khanty and Mansi ethnic ed into four main groups on the basis of the de- groups live in the Khanty-Mansiysk Autonomous gree of their endangerment. The status of Hungar- Area. Khantys also live in the Tomsk Region and ian, Finnish, and Estonian, which all are princi- the Yamalo-Nenets Autonomous Area. pal national languages in independent countries, The indigenous Saami people live in four coun- is protected by law in these countries. The legal- tries: in , , Finland, and Russia administrative and cultural activities in these (Fig. 1). Many speakers of Finnish, Hungarian, and countries support and maintain the position of the Estonian also live in other European countries as languages. The languages have the status of ma- well as in the United States, Canada, and Austral- jority languages, and their future depends first and ia. This migration of the peoples speaking the foremost on the speakers of these languages. Uralic languages, particularly those languages The position of the second group is less stable. with a large number of speakers, is a consequence The languages such as Udmurt, Mordvin, Mari, of numerous factors. The economic factors have and Komi, which have a relatively high number been among the most crucial ones. Extensive mi- of speakers, belong to this group. There are vari- gration from Finland to the USA, Canada and Aus- ous activities within these groups that support tralia at the turn and the first half of the twentieth their continuing existence. These languages have century, and to Sweden in the 1960s and 1970s, an official status in the national administrative re- are examples of migration caused, to a great ex- gions, and they are taught in school at various lev- tent, by periods of structural transformations and els. They are also taught at the universities, in even depression in Finland’s economy. Employ- which they are languages of instruction at the de- ment opportunities in the fishing industry in north- partments where the education of teachers and ern Norway have been important particularly for researchers of these languages is arranged. These people in northern Finland. languages are used in literature, newspapers, and Economic reasons caused migration also in the in productions of modern art and popular tradi- former : the Trans-Baikal railroad and tion, and research of these languages is active. numerous mines in Siberia were built by people That these languages are minority languages in the who moved to the east and to the north in order administrative regions where they are spoken to build a brave new world. In the Soviet period, causes serious problems. The number of people forced transfers of population concerned particu- belonging to the ethnic groups is usually notice- larly national minorities and ethnic groups. Mi- ably higher than that of the native speakers of gration of to the areas originally popu- these languages. In several ways, the position of lated by minority groups changed permanently Karelian, North Saami, and Nenets falls between the structure of population in these areas. For ex- this second and the third, weaker group. Majori- ample, the Estonian and Finnish settlements in Si- ty languages have a strong position in everyday beria have their roots in this era. life, and, when excluding North Saami, they are, Wars have always caused migration, and a war e.g., the languages used in all higher education. often reorganizes the distribution of people and Majority languages also have the most important languages. Political, economic, and social reasons function in administration, and usually all the in- are also most crucial for a language’s death. For novations are learnt through the majority languag- example, Livonian can nowadays be considered es spoken in the area. In particular, to be a dead language, but at the beginning of of Karelians, , and Komi-Permyaks into the last millennium formed a significant the main ethnic groups (that usually is Russian) minority group in Latvia. Little by little Livonians has been most extensive in the twentieth century assimilated to . Also the fightings that took (Suihkonen 1987; Lallukka 1995). The number of place in Courland during World War II were dis- North Saami speakers is much smaller than that astrous for Livonians. The Livonian fishermen in of the Volgaic and , but the ac- Courland on the coast of the were eco- tivities supporting North Saami are even more vi- nomically less dependent on the Latvian commu- tal than those in the Mari, Mordvin, Udmurt, and nity and thus survived the longest (e.g., Laakso Komi Republics. The fact that the speakers of 1991: 116–118). North Saami live in three countries (Norway, Swe- 168Pirkko Suihkonen FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002) den, and Finland) complicates cooperation in guages, circa 1,000 B.C.E. (Korhonen 1981: 27; economic and cultural life. The same kind of sit- Rédei 2000). According to the most recent stud- uation applies to the Nenets and the Karelians ies, the proto-languages in the Uralic branch have who live in a large area in North Siberia and West a longer history. For example, the break-up of Pro- Russia, respectively. to-Finno-Saami is now dated to circa 2,500 B.C.E. The position of the third group is more difficult. (Sammallahti 1998: 33; cf. Koivulehto 1999b). The group consists of Veps, and the -Ugric lan- The structure of the family tree is the outcome of guages Khanty and Mansi (cf. above the North a comparative research method. The further one Saami, the Karelians, and the Nenets). The goes back in time, the more difficult it is to rec- number of speakers is relatively small, as it is with ognize linguistic variety. regard to the number of people belonging to the An alternative approach is given in the wave ethnic group. In spite of the fact that many of the theory, in which the chronological relationships languages have official position in the national ad- of languages do not have priority. Instead, distri- ministrative regions where they are spoken, and bution of various linguistic properties forms areal that various political and cultural activities sup- isoglosses of these properties that are like waves port these languages, the press of the majority cutting across each other. The relative strength of cultures and languages is strong. The economic the boundaries between the isoglosses depends situation is difficult, and the structure of economic on how they bunch together (see Anttila 1989: life does not support minority nationalities. Some 300–309). The family tree and wave theories are parts of the area are rich with natural resources complementary to one another: they stress differ- (e.g., one of the world’s richest oil fields is locat- ent aspects in the relationships of languages. ed in the Khanty-Mansiysk Autonomous Area), but According to a third influential approach, the integration of the local ways of life with the mod- contact theory, many of the properties of the con- ern industry has proved to be difficult. The small temporary languages are a consequence of con- Saami languages in Finland are located between tacts between languages over the course of their this third group and the fourth group that consists history (Wiik 2000; cf. Itkonen 1966: 9; Anttila of the small, seriously endangered Baltic-Finnic, 2000). Evidence of the contacts between the Ural- Saami and Samoyed languages, some of which ic languages and other languages spoken in the are virtually extinct. Documentation on the lan- same area in various periods of time can be found guages belonging to this group is one of the most in all the Uralic languages. In the west, the Ural- urgent tasks of Uralic linguistics. When a lan- ic languages have had most intensive contacts guage disappears, a huge amount of the man- with the Indo-European languages and, in the kind’s cultural inheritance vanishes. Documenta- east, with Turkic languages. Linguistic palaentol- tion of these languages is also important because ogy, a research method in which archaeology and of the languages themselves, as it is the only way linguistics intersect, has had an important role in to store information on them. dating these connections. Linguistic palaentolo- gy compares the distribution of cultural words Relationships within the group (e.g., the names of plants, seeds, and tools) with the results of archaeological studies and relates The family tree theory is one of the most com- the history of the distribution of cultures to lin- mon ways to describe the relationships between guistic evidence. Linguistic palaentology has also the members of the Uralic language group. Ac- been important in the study of the relationships cording to this theory all the branches in the tree between the Uralic languages. The earliest con- represent daughter languages that have developed nections between the Indo-European and Uralic from one proto-language over the course of the proto-languages are dated to at least 4,000 B.C.E. languages’ history. In the study of the history of According to the theoretical framework of this re- the Uralic languages before the 1990s, Proto- search, Proto-Uralic was spoken in the area be- Uralic was dated to circa 6,000–4,000 B.C.E.; tween the Baltic Sea and the middle course of the Proto-Finno-Ugric 4,000–3,000 B.C.E.; Proto-Fin- River, and Proto-Indo-European in the area no-Permic 3,000–2,000 B.C.E.; Proto-Finno-Vol- between the Dnepr and Volga Rivers to the north gaic (Proto-Finno-Mari) 2,000–1,500 B.C.E.; and of the Caspian Sea and the Black Sea. On the ba- Proto-Finno-Saami (Early Proto-Baltic-Finnic), the sis of the distribution of Indo- and Indo-Ira- proto-language of the Baltic-Finnic and Saami lan- nian loanwords, the area where Proto-Ugric (the FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002)The Uralic languages 169 proto-language of the ) and Pro- the structural typological properties used in typo- to-Samoyed (the proto-language of the Samoyedic logical classification of contemporary Uralic lan- languages) were spoken is presumed to have been guages are presented. located to the east of the (Koivu- lehto 1999a; Parpola 1999; Kallio 1999). Phonology Indo-European loanwords, particularly those from Pre-Baltic and Pre-Germanic language forms Phonologically, there are scarcely any properties dated to 3,300–2,300 B.C.E., are also important common to all the Uralic languages, although in dating the break-up of Proto-Finno-Saami (Koi- there are some common tendencies. One of these vulehto 1999b; Sammallahti 1998). In addition to is the avoidance of initial consonant clusters: for the area in the forest belt between the Baltic Sea example, Latin schola (school) has become kou- and the middle course of the Volga River, the orig- lu in Finnish and iskola in Hungarian. In the con- inal homeland of the is thought to temporary languages, this rule no longer obtains. have been located in several places between the For example, in the southwestern dialect of Finn- Altay- and northern and central ish (see Raento & Husso 2002: CD-Fig. 1), the in- Europe. The most important evidence regarding itial clusters are part of the phonotactic system of the areas where the Uralic languages have been the language. harmony – “a rule whereby spoken is based on archaeological data. Evalua- all the of a given word must belong to one tion of archaeological evidence with regard to of a number of partitions of the overall vowel sys- languages spoken in North and Central Eurasia is tem” (Comrie 1988: 454–455) – is a property that still in progress1 . is thought to be inherited from Proto-Uralic. Some During the last twenty years, multidisciplinary variety of palatal is found in the international study regarding the roots of the peo- Baltic-, except for Estonian and ples speaking the Uralic languages has been pro- Livonian. This is true also of West Mari, Hungari- ductive. In the most recent studies, genetic an- an, and some Khanty dialects. The following ex- thropology has become important. Some of the amples are taken from Finnish and Khanty: in Uralic-speaking peoples have been studied with Finnish, metsä+ssä signifies ‘in a/the forest’ (for- regards to their maternal and paternal lineage, est+Inessive), talo+ssa ‘in a/the house’ (house+ that is, the properties of mitochondrial DNA (ma- Inessive), and in Khanty’s Vakh dialect, sem+äm ternal) and the Y chromosome (paternal). The ma- ‘my eye’ (eye+-suffix-Singular1), olv+ ternal inheritance of Uralic people living in Scan- am ‘my threshold’ (threshold+Possessive-suffix- dinavia can be seen as a European subset, and Singular1) (Comrie 1988: 455). Also the South the Saami inheritance as a sub-branch of that, Samoyedic language Kamas, which became ex- whereas the paternal lineage is linked to some Si- tinct in the twentieth century, had partial vowel berian populations. Among the , this Y-chro- harmony (Künnap 1999: 11). mosome type is very extensive, as it is among the Quantity correlation of vowels and consonants , the Komi, and the . So far, the is another property considered to be typical of the results of genetic study cover only some fragments Uralic languages. The quantity correlation of vow- of the genetic inheritance of people of North and els that has been considered to have its origin in Central Eurasia (see Sajantila & Pääbo 1995; Proto-Uralic, is still represented in Finnish, for Savontaus & Lahermo 1999). Interpretation of the example: tuli ‘fire’ : tuuli ‘wind’. In several lan- results of these studies, as well as those obtained guages, the old quantity correlation has been a in archaeology or genetic anthropology, is one of basis for the development of a new quantity cor- the most challenging tasks in the study of the his- relation (Itkonen 1966: 61–69; on the history of tories of the Uralic languages and peoples. the development of vowels in the Uralic languag- es, Sammallahti 1988). , i.e., quantitative and/or qualitative gradation of con- Typological properties of the Uralic sonants in the inflected word forms, is considered languages to be a consequence of parallel developments in the Uralic languages. Consonant gradation is Typologically, languages are defined on the basis found in the Baltic-Finnic languages (except for of linguistic properties and the relationships be- Veps and Livonian) and in the Saami languages tween these properties. In the following, some of (except for South Saami). Representative examples 170Pirkko Suihkonen FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002) in Finnish are kukka : kuka+n ‘a flower’ : ‘of a boundaries between the elements are clear (see flower’ (flower+Genitive) and pata : pada+n ‘a Comrie 1988: 460). The claim that the Uralic lan- pot’ : ‘of a pot’ (pot+Genitive). In the Baltic-Finnic guages are agglutinative is only partly true, how- languages, the gradation concerns stops, where- ever: for example in Estonian, Finnish, Hungari- as in the Saami languages it concerns almost all an and the Saami languages, the degree of mor- the consonants (Korhonen 1981: 135–153). In the phophonological variation is noticeable. The , consonant gradation is range of the indices of (the number found, e.g., in Nganasan (Helimski 1998: 482). of agglutinative constructions per the number of Word initial stress that is thought to have been junctures in the words) calculated for the above- in Proto-Finno-Ugric, occurs in several Finno-Ug- mentioned languages varies between 0.01 (the ric languages, also in Finnish, but this rule has Saami languages except Inari Saami) and 0.37 numerous exceptions in most of the languages. (Hungarian) (Korhonen 1969: 221). The opposite For instance, in Udmurt, the main stress usually technique to agglutination is called fusion. In fu- falls on the last final syllable, but there are ex- sional languages, the boundaries between gram- ceptions to this rule for lexical or grammatical rea- matical elements in a word form cannot be clear- sons. In Erzya, the main stress in the word varies, ly drawn. The morphological technique of Skolt but it tends to fall on the first syllable (Mosin & Saami is markedly more fusional than most other Bajuvskin 1983: 2). In literary East Mari, the stress Uralic languages (Korhonen 1969). is, in most cases, located on the last strong full In the nominal of the Uralic languag- vowel. In West Mari, the rules of stress placement es, there are some elements that have a common are more complex: in principle, West Mari has origin, but most of the typologically characteris- penultimate stress, but there are several excep- tic properties have developed during the separate tions to the rule (Alhoniemi 1985: 17–18). Also developments of individual Uralic languages. The some Khanty dialects lack word-initial stress number of cases varies between three (Khanty, (Kálmán 1976: 1934; cf. Itkonen 1966: 150–159). northern dialects) and twenty-five (Hungarian). In Suffixation is the most common type of mor- Finnish the number of cases is fifteen, and in phological technique in word formation in the North Saami six. In Proto-Uralic, the number of Uralic languages. Prefixation is a common mor- cases is reconstructed to have been at least six phological method only in Hungarian. The Ural- (Itkonen 1966: 69). In the basic form, the core ic languages are regarded as synthetic, i.e., word case system of the Uralic languages contains forms consist of several elements connected with grammatical cases (nominative, accusative, and each other2. Synthetic languages express gram- genitive) as well as cases expressing information matical relationships with affixes, and for this rea- on locational relations. The nominative form is son word forms can be long. Variation in the syn- usually the stem of the word form. The local case thesis index, developed within the framework of systems usually differentiate between cases for quantitative typology, is remarkable in the Uralic static position, motion towards something, and languages. According to the calculations made on motion away from something. The Baltic-Finnic, the basis of the corpora consisting of one hun- Volgaic and Permic languages, and Hungarian dred words collected from South Saami, North further distinguish internal and external local cas- Saami, Inari Saami, Kildin Saami, Finnish, Erzya, es. This local case system is illustrated with the Mari, Udmurt, and Hungarian, the synthesis in- following examples from Finnish: pöydä+ssä ‘in dex (the number of morphemes per number of a/the table’ (table+Inessive); pöydä+stä ‘from a/ words) lay between 1.81 (Erzya) and 2.22 (Finn- the table’ (table+Elative); pöytä+än ‘to a/the ta- ish) (Korhonen 1969: 221). The indices are aver- ble’ (table+Illative); pöydä+llä ‘on a/the table’ age figures of the elements they are calculated (table+Adessive; pöydä+ltä ‘from a/the table’ from. They are based on the structural informa- (table+Ablative); pöydä+lle ‘on(to) a/the table’ tion on the word form, and they tell nothing about (table+Allative) (e.g., Itkonen 1966: 69–78; Ko- the categories the elements represent. rhonen 1992). In the external local cases, Hungar- The Uralic languages are also considered to be ian makes further an additional three-fold distinc- agglutinative. This means that a grammatical - tion between cases that express location close to, egory is always expressed with the same mor- and connected with, the element the word form de- pheme, and that the morphological elements are notes (‘to’, ‘at’ and ‘(away) from’ and ‘on’, ‘on(to)’ connected to the roots one after another, and the and ‘off’). FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002)The Uralic languages 171

Except for Estonian, all the Uralic languages use The borders between the parts of speech are not suffixes to express . In the Baltic-Finnic, always clear in the Uralic languages. In all of Volgaic and Saami languages, the case ending them, there are many words that can be inflected precedes the possessive suffix, as in Finnish: talo+ both nominally and verbally. A Finnish example ssa+ni ‘in my house’ (house+Inessive+Possessive- is the word stem tuule- ‘wind’, ‘to blow’ (Itkonen suffix-Singular1). In the Ugric languages, the pos- 1966: 80): sessive suffix is located before the case suffix, as in Mansi: kol+uw+n ‘in our house’ (house+ tuuli = Noun+Singular : tuule+n = Lative+Possessive-suffix-Singular1) (Kálmán 1976: Noun+Singular+Genitive 44–46) and in Hungarian: ház+am+ban ‘in my tuule- house’ (house+Possessive-suffix-Singular1+Ines- tuulla = Verb+Infinitive : tuule+e = sive). In the Permic languages and Mari, both of Verb+Present+Singular3 these orders are found. The following examples are from Udmurt: val+ez+tek means ‘without his/ These words belong to the ancient vocabulary her horse’ (horse+Possessive-suffix-Singular3+ of the Uralic languages. The distinction between Caritive), and gurt+jos+a+z ‘in/to his/her villag- nouns and adjectives is weak, as is that between es’ (village++Inessive/Illative+Possessive- adjectives and adverbs. The distinction between suffix-Singular3) (Comrie 1988: 464). All the Ural- nouns and adjectives is differentiated furthest in ic languages have postpositions, and, in some lan- the Saami languages, in which most of the adjec- guages, they can be used as prepositions, as in tives have different forms in the prenominal and Finnish: pöydä+n keske+llä : keske+llä pöytä+ä the predicative position. In addition to adjectives, ‘in the centre of the table’ (table+Genitive also some nouns can take the comparison suffix centre+Allative : centre+Allative table+). as in Finnish: ranta : ranne+mpa+na ‘shore’ : In expressing number, all the Uralic languages ‘closer to the shore’ (shore+Comparative+Essive). distinguish singular and plural, and the Saami, An interesting property typical of all these lan- Ob-Ugric and Samoyedic languages also a dual. guages is a large number of nominal and adver- In some languages, the number distinction of pos- bial forms of verbs. sessive suffixes concerns both possessors and the All the Uralic languages distinguish person and possessed. Particularly the third person possessive number in verbal inflection. The personal endings suffixes have developed to express other grammat- are connected to the verbs after the modal and ical categories. For example in Udmurt, the third temporal suffixes. In the compound tense forms person possessive suffixes have developed nomi- the personal suffixes are often connected with nalizers that are used in defining a delimited, auxiliaries that can also contain information on closed set: meha7nik+jos+my+ly+os+yz ‘those temporal and modal distinctions. In number, the which belong to our mechanics’ (mechanic+ Saami, Ugric and Samoyedic languages distin- Plural+Possessive-suffix-Plural1+Dative+Plural+ guish a dual in addition to a singular and a plu- Nominalizer) (Suihkonen 1990: 297–301, 305– ral. The Uralic languages differ with regard to the 307; cf. Kálmán 1976: 69–70 on Mansi; Nikolae- types of tense and mood expressed with verbal va 1999: 80–84 on Khanty). Hungarian is the only inflection. The basic distinction in expressing Uralic language in which the distinction between tense concerns the opposition between present indefinite and definite noun phrases is systematical- and past, or past and non-past, but the variety ly done with the aid of articles: egy ház ‘a house’, within these categories is remarkable. Grammati- a ház ‘the house’, ez a ház ‘this (the) house’. In cal future is found in Komi, Udmurt, Hungarian, Mordvin, the of noun phrases is ex- and in some Khanty dialects. In Udmurt, the fu- pressed with a separate definite : kudo ture is expressed synthetically with the suffix -o-: ‘a house’, kudo+7s ‘the/that house’ (Mosin & Ba- jual+o+d ‘you will ask’ (ask+Future+Singular2), juskinv 1983: 24). It must also be noted that gram- mis’k+o+dy ‘they will wash’ (wash+Future+Plu- matical gender does not exist in the Uralic lan- ral2) (Perevovsvcikov et al. 1962: 201). In Hungari- guages, although nominal derivation does admit an, the verb lesz (to be) has the future form: distinctions of natural gender. For example in lesz+ek ‘I shall be’, etc. From the other verbs, the Finnish, kirjailija, ‘a writer (male or female)’, and future is formed with the present tense of the aux- kirjailija+tar, ‘a female writer’, can be distin- iliary fog (start) and the infinitive form of the verb guished. that carries the lexical meaning of the verbal 172Pirkko Suihkonen FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002) phrase: vár+ni fog+nak ‘they will wait’ (wait+In- ny ‘to wave’ (non-terminative; -ny = Infinitive). finitive will+Plural3). In practice, the compound Information on aspect is given, e.g., with the def- future is no longer used frequently (Keresztes inite verbal conjugation in the Ugric and Samo- 1974: 49). yedic languages and Mordvin (Hajdú 1988: 16– Some tense markers that have been reconstruct- 17; Mosin & Bajuvskin 1983: 95–105). In the defi- ed to be Proto-Finno-Ugric are represented in the nite verbal conjugation verbal inflection is used verbal morphology of the modern Finno-Ugric to give information on the person and number of languages. Also some elements of expressing the subject and object (or, the agent and the tar- mood (imperative and conjunctive/subjunctive get). The following examples are from Khanty’s suffixes) from the same historical period have their Muzhi dialect: ma++s+em stands for ‘I gave it’ representatives in the contemporary languages. (give+Past+Singular1/Singular3), ma++s+lam ‘I gave All the Uralic languages have the indicative and those two / those’ (give+Past+Singular1-Dual3/ the imperative moods, and all the Finno-Ugric Plural3) (Rédei 1965: 66–67). languages (except for Mansi) have the condition- The variety in expressing diathesis can be com- al. Various types of are expressed pared with that of expressing aspect [diathesis: grammatically in several Finno-Ugric languages. “the sense is that of the role or ‘placing’ of a sub- In Estonian, there is a special mood used in these ject, e.g. as agent in relation to an active verb, or kinds of expressions (indirect mood, Modus ob- as patient or ‘undergoer’ in relation to a passive.” liquus): sa luge+vat signifies ‘it is said that you (Matthews 1997: s. v. diathesis)]. A personal pas- read’ (you read+Indirect-mood+Present) (Laanest sive is found in the Ob-Ugric languages: in 1982: 239, 265). In Udmurt, some tense forms Khanty’s Muzhi dialect, -aj- is the passive suffix, also carry information on evidentiality: bödti+7sko and kit+s+aj+qn means ‘I was sent’ (send+Past+ völ+em ‘it was said that I was finishing’ (finish+ Passive+Singular1), kit+s+aj+mqn ‘we two were Present+Singular1 Auxiliary+Perfect) (Fokos-Fuchs sent’ (send+Past+Passive+Dual1), and kit+s+aj+uw 1954: 154). The indicative, the basic mood, is not ‘we were sent’ (send+Past+Passive+Plural1) (Ré- marked in the Uralic languages. dei 1965: 71). Finnish has the impersonal passive, In the Baltic-Finnic languages, the aspectual in which the personal ending of the passive form distinctions are involved with case marking, e.g., contains information on the agent that is not spec- when the activity is not terminative, the noun is ified: talo rakenne+tt+i+in ‘the house was built’ in the partitive form, as in Finnish: rakenna+n (house build+Passive+Past+Unspecified-agent). talo+a ‘I am building a house’ (build+Singular1 The verbal nominal system (e.g., participles) usu- house+Partitive). When the activity is terminative, ally contains passive and active forms. In Udmurt, the noun is in the genitive form: rakenna+n talo+n the participle -emyn often has also the passive ‘I build a/the house’ (build+Singular1 house+ function: pinal+ez vande+myn… ‘her child was Genitive). In the case ending -n above, which is stabbed…’ (child+Possessive-suffix+Singular3 analysed as genitive in the modern Finnish, the stab+Participle+Past+Passive) (Fokos-Fuchs 1954: old Proto-Uralic *-n and the accu- 160). In the Uralic languages, also some verbal sative case *-m have fallen together. In Hungari- derivational suffixes have passive and reflexive an, imperfective verbs are changed to perfective function – cf. the examples from Finnish: kulke+a ones with perfective prefixes: a barát+om+hoz ‘to go’ (go+Infinitive): kulke+utu+a ‘be carried men+t+em ‘I was going to my friend’ (Definite- (conveyed); drift, be driven (with the wind)’ Article friend+Possessive-Suffix+Singular1+Alla- (go+Reflexive-passive-derivation-suffix+Infinitive); tive go+Past+Singular1); el+men+t+em a barát+ puke+a ‘dress’ (dress+Infinitive): puke+utu+a om+hoz ‘I went to my friend’ (Perfective-prefix+ ‘dress oneself’ (dress+Reflexive-passive-deriva- go+Past+Singular1 Definite-article friend+Posses- tional-suffix+Infinitive). Reflexivity is one of the sive-suffix-Singular1+Allative) (Kerestesz 1974: categories that are typically expressed with ver- 114–115). The progressive aspect is typically ex- bal derivation in the Uralic languages: in Mansi, pressed with verbal nominal forms inflected with the suffix -xat- has the reflexive function: masi ‘to locative cases: in Finnish, tyttö o+n kävele+mä+s- clothe’: mas+xat+i ‘to clothe oneself’ (Kálmán sä ‘a girl is walking’ (girl be+Singular3 walk+ 1976: 56). Also causativity and double causativi- Infinitive3+Inessive). Also verbal derivation is ty in the Uralic languages are expressed by the used in expressing aspectual distinctions as in verbal derivation, as in Finnish: kulke+a ‘to go’ Udmurt: vsonty+ny ‘to wave’ (terminative): vsona+ (go+Infinitive): kulje+tta+a ‘to transport; to carry; FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002)The Uralic languages 173 to take’ (go++Infinitive): kulje+t+utta+a o+n suuri ‘the house is big’ (house be+Singular3 ‘let/make smbd transport, carry, take smthg’ big). In some languages, in the present tense, this (go+Causative+Causative+Infinitive) (-a = Infini- sentence type also occurs without a copular verb, tive suffix). as in Udmurt: arvsin ku7z ‘arvsin is long’ (arvsin long; With some exceptions, the use of the conjugat- arvsin is an old measure of length) (Perevosvcikovv et ed is typical of the Uralic languag- al. 1962: 137; cf. Comrie 1988: 473). Nouns can es, although other kinds of techniques of express- be conjugated in nominal and locative sentences ing negation exist as well. In Finnish, minä e+n in some Samoyedic languages and Mordvin. The tule signifies ‘I do not come’ (I not+Singular1 following examples are from Erzya: od ‘young’, come+Connegative-form), sinä e+t tule ‘you do od+an ‘I am young’ (young+Singular1), od+tado not come’ (you not+Singular2 come+Connegat- ‘you are young’ (young+Plural2); tese ‘here’, tes+ ive-form). In Estonian, negation is not expressed an ‘I am here’ (here+Singular1), tes+at ‘you are with finite negative verb, but information on the here’ (here+Singular2) (Mosin & Bajuvskin 1983: person is given with personal pronouns: ma ei 4). The Uralic languages differ from one another palu ‘I do not ask’ (I not ask); sa ei palu ‘you do with regard to the order of the comparative stand- not ask’ (you not ask). Person and number are dis- ard and adjective, and the use of comparative tinguished in the prohibitive forms (Laanest 1982: conjunctions. In Finnish, the comparative con- 242–271). Prohibition and sentential negation are junction kuin ‘than’ is used: Markus o+n van- given with different lexical elements, e.g., in the he+mpi kuin Matias ‘Markus is older than Ma- Ugric languages, which have different particles in tias’ (Markus be+Singular3 old+Comparative-Suffix expressing prohibition and negation (Mansi: taw than+Comparative-Conjunction Matias=Compar- at kasalaste– ‘s/he did not note it’, tuw ul minen! ative-standard). In Udmurt, instead of the com- ‘do not go there!’) (Kálmán 1976: 67–68). parative conjunction, the comparative standard is marked by the ending: metr arvsin+ The syntactic level le7s ku7z+gem ‘the meter is longer than arvsin’ (me- ter arvsin+Ablative=Comparative-standard long+ The Uralic languages also differ from one anoth- Comparative-Suffix) (Perevosvcikovv et al. 1962: 137). er syntactically. In Proto-Uralic, the order of the Conjunctions in the modern Uralic languages are main syntactic constituents, O(bject) and V(erb, loans or they have developed after the Proto-Ural- predicate), is reconstructed as OV, and in Proto- ic period. In Proto-Uralic, subordination was ex- Finno-Ugric, when taking S(ubject) into account, pressed with non-finite verbal forms (Korhonen SOV (Raun 1988: 568–569). In the modern Ural- 1981: 346). Asyndetic co-ordination is still used, ic languages this order varies. The principal word e.g., in Mansi (cf. Kálmán 1976: 346), and coor- order of neutral statements in Udmurt is SOV, but, dinative conjunction is not obligatory in some for instance, in the Baltic-Finnic languages it is structures, e.g., in Finnish: toinen auto ol+i sini- SVO. Thus, in Udmurt, ta piosmurt val+ez vi+i+z nen, toinen (ol+i) punainen ‘one car was blue, the ‘this man killed the horse’ (This man horse+Ac- other one (was) red’ (one car be+Past+Singular3 cusative kill+Past+Singular3), but in Finnish, minä blue, other-one (be+Past+Singular3) red). Also the ota+n kirja+n ‘I take the book’ (I take+Singular1 instructive case connected with nouns has the book+Genitive). In the Uralic languages, the sub- coordinative function, e.g., in adverbial phrases: ject of the sentence is typically in the nominative kävele+n aamu+i+n illo+i+n ‘I walk every morn- form, and the object of the transitive sentences is ing and evening’ (walk+Singular1 morning+Plu- distinguished with the case marking. The agent of ral+Instructive evening+Plural+Instructive). the passive sentences in Khanty is in the locative In the noun phrases, the determiner, adjective case form. In Finnish, the agent of the passive type attribute, and precede the noun, as in sentences, in which the predicate is formed by the Hungarian: piros alma ‘a red apple’ (red apple); két III infinitive, is in the genitive form: taulu o+n barát ‘two friends’ (two friend+Singular), ez a ház: Peka+n maalaa+ma ‘the picture is painted by ‘this house’ (this Definite-Article house) (Keresztes Pekka’ (picture+Nominative be+Present+Singu- 1974: 145). In the noun phrases consisting of a nu- lar3 Pekka+ Genitive paint+Infinitive-III+Nomi- meral and a noun, the noun is in the singular form native). also when the value of the numeral is more than In basic nominal sentences, several Uralic lan- one, as in Udmurt: vit nunal ‘five days’ (five guages have a . Finnish is one of them: talo day+Singular+Nominative) (cf. the above example 174Pirkko Suihkonen FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002) in Hungarian). In Finnish, the noun is in the parti- said that the differences between individual Uralic tive case: kaksi päivä+ä ‘two days’ (two day+ languages are even more notable than expected. Singular+Partitive). In most of the Uralic languag- An interesting question is how close the contem- es, the determiner and adjective do not agree with porary Uralic languages are to one another typo- the noun, although they do, for instance, in the logically. The Uralic languages are not isolated: Baltic-Finnic languages such as Finnish: tä+ssä over the course of history they have been influ- piene+ssä kaupungi+ssa ‘in this small town’ (this+ enced by numerous other languages and cultures. Inessive small+Inessive town+Inessive). “The Uralic languages belong to a large areal-ty- In linguistic typology, the order of the noun pological linguistic group extending from the At- modifiers and the main syntactic constituents lantic to the Pacific, also including the Turkic, [(S)ubject, V(predicate), (O)bject] has been one of Mongolic and . The members the characteristic parameters in defining language of these four language families are characterised types. Realization of the word order combinations by a number of common structural features” (Kor- within noun phrases and sentences has been honen 1992: 163). Another interesting and impor- linked with the occurrence of adpositions in lan- tant question is how close the Uralic languages guages and used in defining word-order univer- are typologically to the other languages spoken sals (Greenberg 1980; Hawkins 1983: 74, 67, 79, in North and Central Eurasia. 81). The pre-nominal order is typical of SOV lan- The Uralic languages are spoken in a large area guages, which is the reconstructed order of Pro- in Europe and North and Central Asia. Archaeo- to-Uralic. It is claimed that the surface word or- logical studies have provided us with information der of the Uralic languages is relatively free ow- on the material cultures of the area. The rapid ad- ing to their high degree of synthesis. Because of vances in genetic science have produced a great the variety in the word order patterns in the Ural- amount of data on the genetic inheritance of the ic languages it is also claimed that (at least in the peoples living in this area. There still remain nu- Uralic languages spoken in Europe) there is no merous unanswered questions about the history word order model that can be said to be typical of the Uralic languages and peoples. The use of of them all (Vilkuna 1998). Word-order typology multidisciplinary methods in trying to find an- as such is not exhaustive in describing the syn- swers to these questions is not only promising and tactic relationships of languages. In the Uralic lan- challenging, but also very complex, and usually guages, grammatical relations are expressed with the methods used in other fields of science shed dependent morphological elements and, quite of- no direct light on the history of languages. Also, ten, the word order is used to express thematic interactions between the research methods used relations. Typology developed for investigating the in comparative historical studies and multi-disci- organization of syntactic elements on the basis of plinary studies are waiting for new evaluations. discourse-pragmatic reasons can be better used The more information on the history of the Uralic in characterizing the free word order languages, languages and the other languages spoken in the such as Hungarian and Finnish (see Sasse 1995). same area will be available, the better the rela- tionships between these languages can be under- stood. Summary

The relationships of the Uralic languages are de- ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS fined with the aid of genetic classification. On the My sincerest thanks to Raimo Anttila and Bernard basis of historical linguistic studies and by using Comrie for their comments on this article, and David typological information on languages we can fol- Steadman for his comments on the low the processes that take place when a lan- of some parts of the manuscript. The principal part guage is changing. In spite of the fact that the his- of the article was prepared at the Max Planck Insti- tute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Department of torical documents of the Uralic languages are rel- Linguistics, Leipzig. atively young – the oldest documents, from Hun- garian and Karelian, date from the thirteenth cen- tury – many historical properties of the Uralic lan- NOTES guages are known. On the basis of the typologi- 1 On different aspects on the history of the Uralic people cal description of the Uralic languages it can be and Uralic languages, see Itkonen 1966: 22–31. A sum- FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002)The Uralic languages 175 mary of the theories concerning the original homeland (Ur- Janhunen (2000). Reconstructing Proto-Uralic ty- heimat) of the Uralic peoples is given in Rédei (2000). For pology spanning the millennia of linguistic evo- a complete summary of the most recent studies on the con- lution. In Nurk, Anu, Triinu Palo & Tõnu Sei- nections of the Indo-European and Baltic-Finnic languag- lenthal (eds). Congressus nonus internationalis es, see Anttila (2000). Fenno-Ugristarum, 7.–13.8.2000, Tartu. Pars I. 2 Historically, the traditional morphological typological Orationes plenariae & Orationes publicae, 59– classification of languages, based mostly on Edward Sapir’s 76. Tartu. work in the 1920s (see Sapir 1949), has its roots in the Ger- Kallio P (1999). Varhaiset indoeurooppalaiskontak- man language typology (Sprachtypology) from the nine- tit. In Fogelberg P (ed). Pohjan poluilla. Suoma- teenth century. In this classification, isolation, agglutina- tion, and fusion are the main classes of the morphological laisten juuret nykytutkimuksen mukaan. Bidrag technique. These techniques are specified with the proper- till kännedom av natur och folk 153, ties derived from other levels of languages. Moreover, it 237–239. has been considered that the development of the morpho- Kálmán B (1976). Chrestomathia vogulica. Tankönyv- logical technique of languages is cyclic. kiadó, Budapest. Keresztes L (1974). Unkarin kieli. SKS, Helsinki. Koivulehto J (1999a). Varhaiset indoeurooppa- laiskontaktit: aika ja paikka lainasanojen valos- REFERENCES sa. In Fogelberg P (ed). Pohjan poluilla. Suoma- laisten juuret nykytutkimuksen mukaan. Bidrag Alhoniemi A (1985). Marin kielioppi. Apuneuvoja till kännedom av Finlands natur och folk 153, suomalais-ugrilaisten kielten opintoja varten X. 207–236. Suomalais-Ugrilainen Seura, Helsinki. Koivulehto J (1999b). Verba mutuata. Quae vestigia Anttila R (1989). Historical and comparative linguis- antiquissimi cum Germanis aliisque Indo-Euro- tics. Amsterdam Studies in the Theory and Histo- paeis contactus in linguis Fennicis reliquerint (Ed. ry of Linguistic Science, Series IV. 1st ed. 1972. KPh Ruppel). Suomalais-Ugrilaisen Seuran toi- John Benjamins, Amsterdam. mituksia 237. Anttila R (2000). The Indo-European and Baltic- Korhonen M (1969). Die Entwicklung der morphol- Finnic interface: time against the ice. In Renfrew ogischen Methode im Lappischen. Finnisch- C, A McMahon & L Trask (eds.) Time depth in his- ugrische Forschungen XXXVII, 203–362. torical linguistics, 421–528. The McDonald Insti- Korhonen M (1981). Johdatus lapin kielen historiaan. tute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge. Suomalaisen Kirjallisuuden Seuran toimituksia Census of Finland in 1999 (1999). Statistics Finland. 370. Autumn, 2001. history of the Uralic case system. Journal de la Comrie B (1988). General features of the Uralic lan- Société Finno-Ougrienne 82, 163–180. guages. In Sinor D (ed). The Uralic languages. Künnap A (1999). Kamass. Languages of the world, Description, history and foreign influences. Materials 185. Lincom , München. Handbook of Uralic Studies, 451–477. EJ Brill, Laakso J (1991). Itämerensuomalaiset sukukielemme Leiden. ja niiden puhujat. In Laakso J (ed). Uralilaiset Fokos-Fuchs DR (1954). Grammaticalischer Abriss. kansat. Tietoa suomen sukukielistä ja niiden In Wichmann Y (ed). Wotjakischer Chrestomath- puhujista, 49–122. WSOY, Porvoo. ie mit Glossar. Apuneuvoja suomalais-ugrilaisten Laanest A (1982). Einführung in die ostseefinnischen kielten opintoja varten II, 135–166. Suomalais- Sprachen. 1st ed. 1975. Helmut Buske, Hamburg. Ugrilainen Seura, Helsinki. Lallukka S (1995). Assimilation and its measurement: Greenberg JH (1980). Some universals of grammar Finno-Ugrian peoples of Russia. In Leskinen, H, with particular reference to the order of mean- P Pulkkinen, R Raittila & T Seilenthal (eds). Con- ingful elements. In Greenberg JH (ed). Universals ressus octavus internationalis Fenno-Ugristarum, of language, 73–113. 1st ed. 1963. MIT Press, Jyväskylä 10–15.8.1995. Pars I. Orationes plena- Cambridge. riae et conspectus quinquennales, 83–95. Mode- Hajdú P (1988). Die samojedischen Sprachen. In Si- ratores, Jyväskylä. nor D (ed). The Uralic languages. Description, Mosin MV & NS Bajuskinv (1983). Ersämordvan op- history and foreign influences. Handbook of Ural- pikirja. Apuneuvoja suomalais-ugrilaisten kielten ic Studies, 3–40. EJ Brill, Leiden. opintoja varten VIII. Suomalais-Ugrilainen Seura, Hawkins JA (1983). Word order universals. Academ- Helsinki. ic Press, New York. Nacionalnyj sostav naselenija RSFSR po dannym Helimski E (1998). Nganasan. In Abondolo D (ed). vsesojuznoj perepisi naselenija 1989 g. Gos- The Uralic languages, 480–515. Routledge, Lon- udartsvennyj komitet RSFSR po statistike (1990). don. Respublikanskij informacionno-izdatel’skij centr, Itkonen E (1966). Suomalais-ugrilaisen kielen- ja his- Moskva. toriantutkimuksen alalta. Suomalaisen Kirjalli- Nikolaeva I (1999). . Languages of the world, suuden Seura, Tietolipas 20. Materials 305. Lincom Europa, München. 176Pirkko Suihkonen FENNIA 180: 1–2 (2002)

Parpola A (1999). Varhaisten indoeurooppalaiskon- Sammallahti P (1998). Saamic. In Abondolo D (ed.) taktien ajoitus ja paikannus kielellisen ja arkeo- The Uralic languages, 43–95. Routledge, London. logisen aineiston perusteella. In Fogelberg P (ed). Sapir E (1949). Language. An introduction to the Pohjan poluilla. Suomalaisten juuret nykytut- study of speech. 1st ed. 1921. Harcourt, Brace & kimuksen mukaan. Bidrag till kännedom av Fin- World, New York. lands natur och folk 153, 180–206. Sasse HJ (1995). Prominance typology. In Jacobs J, Perevovsvcikov PN, VM Vahrusev,v VI Alatyrev, AA Poz- W Sternefeldt & T Vennemann (eds). Syntax. Ein deeva & IV Tarakanov (eds) (1962). Grammatika internationales Handbuch zeitgenössischer sovremennogo udmurtskogo jazyka. Fonetika i Forschung/An International Handbook of Con- morfologija. Udmurtskoe knivznoe izdatel’stvo, temporary Research 2, 1065–1075. Walter de Izhevsk. Gruyter, Berlin. Raento P & K Husso (2002). Cultural diversity in Fin- Savontaus M-L & P Lahermo (1999). Uralilainen land. Fennia 180, 151–164. muinaisuutemme väestögenetiikan valossa. In Fo- Raun A (1988). Proto-Uralic comparative, historical gelberg P (ed). Pohjan poluilla. Suomalaisten juu- morphosyntax. In Sinor D (ed). The Uralic lan- ret nykytutkimuksen mukaan. Bidrag till känne- guages. Description, history and foreign influenc- dom av Finlands natur och folk 153, 60–63. es. Handbook of Uralic Studies, 555–571. EJ Brill, Suihkonen P (1987). Suomensukuiset kansat väes- Leiden. tötilastojen valossa. Terra 99, 209–232. Rédei K (1965). Northern Ostyak chrestomathy. In- Suihkonen P (1990). Korpustutkimus kielitypologias- diana University Publications, Uralic and Altaic sa sovellettuna udmurttiin (Computer corpus Series 47. analysis in language typology applied to Ud- Rédei K (2000). Urheimat und Grundsprache (Wis- murt). Suomalais-Ugrilaisen Seuran toimituksia senschaftliche Hypothesen und unwissenschaftli- 207. che Fehlgriffe). In Nurk A, T Palo & T Seilenthal Viitso T-R (2000). Finnic affinity. In Nurk A, T Palo & (eds). Congressus nonus internationalis Fenno- T Seilenthal (eds). Congressus nonus internation- Ugristarum, 7.–13.8.2000, Tartu. Pars I. Orationes alis Fenno-Ugristarum, 7.–13.8.2000, Tartu. Pars plenariae & Orationes publicae, 109–126. Tartu. I. Orationes plenariae & Orationes publicae, Rikkinen K, P Maaranen & P Suihkonen (1999). Eräta- 153–178. Tartu. loudesta nyky-yhteiskuntaan. In Westerholm J & Vilkuna M (1998). Word order in European Uralic. P Raento (eds). Suomen kartasto, 48–53. Suomen In Siewierska A (ed). Constituent order in the lan- Maantieteellinen Seura & WSOY, Helsinki. guages of Europe. Empirical approaches to lan- Sajantila A & S Pääbo (1995). Language replacement guage typology. Eurotyp 20: 1, 173–233. Mou- in . Nature Genet 11, 359–360. ton de Grueter, Berlin. Sammallahti P (1988). Historical phonology of the Wiik K (2000). European lingua francas. In Künnap Uralic languages. In Sinor D (ed). The Uralic lan- A (ed). The roots of peoples and languages of guages. Description, history and foreign influenc- Northern Eurasia II & III. Fenno-Ugristica 23, His- es. Handbook of Uralic Studies, 478–554. EJ Brill, torica Fenno-Ugristica, 202–236. Tartu. Leiden.