Atypology of Marked-S Languages

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Atypology of Marked-S Languages View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Institutional Repository of the Freie Universität Berlin A typology of marked-S languages Corinna Handschuh language Studies in Diversity Linguistics, No 1 science press A typology of marked-S languages Case-systems all over the world exhibit striking similarities. In most lan- guages intransitive subjects (S) receives less overt marking than one of the two transitive arguments (agent-like A or patient-like P); the other one of these two arguments is usually encoded by the same form as S. In some languages the amount of overt marking is identical between S, A, and P. But hardly ever does the S argument receive more overt marking than A or P. Yet there are some languages that do not follow this general pattern. This book is about those languages that behave differently, the marked-S languages. Marked-S languages are well-known to be found in East Africa, where they occur in two different language families, Afro-Asiatic and Nilo-Sa- haran. They can also be found in North-Western America and the Pacific region. This book is the first investigation of marked S-languages that treats the phenomenon on a global scale. The study examines the functional distribution of the two main case- forms, the form used for S (S-case) and the case-form of the transitive ar- gument which receives less marking (the zero-case). It offers a very fine- grained perspective considering a wide range of constructions. The con- texts in which the case-marking patterns are investigated include nom- inal, existential and locational predication, subjects in special discourse function (e. g. focused constituents), subjects of passives and dependent clauses, as well as the forms used for addressing someone (vocative form) and for using a noun in isolation (citation form). Apart from the functional distribution of case forms, the formal means of marking are also considered. The main focus is on the synchronic de- scription and comparison of marked-S languages, but historical explana- tions for the unusual case-marking pattern are also discussed. ISBN 978-3-944675-19-0 Corinna Handschuh A typology of marked-S languages Studies in Diversity Linguistics Chief Editor: Martin Haspelmath Consulting Editors: Fernando Zúñiga, Peter Arkadiev, Ruth Singer, Pilar Valen zuela In this series: 1. Handschuh, Corinna. A typology of marked-S languages 2. Rießler, Michael. Adjective attribution 3. Klamer, Marian (ed.). The Alor-Pantar languages: History and typology 4. Berghäll, Liisa. A grammar of Mauwake (Papua New Guinea) A typology of marked-S languages Corinna Handschuh language science press Language Science Press Berlin Language Science Press Habelschwerdter Allee 45 14195 Berlin, Germany langsci-press.org This title can be downloaded at: http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/18 © 2014, Corinna Handschuh Published under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Licence (CC BY 4.0): http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ISBN: 978-3-944675-19-0 Cover and concept of design: Ulrike Harbort Typesetting: Corinna Handschuh Proofreading: Eitan Grossman, Daniel W. Hieber, Aaron Sonnenschein Storage and cataloguing done by FU Berlin Language Science Press has no responsibility for the persistence or accuracy of URLs for external or third-party Internet websites referred to in this publication, and does not guarantee that any content on such websites is, or will remain, ac- curate or appropriate. Information regarding prices, travel timetables and other factual information given in this work are correct at the time of first publication but Language Science Press does not guarantee the accuracy of such information thereafter. Für Tommeck Contents Acknowledgments ix List of abbreviations xi I Preliminaries 1 1 Introduction 3 1.1 Marked-S coding ........................... 3 1.2 Definition .............................. 5 1.3 Markedness in grammar ...................... 8 1.4 Case-labels .............................. 9 1.5 Explaining the existence of marked-S ............... 13 1.5.1 Rare and geographically skewed ............. 13 1.5.2 Historical explanations ................... 14 1.5.3 Functional explanations .................. 17 1.6 Implications for formal approaches to case-marking ....... 20 1.7 Domains of alignment ........................ 24 1.7.1 Beyond case-marking ................... 24 1.7.2 Head-marking ........................ 24 1.7.3 Word order ......................... 25 1.7.4 Behavioral properties .................... 28 1.7.5 The domain of investigation ................ 29 1.8 Outlook ................................ 30 2 Redefining alignment 31 2.1 Introduction ............................. 31 2.2 Split alignment ............................ 32 2.3 Micro-alignment ........................... 33 2.4 Contexts of investigation ...................... 37 Contents 2.5 Incidental splits ........................... 39 2.5.1 Contexts versus other splits ................ 39 2.5.2 Splits based on the semantics of the noun phrase .... 40 2.5.3 Splits based on the semantics of the verb ......... 42 2.5.4 Splits based on morphophonological factors ....... 43 2.6 Usage-based factors ......................... 44 2.6.1 Frequency .......................... 44 2.6.2 Omission of arguments ................... 46 2.6.3 Optional case-marking ................... 47 2.7 Summary ............................... 49 II The contexts studied 51 3 Nominal predication 53 3.1 Introduction ............................. 53 3.2 Case-marking in nominal predication ............... 55 3.3 Identity predication ......................... 57 3.4 Research questions ......................... 58 3.5 North America ............................ 60 3.6 Afro-Asiatic ............................. 64 3.7 Nilo-Saharan ............................. 66 3.8 Pacific ................................ 69 3.9 Summary ............................... 71 4 Existential and locational predication 75 4.1 Introduction ............................. 75 4.2 Linguistic properties ........................ 76 4.3 Research question .......................... 78 4.4 Nilo-Saharan ............................. 80 4.5 Afro-Asiatic ............................. 85 4.6 North America ............................ 88 4.7 Pacific ................................ 92 4.8 Summary ............................... 96 5 Emphatic subjects 99 5.1 Introduction ............................. 99 5.2 Case-marking and discourse structure ............... 100 5.3 Zero-coded emphatic subjects ................... 102 vi Contents 5.4 Overtly coded emphatic subjects .................. 107 5.5 Research questions ......................... 110 5.6 Nilo-Saharan ............................. 113 5.7 Afro-Asiatic ............................. 116 5.8 Pacific ................................ 119 5.9 North America ............................ 126 5.10 Summary ............................... 128 6 Subjects of non-basic clauses 131 6.1 Introduction ............................. 131 6.2 Dependent clauses .......................... 132 6.3 Valency-decreasing operations ................... 136 6.4 Research questions ......................... 139 6.5 North America ............................ 142 6.6 Pacific ................................ 150 6.7 Nilo-Saharan ............................. 155 6.8 Afro-Asiatic ............................. 161 6.9 Summary ............................... 165 7 Non-clause-level case marking 169 7.1 Introduction ............................. 169 7.2 Attributive possessors ........................ 170 7.3 Forms of address ........................... 172 7.4 Citation form ............................. 173 7.5 Research questions ......................... 174 7.6 Nilo-Saharan ............................. 177 7.7 Afro-Asiatic ............................. 182 7.8 Pacific ................................ 186 7.9 North America ............................ 189 7.10 Summary ............................... 194 III Analysis of the data 197 8 Typological comparison of marked-S languages 199 8.1 Introduction ............................. 199 8.2 Making generalizations ....................... 200 8.3 Comparison across roles ...................... 203 8.4 Comparison across languages ................... 209 vii Contents 8.5 Similarity networks ......................... 213 8.6 Geographical patterns ........................ 221 8.7 Summary ............................... 225 9 Conclusion 229 9.1 Summary of the findings ...................... 229 9.2 Generalizations about the functional motivations for marked-S languages ............................... 231 9.3 Concluding remarks on the micro-alignment approach ..... 233 9.4 Consequences for formal theories ................. 234 9.5 Future research ........................... 238 Bibliography 241 viii Acknowledgments This book started out as a PhD dissertation at Universität Leipzig by thesame title, which I submitted in December 2010 and successfully defended in July 2011. The original dissertation has undergone minor revisions, but its contents havein essence remained the same. Many people have contributed to the work in its current state, : to all of whom I would like to express my gratitude. The original research started out as part ofthe project ‘Marked-absolutive and marked-nominative case systems in synchronic and diachronic perspective’ of the Forschergruppe ‘Grammatik und Verarbeitung verbaler Argumente’ in Leipzig. Financial support for my research has been pro- vided by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft through a grant to
Recommended publications
  • Polyvalent Case, Geometric Hierarchies, and Split Ergativity
    [For Jackie Bunting, Sapna Desai, Robert Peachey, Chris Straughn, and Zuzana Tomkova (eds.), (2006) Proceedings of the 42nd annual meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society, Chicago, Ill.] Polyvalent case, geometric hierarchies, and split ergativity JASON MERCHANT University of Chicago Prominence hierarchy effects such as the animacy hierarchy and definiteness hierarchy have been a puzzle for formal treatments of case since they were first described systematically in Silverstein 1976. Recently, these effects have received more sustained attention from generative linguists, who have sought to capture them in treatments grounded in well-understood mechanisms for case assignment cross-linguistically. These efforts have taken two broad directions. In the first, Aissen 1999, 2003 has integrated the effects elegantly into a competition model of grammar using OT formalisms, where iconicity effects emerge from constraint conjunctions between constraints on fixed universal hierarchies (definiteness, animacy, person, grammatical role) and a constraint banning overt morphological expression of case. The second direction grows out of the work of Jelinek and Diesing, and is found most articulated in Jelinek 1993, Jelinek and Carnie 2003, and Carnie 2005. This work takes as its starting point the observation that word order is sometimes correlated with the hierarchies as well, and works backwards from that to conclusions about phrase structure geometries. In this paper, I propose a particular implementation of this latter direction, and explore its consequences for our understanding of the nature of case assignment. If hierarchy effects are due to positional differences in phrase structures, then, I argue, the attested cross-linguistic differences fall most naturally out if the grammars of these languages countenance polyvalent case—that is, assignment of more than one case value to a single nominal phrase.
    [Show full text]
  • Case Typology and Case Theory* 1. Overview of the Issues 2
    December 1997 Case Typology and Case Theory* Yehuda N. Falk Department of English The Hebrew University of Jerusalem [email protected] 1. Overview of the Issues In recent years, as the question of the nature of Case assignment has come into focus, the question of Case typology in general, and ergative Case marking (and agreement) systems in particular, has come to be seen as very important. The interdependence between typology and theory is perhaps nowhere clearer than in the area of Case. On one hand, a typology without a theoretical basis is merely descriptive and cannot lead us to a true understanding of the reasons for whatever typological generalizations are discovered. On the other hand, a (generative) theory of Case, which aims to be a model of the Case component of Universal Grammar and not just a theoretically elegant description of familiar types of Case, has to be able to predict the typological facts; a clear understanding of typology is necessary to be able to do this. One fact that has become clear from typological studies of Case is that a model based only on the familiar nominative-accusative pattern is doomed to be inadequate as an expression of universal Case theory. The problems raised for a theory of Universal Grammar by ergative systems are compounded by the demonstrable syntactic differences between (some) ergative languages and the more familiar nominative-accusative languages. A theory of ergativity must address the question of ergative syntax and its relation to ergative Case and agreement. Here in particular it is important to resist the temptation to shoehorn ergative languages into a nominative- accusative mold.
    [Show full text]
  • Syntax Case Theory
    Syntax Case Theory Modul 04-006-2002 Phonology – Morphology – Syntax Institut für Linguistik Universität Leipzig home.uni-leipzig.de/heck Morphological case Point of departure: Many languages involve morphological case marking on noun phrases (and its dependents, such as determiners and adjectives). How many different morphological cases there are depends on the language. German, for instance, distinguishes nominative, genitive, dative, and accusative (1-a-d). (1) a. Die blaue Elise schlä. the.nom blue.nom Elise.nom sleeps b. Die blaue Elise bedarf der Hilfe (der Ameise). the blue Elise needs the.gen help.gen the.gen ant.gen c. Die Ameise entkommt der blauen Elise. the ant escapes the.dat blue.dat Elise.dat d. Die blaue Elise hat einen Plan. the blue Elise has a.acc plan.acc Structural and abstract case The syntactic dependency of case: (Morphological) case is syntactically dependent. The case on an NP depends on the abstract syntactic structure the NP shows up in. In German, for instance, the object of a verb generally bears accusative, the subject generally bears nominative. Such cases do not depend on the concrete predicate that is involved (2): structural case. Even if a case is not always morphologically visible (cf. no morphological marking on the noun Elise itself in (2-a-c)), it is assumed to be abstractly present. (2) a. Die blaue Elise hat einen Plan. the.nom blue.nom Elise.nom has a.acc plan.acc b. Die blaue Elise hasst die Ameise. the.nom blue.nom Elise.nom hates the.acc ant.acc c. Die Ameise überlisted die blaue Elise.
    [Show full text]
  • Atypology of Marked-S Languages
    A typology of marked-S languages Corinna Handschuh language Studies in Diversity Linguistics, No 1 science press Studies in Diversity Linguistics Chief Editor: Martin Haspelmath Consulting Editors: Fernando Zúñiga, Peter Arkadiev, Ruth Singer, Pilar Valen zuela In this series: 1. Handschuh, Corinna. A typology of marked-S languages 2. Rießler, Michael. Adjective attribution 3. Klamer, Marian (ed.). The Alor-Pantar languages: History and typology 4. Berghäll, Liisa. A grammar of Mauwake (Papua New Guinea) A typology of marked-S languages Corinna Handschuh language science press Language Science Press Berlin Language Science Press Habelschwerdter Allee 45 14195 Berlin, Germany langsci-press.org This title can be downloaded at: http://langsci-press.org/catalog/book/18 © 2014, Corinna Handschuh Published under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Licence (CC BY 4.0): http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ISBN: 978-3-944675-19-0 Cover and concept of design: Ulrike Harbort Typesetting: Corinna Handschuh Proofreading: Eitan Grossman, Daniel W. Hieber, Aaron Sonnenschein Storage and cataloguing done by FU Berlin Language Science Press has no responsibility for the persistence or accuracy of URLs for external or third-party Internet websites referred to in this publication, and does not guarantee that any content on such websites is, or will remain, ac- curate or appropriate. Information regarding prices, travel timetables and other factual information given in this work are correct at the time of first publication but Language Science Press does not guarantee the accuracy of such information thereafter. Für Tommeck Contents Acknowledgments ix List of abbreviations xi I Preliminaries 1 1 Introduction 3 1.1 Marked-S coding ..........................
    [Show full text]
  • Transitive and Intransitive Constructions in Japanese and English: a Psycholinguistic Study
    TRANSITIVE AND INTRANSITIVE CONSTRUCTIONS IN JAPANESE AND ENGLISH: A PSYCHOLINGUISTIC STUDY by Zoe Pei-sui Luk BA, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, 2006 MA, University of Pittsburgh, 2009 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Kenneth P. Dietrich School of Arts and Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy University of Pittsburgh 2012 UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH THE KENNETH P. DIETRICH SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES This dissertation was presented by Zoe Pei-sui Luk It was defended on April 24, 2012 and approved by Alan Juffs, Associate Professor, Department of Linguistics Charles Perfetti, University Professor, Department of Psychology Paul Hopper, Paul Mellon Distinguished Professor of the Humanities, Department of English, Carnegie Mellon University Dissertation Advisor: Yasuhiro Shirai, Professor, Department of Linguistics ii Copyright © by Pei-sui Luk 2012 iii TRANSITIVE AND INTRANSITIVE CONSTRUCTIONS IN JAPANESE AND ENGLISH: A PSYCHOLINGUISTIC STUDY Zoe Pei-sui Luk, PhD University of Pittsburgh, 2012 Transitivity has been extensively researched from a semantic point of view (e.g., Hopper & Thompson, 1980). Although little has been said about a prototypical intransitive construction, it has been suggested that verbs that denote actions with an agent and a patient/theme cannot be intransitive (e.g., Guerssel, 1985). However, it has been observed that some languages, including Japanese, have intransitive verbs for actions that clearly involve an animate agent and a patient/theme, such as ‘arresting’ (e.g., Pardeshi, 2008). This dissertation thus attempts to understand how causality is differentially interpreted from transitive and intransitive constructions, including non-prototypical intransitive verbs, by rating and priming experiments conducted in both English and Japanese.
    [Show full text]
  • Syntactic Ergativity: Analysis • Explore Related Articles • Search Keywords and Identification
    LI02CH09-Deal ARI 5 December 2015 12:3 ANNUAL REVIEWS Further Click here to view this article's online features: • Download figures as PPT slides • Navigate linked references • Download citations Syntactic Ergativity: Analysis • Explore related articles • Search keywords and Identification Amy Rose Deal Department of Linguistics, University of California, Berkeley, California 94720; email: [email protected] Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2016. 2:165–85 Keywords The Annual Review of Linguistics is online at ergativity, A¯ movement, inherent case, dependent case, case discrimination, linguist.annualreviews.org agent focus, wh-agreement, antiagreement, typology, morphology, syntax This article’s doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011415-040642 Abstract Copyright c 2016 by Annual Reviews. Some languages showing morphological ergativity in case and/or agreement All rights reserved also show ergative patterns in core syntactic domains—syntactic ergativity. The most-studied type of syntactic ergativity is a ban on the A¯ movement of ergative subjects; an additional type concerns the distribution of abso- lutives in nonfinite clauses. This article first presents the standard view of syntactic ergativity, which is closely connected to the treatment of ergative as an inherent case. Evidence from Shipibo suggests that a ban on erga- ¯ Annu. Rev. Linguist. 2016.2:165-185. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org tive A extraction does not require inherent ergative. This points to a view of syntactic ergativity centered around morphological case discrimination. Access provided by University of California - Berkeley on 01/25/16. For personal use only. One consequence is that pure head-marking languages cannot feature a true ban on ergative extraction, because ergative morphological case is not in use.
    [Show full text]
  • Sketch of the Grammar of Kubachi the Simple Sentence
    Lund University, Dept. of Linguistics 221 Working Papers 38 (1991), 221-36 Sketch of the Grammar of Kubachi The simple sentence Karina Yamling and Revaz Tchantouria In this paper we present a sketch of Kubachi, a language spoken in the mountainous Dagestan in the Eastern part of the Caucasus. We focus on the major nominal and verbal categories and the structure of the simple sentence. A specific feature of Kubachi is its mixed system of noun class and person marking, which is shown to correlate with a split ergative-absolutive and nominative-accusative alignment of the marking of grammatical relations. Kubachi has also a rich system of non-finite verb forms, including participles, gerunds, verbal nouns and infinitives. 1. Introduction In this paper we describe major nominal and verbal categories in Kubachi and give a characterisation of the marking of grammatical relations in the simple sentence. A substantial part of the paper is also devoted to non-finite verb forms and the marking of grammatical relations in non-finite clauses. Our goal is to provide a grammar of the simple sentence, which may serve as the necessary background to a forthcoming paper on the complement structures of Kubachi. We are most grateful to professor Aleksandr Magometov of the Georgian Academy of Sciences for helping us with material about Kubachi and acting as our informant. His monograph about the grammar of Kubachi (Magometov 1963 (=M.)) has been our main source about the language. Where not otherwise noted, the examples given in the paper have been elicited during field work. The representation of the Kubachi examples is given in IPA symbols, but otherwise follows the system used in Magometov 1963 (see Appendix).
    [Show full text]
  • Two Modalities of Case Assignment: Case in Sakha*
    Two Modalities of Case Assignment: Case in Sakha* Mark C. Baker and Nadya Vinokurova Rutgers University and Research Institute of Humanities -Yakutsk Abstract: Two competing ideas about how morphological case is assigned exist in the recent generative literature: the standard Chomskian view that case is assigned by designated functional heads to the closest NP via an agreement relationship, and an alternative view in which case is assigned to one NP if there is a second NP in the same local domain (Marantz 1991). We argue that these two ways of assigning case are complementary, based on data from the Turkic language Sakha. Accusative case and dative case in this language are assigned by Marantz-style configurational rules that do not refer directly to functional categories, as shown by evidence from passives, agentive nominalizations, subject raising, possessor raising, and case assignment in PPs. In contrast, there is evidence that nominative and genitive are assigned by functional heads in the Chomskian way, as shown by the distribution of nominative case and the relationship between case marking and agreement. The two methods of case assignment thus coexist, not only in Universal Grammar, but in the grammar of a single language. 1. Some theoretical background on modes of case assignment In the recent generative syntax literature, there are (at least) two major ideas about how morphological case markers come to be associated with individual noun phrases in ways that reflect aspects of the syntactic structures those noun phrases appear in. 1 The more widely-adopted idea is that structural case features are assigned to NPs by nearby functional heads.
    [Show full text]
  • Morphological and Abstract Case
    Morphological and Abstract Case Julie Anne Legate This article examines the relationship between abstract and morpho- logical case, arguing that morphological case realizes abstract Case features in a postsyntactic morphology, according to the Elsewhere Condition. A class of prima facie ergative-absolutive languages is iden- tified wherein intransitive subjects receive abstract nominative Case and transitive objects receive abstract accusative Case; these are real- ized through a morphological default, which is often mislabeled as absolutive. Further support comes from split ergativity based on a nominal hierarchy, which is shown to have a morphological source. Proposals that case and agreement are purely morphological phenom- ena are critiqued. Keywords: ergativity, Warlpiri, Niuean, Enga, Hindi, Pama-Nyungan, Icelandic, inherent Case, quirky Case, morphological case, abstract Case 1 Introduction This article explores the relationship between abstract Case and morphological case. I argue that abstract Case features are determined syntactically and realized in a postsyntactic morphological component. This morphological realization of abstract Case features is governed by the Elsewhere Condition (Anderson 1969, Kiparsky 1973, Halle and Marantz 1993, Halle 1997), resulting in an imperfect relationship between syntax and morphology, but one that is as faithful as possible given the morphological resources of the language. The data used in the argumentation come primarily from ergative languages. I identify a class of prima facie ergative-absolutive languages in which absolutive—that is, a case that groups together intransitive subjects and transitive objects—does not exist, either as an abstract Case or as a morphological case. Instead, the ‘‘absolutive’’ is the default morphological realization of abstract Case features, used when no realization of the specific Case feature is available.
    [Show full text]
  • The Basque Auxiliary Verb: Morphosyntactic Analysis and Implications for Second Language Acquisition
    THE BASQUE AUXILIARY VERB: MORPHOSYNTACTIC ANALYSIS AND IMPLICATIONS FOR SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION A Dissertation submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences of Georgetown University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics By Laura Siebecker, M.S. Washington, DC March 25, 2015 Copyright 2015 by Laura Siebecker All Rights Reserved ii THE BASQUE AUXILIARY VERB: MORPHOSYNTACTIC ANALYSIS AND IMPLCATIONS FOR SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION Laura Siebecker, M.S. Thesis Advisor: Ruth T. Kramer, Ph.D. ABSTRACT This dissertation presents a morphosyntactic analysis of the Basque auxiliary verb (AUX) and Case system. Based on this analysis, predictions for the trajectory of acquisition of AUX and Case marking by Spanish-speaking adult learners were generated and tested in a pilot study. Findings provide a solid foundation for future research, particularly into the impact of age of initial exposure to Basque. AUX is a highly complex, multi-morphemic structure that includes clitics doubling the subject, direct object, and indirect object arguments. The distribution of these clitics is claimed to be further restricted by their featural content and the arguments with which they co-occur (Arregi & Nevins, 2012). The patterning of doubled clitics in Basque proves a challenge to many analyses of this phenomenon (e.g., Jaeggli, 1992; Roberts, 2010; Sportiche, 1996; Suñer, 1988; Uriagereka, 1995). Further, current analyses of clitic doubling in Basque (Arregi & Nevins, 2012) are predicated on questionable assumptions about underlying syntactic structure. The analysis here extends the M-merger approach to clitic doubling (Harizanov, 2014; Kramer, 2014) to the Basque data, suggesting further restrictions and modifications to this operation.
    [Show full text]
  • On Structural Case in Finnish and Korean
    Lingua 121 (2011) 100–127 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Lingua journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lingua On structural case in Finnish and Korean Arto Anttila a,*, Jong-Bok Kim b a Department of Linguistics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305-2150, USA b School of English, Kyung Hee University, Seoul 130-701, Republic of Korea ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Article history: The Case Tier Hypothesis (CTH, Yip, Maling, and Jackendoff, 1987) accounts for the Received 12 February 2009 distribution of nominative and accusative in Finnish and Korean remarkably well, but a Received in revised form 23 April 2010 number of outstanding puzzles remain. We reformulate the CTH in Optimality Theory Accepted 1 May 2010 (Prince and Smolensky, 1993/2004), showing that the new proposal covers all the standard Available online 21 August 2010 data and extends to several previously problematic phenomena, such as multiple nominatives and case variation in adverbials. After analyzing the core case patterns of Keywords: Case Tier Hypothesis Finnish and Korean, we go beyond these two languages and work out the general Optimality Theory typological predictions of our proposal. This reveals what the theory admits and what it Typology of case excludes, two central questions in theoretical linguistics. ß 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Case in tiers The assignment of structural case to subjects, objects, and adverbials is sensitive to at least two kinds of factors, both illustrated by the Finnish sentences in (1) and (2): (1) Ota minu-t / kirja mukaan! take.IMP 1P.SG-ACC / book.NOM along ‘Take me / the book along!’ (2) Esa ott-i minu-t / kirja-n mukaan.
    [Show full text]
  • The Structure of Transitivity in Gitksan*
    The Structure of Transitivity in Gitksan* Clarissa Forbes University of Arizona Abstract: This paper presents a morphosyntactic analysis of transitive verbal morphology in Gitksan (Tsimshianic). I synthesize the Interior Tsimshianic literature on these morphemes and present some novel data. I then present a Distributed Morphology analysis of the Gitksan vP, arguing that transi- tivity is encoded with minimally two projections, v and Voice. In other words, Gitksan is a ‘split-vP’ language in the vein of Pylkkänen (2002). Transitivizers such as causatives and comitatives appear in v, while the independent-order transitive vowel is in Voice. The (ergative) external argument is merged in Voice, which also serves as the locus of transitive case and agreement. Finally, I argue that the transitivizers I locate in v all have distinct underlying semantic-syntactic representations—that is, they are not allomorphs—and explore some issues to be tackled regarding their semantic decompo- sition, including redundant morpheme-stacking. Keywords: Gitksan, Tsimshianic, transitivity, argument structure, little v, Voice 1 Introduction This paper examines some properties of transitivity and argument structure in Gitksan, a Tsimshianic language of the British Columbia northern interior. I consider these topics from a morphosyntactic perspective: how is transitivity composed in the syntactic structure of this language, and how does overt ‘transitive’ morphology map to this structure? This paper adopts the framework of Distributed Morphology, or DM (Halle and Marantz 1993), which assumes that clausal structures are compositionally constructed from Roots (√) from which lexical content is derived, and verbalizers (v) from which argument structure is syntactically pro- jected. While the lexical content of the Root contributes properties of the internal argument, it is v and related projections (e.g.
    [Show full text]