Typology of grammatical relations: Explanations in the typology of grammatical relations and alignment systems

S.A. (Simon) Claassen

Journal’s Website: http://ruts-journal.ruhosting.nl

Published under CC-BY-NC licence Stichting RU:ts Student Linguistics Journal RU:ts 1

Typology of grammatical relations: Explanations in the typology of grammatical relations and alignment systems1

ǯǯȱǻ’–˜—Ǽȱ•ŠŠœœŽ— Language and Communication (research)

Abstract7KHODQJXDJHVRIWKHZRUOGDUHYHU\VLPLODULQWKHLUIXQFWLRQWRIDFLOL- WDWHEDVLFKXPDQQHHGVIRUVHOIH[SUHVVLRQDQGFRPPXQLFDWLRQ,QRUGHUWRGR WKLVODQJXDJHVW\SLFDOO\DVVLJQSURSHUWLHVLQWKHIRUPRISUHGLFDWHVWRHQWLWLHV RUFRQFHSWVH[SUHVVHGLQODQJXDJHDVDUJXPHQWVRIWKHVHSUHGLFDWHV9HUEDO SUHGLFDWHVFDQYDU\LQYDOHQF\EHLQJDEOHWRKDYHXSWRWKUHHFRUHDUJXPHQWV DWWDFKHGWRWKHP,QRUGHUWRGLVWLQJXLVKWKHVHDUJXPHQWVIURPHDFKRWKHUODQ- JXDJHV FDQ DSSO\ D ZLGH YDULHW\ RI VWUDWHJLHV ZKLFK DUH UHDOL]HG DW GL੔HUHQW OLQJXLVWLFOHYHOVVXFKDVPRUSKRORJ\DQGV\QWD[6\QWDFWLFDOO\ODQJXDJHVFDQXVH ULJLGZRUGRUGHUSDWWHUQVWRHQFRGHJUDPZPDWLFDOUHODWLRQV7KH\FDQDOVRHP- SOR\PRUSKRORJLFDOPDUNLQJRIHLWKHUWKHYHUERUWKHDUJXPHQWVNQRZQDVKHDG PDUNLQJDQGGHSHQGHQWPDUNLQJUHVSHFWLYHO\(YHQZLWKLQWKHVHGL੔HUHQWVWUDWH- JLHVODQJXDJHVKDYHDYDULHW\RIRSWLRQVWRVSHFLÀFDOO\UHDOL]HWKHP)RULQVWDQFH dependent marking languages that use case markers for their arguments can HPSOR\YDULRXVDOLJQPHQWV\VWHPV$GGLWLRQDOO\WKH\FDQXVHGL੔HUHQWFRPELQD- WLRQVRIDOLJQPHQWV\VWHPVLQGL੔HUHQWOLQJXLVWLFFRQWH[WV2QHFDQWKXVVHHWKDW ODQJXDJHVGLVSOD\DFRQVLGHUDEOHDPRXQWRIYDULDWLRQLQDIDLUO\EDVLFDVSHFWRI ODQJXDJHQDPHO\WKHIXQGDPHQWDOVWUXFWXUHRIWKHLUEDVLFVHQWHQFHV7KLVSDSHU ZLOOH[SORUHGL੔HUHQWH[SODQDWLRQVDVWRZK\ODQJXDJHVVKRZWKLVYDULDWLRQPRVW- O\IRFXVLQJRQJUDPPDWLFDOUHODWLRQVDQGDOLJQPHQWV\VWHPV7KLVZLOOEHGRQH WKURXJKFORVHDQDO\VLVRISUHYLRXVOLWHUDWXUHRQDOLJQPHQWV\VWHPV7KLVDQDO\VLV VKRZHGWKDWIRUDOOSRVVLEOHDOLJQPHQWW\SHVDFOHDUH[SODQDWLRQFRXOGEHR੔HUHG

Key words: typology, grammatical relations, alignment systems, morpho- syntax, case marking

1. Introduction

Regardless of their endless diversity, all of the world’s languages are ŸŽ›¢ȱ œ’–’•Š›ȱ ’—ȱ ‘Ž’›ȱ ‹Šœ’Œȱ ž—Œ’˜—ǯȱ —ȱ ˜›Ž›ȱ ˜ȱ ŽěŽŒ’ŸŽ•¢ȱ ŽœŒ›’‹Žȱ ŠŒ’˜—œȱ and concepts in the real world, languages assign predicates, often, but not always, in the form of verbs, to certain arguments, which take the form of nouns. Where these nouns refer to the objects or concepts in the real world that speakers want to say something about, the predicate expresses the ac- tion or property that they want to assign to these nouns. The number of Š›ž–Ž—œȱŠĴŠŒ‘Žȱ˜ȱŠȱŸŽ›‹ȱŒŠ—ȱŸŠ›¢ǯȱ —›Š—œ’’ŸŽǰȱ›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱŠ—ȱ’›Š—œ’- ’ŸŽȱŸŽ›‹œȱŠ”Žȱ˜—Žǰȱ ˜ȱ˜›ȱ‘›ŽŽȱŠ›ž–Ž—œǰȱ›Žœ™ŽŒ’ŸŽ•¢ȱǻŠž•‘Š‹Ž›ǰȱŘŖŗŗǼǯȱ Additionally, many languages have so-called impersonal verbs, which do not take any arguments whatsoever. This class of verbs usually mainly in-

ŗȱ ȱ‘ŠŸŽȱ ›’ĴŽ—ȱ‘’œȱ™Š™Ž›ȱ˜›ȱŠȱŒ˜ž›œŽȱŠž‘ȱ‹¢ȱŸŠȱŒ‘ž•ĵŽȬŽ›—ȱ‘Šȱ ȱ˜••˜ ŽȱŠȱ‘Žȱȱ Winter School 2019 at the University of Amsterdam. 12 RU:ts 1 cludes weather verbs or verbs relating to atmospheric conditions (Malchuk- ˜ŸǰȱŠ Šǰȱǭȱ’Ž ’Ž›œ”ŠǰȱŘŖŗŗǼǯȱ–˜—ȱ‘Žȱ™˜œœ’‹•ŽȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱ˜ȱŠȱŸŽ›‹ǰȱŠȱ distinction is usually made between subjects and objects, with an additional distinction between direct and indirect objects in case of a ditransitive verb. Despite their universality in argument structures, languages display considerable diversity in their overt expression of these structures. Lan- žŠŽœȱ Ž–™•˜¢ȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ œ›ŠŽ’Žœȱ ’—ȱ ˜›Ž›ȱ ˜ȱ ’œ’—ž’œ‘ȱ ŒŽ›Š’—ȱ Š›ž- ments from each other. Not only do they have the possibility to make ‘ŽœŽȱ ’œ’—Œ’˜—œȱ Šȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ •ŽŸŽ•œȱ ˜ȱ •Š—žŠŽǰȱ œžŒ‘ȱ Šœȱ –˜›™‘˜•˜¢ȱ Š—ȱ œ¢—Š¡ǰȱ ’—ȱ ‘Žȱ ˜›–œȱ ˜ȱ ’—ĚŽŒ’˜—ȱ Š—ȱ ˜›ȱ ˜›Ž›ǰȱ ›Žœ™ŽŒ’ŸŽ•¢ǰȱ •Š—- guages also vary in the exact ways in which these strategies are em- ™•˜¢Žǰȱ œžŒ‘ȱ Šœȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ –˜›™‘˜œ¢—ŠŒ’Œȱ Š•’—–Ž—œȱ ǻ’••’Š–œǰȱ ŗşŞŗǼǯ One logical option for languages to mark their arguments is the use of ˜›ȱ˜›Ž›ǯȱŠ—žŠŽœȱ ’‘ȱŠȱę¡Žȱ ˜›ȱ˜›Ž›ȱŒŠ—ȱ›ŽœŽ›ŸŽȱœŽ—Ž—ŒŽȱ™˜œ’- ’˜—œȱ˜›ȱ’쎛Ž—ȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱǻ’——Ž–§”’ǰȱŘŖŗŖǼǯȱ‘’œȱ ˜›”œȱŽœ™ŽŒ’Š••¢ȱ Ž••ȱ in languages that place one argument before the verb and the other argu- ment after the verb, such as the many SVO languages the world has, as well Šœȱ‘˜œŽȱŸŽ›¢ȱŽ ȱ•Š—žŠŽœȱ‘Šȱ‘ŠŸŽȱŠĴŽœŽȱȱ ˜›ȱ˜›Ž›ǰȱŽœ™’Žȱ‘Žȱ fact that these languages are really rare. Languages with other basic word orders can of course still employ word order to mark their arguments, as long as they are rigid in their basic order. In these languages, howev- er, it might become problematic when one of the arguments is not overt- •¢ȱ Ž¡™›ŽœœŽǰȱ ˜›ȱ ’—œŠ—ŒŽȱ ‘Ž—ȱ ˜™’Œȱ ›˜™ȱ ˜ŒŒž›œȱ ǻ’ŒŽ›Šœȱ ǭȱ NJ£ǰȱ ŗşşşǼǯ —˜‘Ž›ȱ ŽěŽŒ’ŸŽȱ œ›ŠŽ¢ȱ ˜›ȱ •Š—žŠŽœȱ ˜ȱ ’œ’—ž’œ‘ȱ ‘Ž’›ȱ Š›ž- ments is marking. Many languages encode information such as person, number and gender about either the subject, the or both on verb in- ̎Œ’˜—ǰȱ œ˜ȬŒŠ••Žȱ ‘ŽŠȱ –Š›”’—ǯȱ —˜‘Ž›ȱ ˜›–ȱ ˜ȱ –Š›”’—ȱ ’œȱ Ž™Ž—- ent marking, where arguments are assigned case markers that encode ‘Ž’›ȱ ›Š––Š’ŒŠ•ȱ ž—Œ’˜—ȱ ǻ’Œ‘˜•œǰȱ ŗşŞŜǼǯȱ ‘’œȱ •ŠĴŽ›ȱ œ›ŠŽ¢ȱ ’œȱ šž’Žȱ Ž- fective, because these case markers are usually very easy to distinguish. In order to further analyze these case markers and the grammatical func- ’˜—œȱ‘Ž¢ȱŽ—Œ˜Žǰȱ’ȱ’œȱžœŽž•ȱ˜ȱŽę—ŽȱŒŽ›Š’—ȱœŽ–Š—’Œȱ–ŠŒ›˜Ȭ›˜•Žœǰȱ ‘’Œ‘ȱ Ž—Œ˜–™ŠœœȱŠȱ–ž•’žŽȱ˜ȱœŽ–Š—’Œȱ›˜•ŽœȱŠŒ›˜œœȱ•Š—žŠŽœȱŠ—ȱŠŒ›˜œœȱ’쎛- ent semantic verb types. Although a variety of terms have originally been proposed, a distinction is now often made between intransitive subjects, ’—’ŒŠŽȱ ‹¢ȱ ‘Žȱ •ŽĴŽ›ȱ ǰȱ ›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱ œž‹“ŽŒœǰȱ ’—’ŒŠŽȱ ‹¢ȱǰȱ Š—ȱ ›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱ objects, indicated by O (Dowty, 1991). In (1-2) below, examples of an intran- œ’’ŸŽȱŠ—ȱ›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱœŽ—Ž—ŒŽȱ›˜–ȱŠ’—ȱŒŠ—ȱ‹ŽȱœŽŽ—ǯȱ‘Žȱꛜǰȱ’—›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱ sentence has only one single argument, the S argument, whereas the sec- ond one has both a subject, the A argument, and an object, the O argument.

(1) canis curr-it dog.ћќњ run-3 “The dog runs.” (Ayer, 2014)

(2) canis occid-it ŒŠĴȬž– dog.ћќњ kill.ѝђџѓ-3 cat-юѐѐ ȃ‘Žȱ˜ȱ”’••Žȱ‘ŽȱŒŠǯȄȱǻŠ—ȱŸŽ›‹›˜ŽŒ”ǰȱŘŖŖřǼ

13 RU:ts 1

Using these macro-roles, one can more easily compare the case marking sys- Ž–œȱ˜ȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ•Š—žŠŽœȱŠ—ȱœŽŽȱ’ȱ•Š—žŠŽœȱžœŽȱœ’–’•Š›ȱ˜›–œȱ˜›ȱŒŽ›Š’—ȱ Š›ž–Ž—œȱ ’‘ȱœ’–’•Š›ȱž—Œ’˜—œǰȱ›Žœž•’—ȱ’—ȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ¢™Žœȱ˜ȱ–˜›™‘˜œ¢—- ŠŒ’ŒȱŠ•’—–Ž—ǯȱ‘’œȱŠ›’Œ•Žȱ ’••ȱŠĴŽ–™ȱ˜ȱŽ¡™•Š’—ȱ ‘¢ȱ‘Žȱ•Š—žŠŽœȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ world are so diverse in their alignment strategies. The expectations are that an explanation of some kind, ranging from speakers’ preferences to notions like ›ŽšžŽ—Œ¢ȱŠ—ȱŽŒ˜—˜–¢ǰȱŒŠ—ȱ‹Žȱ˜ěŽ›Žȱ˜›ȱŠ••ȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ¢™Žœȱ˜ȱŠ•’—- –Ž—ȱ˜ž—ǯȱ —ȱ‘Žȱ˜••˜ ’—ǰȱŠ—ȱ˜ŸŽ›Ÿ’Ž ȱ ’••ȱꛜȱ‹Žȱ™›ŽœŽ—Žȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ’쎛- ent types of alignment, followed by possible explanations for the occurrence of certain alignment types as well as the preference for some types over others.

2. Alignment types

’›œȱ ˜ȱ Š••ǰȱ •Š—žŠŽœȱ ŒŠ—ȱ žœŽȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ –˜›™‘˜•˜’ŒŠ•ȱ ˜›–œȱ ˜›ȱ ŒŽ›Š’—ȱ ›Š––Š’ŒŠ•ȱ ›˜•Žœȱ ˜›ȱ ‘Ž¢ȱ ŒŠ—ȱ žœŽȱ ‘Žȱ œŠ–Žȱ ˜›–ȱ ˜›ȱ –ž•’™•Žȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ ›˜•Žœǰȱ œ˜ȬŒŠ••Žȱ œ¢—Œ›Ž’œ–ȱ ǻ’Œ”Ž•ȱ ǭȱ ’Œ‘˜•œǰȱ ŘŖŖşǼǯȱ ŽŒ˜—•¢ǰȱ •Š—žŠ- Žœȱ ŒŠ—ȱ Š•œ˜ȱ žœŽȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ –˜›™‘˜•˜’ŒŠ•ȱ ˜›–œȱ ˜›ȱ ‘Žȱ œŠ–Žȱ –ŠŒ›˜Ȭ›˜•Žȱ ’—ȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ œ’žŠ’˜—œǰȱ œ˜ȬŒŠ••Žȱ œ™•’ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ǯȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ œ™•’œȱ ŒŠ—ȱ ‹Žȱ –˜- erated by a variety of linguistic factors, which will be discussed later on. Logically, when relating syncretism to the S, A and O arguments, there are ꟎ȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ™˜œœ’‹’•’’ŽœȱŠœȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ˜›ȱ’Ž—’ŒŠ•ȱ˜›–œȱ‘Ž¢ȱŒŠ—ȱ‘ŠŸŽǯȱ ’›œȱ˜ȱŠ••ǰȱŠ••ȱ‘›ŽŽȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱŒŠ—ȱ‘ŠŸŽȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ˜›–œǯȱ’’˜—Š••¢ǰȱŽ’‘Ž›ȱ ˜—Žȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ‘›ŽŽȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱŒŠ—ȱ‘ŠŸŽȱŠȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ˜›–ȱ‘Š—ȱ‘Žȱ›Ž–Š’—’—ȱ ˜ǰȱ ‘’•Žȱ‘Žȱ˜‘Ž›ȱ ˜ȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱ‘ŠŸŽȱ‘ŽȱœŠ–Žȱ˜›–ǯȱ’—Š••¢ǰȱŠ••ȱ‘›ŽŽȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱ can have the same form. All of these possibilities occur in the languages of the world, although some of them are considerably more frequent than others. ‘Žȱ–˜œȱŒ˜––˜—ȱŠ•’—–Ž—ȱ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱ’œȱ—˜–’—Š’ŸŽȬŠŒŒžœŠ’ŸŽȱŠ•’—–Ž—ǯȱ Languages with this type of alignment mark the object of a transitive sentence with a distinct case marker, the . The subject of an intransitive subject and that of a transitive sentence have the same form. This case form, ‘’Œ‘ȱ’œȱžœžŠ••¢ȱ‹žȱ—˜ȱŠ• Š¢œȱž—–Š›”Žǰȱ’œȱŒŠ••Žȱ—˜–’—Š’ŸŽǯȱŠ—¢ȱ —˜Ȭž- ropean languages, including Latin, have nominative-accusative alignment. As one can see in (1-2) above, intransitive and transitive subjects appear in the in Latin, while transitive objects receive an accusative marker, ‘’Œ‘ȱ‘ŠœȱœŽŸŽ›Š•ȱ’쎛Ž—ȱŠ••˜–˜›™‘œǰȱ’—Œ•ž’—ȱ-am, -um and -emǰȱ˜›ȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ and genders. Similarly, in Japanese, as can be seen in (3-4) below, the case marker -ga indicates nominative case, while -o marks accusative case.

(3) ˜˜”˜ȬŠ tsui-ta

man-ћќњ arrive-ѝѓѣ ȃ‘Žȱ–Š—ȱŠ››’ŸŽǯȄȱǻŠ£ž”Šȱǭȱ ˜‘ǰȱŗşşśǼ

(4) ˜˜”˜ȬŠ ”˜–˜˜Ȭ˜ mi-ta man-ћќњ child-юѐѐ see-ѝѓѣ ȃ‘Žȱ–Š—ȱœŠ ȱ‘ŽȱŒ‘’•ǯȄȱǻŠ£ž”Šȱǭȱ ˜‘ǰȱŗşşśǼ

The second most common type of alignment is ergative-absolutive align- ment. In these languages, rather than with transitive subjects, the intransitive œž‹“ŽŒœȱ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱ˜Ž‘Ž›ȱ ’‘ȱ›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱ˜‹“ŽŒœǯȱ‘ŽȱŽ›Š’ŸŽȱŒŠœŽȱ’œȱžœŽȱ˜›ȱ 14 RU:ts 1 the subjects of transitive sentences, whereas intransitive subjects and transi- tive objects are marked with the same, usually unmarked, . Basque is an example of such a language, as can be seen in (5-6), which show how transitive subjects are distinguished from zero-marked intransitive sub- jects and transitive objects through the marker ȬŽ”ǯȱȱꛜȱœ’‘ǰȱ’ȱ one looks at these two alignment types neutrally, neither of these types seems to be unambiguously more logical than the other. Still, nominative-accusative languages are much more frequent than ergative-absolutive languages, the ˜›–Ž›ȱ‹Ž’—ȱŠ›˜ž—ȱ‘›ŽŽȱ’–Žœȱ–˜›Žȱ›ŽšžŽ—ȱ‘Š—ȱ‘Žȱ•ŠĴŽ›ȱǻ’Œ‘˜•œǰȱŗşşřǼǯ

(5) gizon-a etorri da man-ћќњ arrive-ѝѓѣ be.3 “The man has arrived.” (King, 1994)

(6) ’£˜—ȬŠȬ” mutil-a ’”žœ’ du

man-ѠєȬђџє boy-Ѡє see have.3 “The man saw the boy.” (King, 1994)

Š—žŠŽœȱ ‘Šȱ –Š›”ȱ Š••ȱ ‘›ŽŽȱ –ŠŒ›˜Ȭ›˜•Žœȱ ’‘ȱ Šȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ ŒŠœŽȱ Š›Žȱ called tripartite languages or ergative-accusative languages. Aside from using an ergative case for transitive subjects and an accusative case for transitive objects, tripartite languages also have a distinct case for in- transitive subjects. This case is usually called the and al- ways has zero marking. Tripartite languages are fairly uncommon. An ex- Š–™•Žȱ ˜ȱ œžŒ‘ȱ Šȱ •Š—žŠŽȱ ’œȱ Ž£ȱ Ž›ŒŽǯȱ —ȱ ǻŝȬŞǼǰȱ Ž¡Š–™•Žœȱ œ‘˜ ’—ȱ ‘Žȱ intransitive, ergative and accusative cases used in this language are shown.

(7) hi-páayn-a háama

3.іћѡџ-arrive-юѠѝ man ȃ‘Žȱ–Š—ȱŠ››’ŸŽǯȄȱǻžŽǰȱŗşŞŜǼ

ǻŞǼ háama-nm pée-‘wi-ye Ž ø”’¢ŽȬ—Ž man-ђџє 3.ѡџ-shoot-юѠѝ elk-юѐѐ ȃ‘Žȱ–Š—ȱœ‘˜ȱ‘ŽȱŽ•”ǯȄȱǻžŽǰȱŗşŞŜǼ

Š—žŠŽœȱ ’‘ȱ ™ž›Ž•¢ȱ ’›ŽŒȱ Š•’—–Ž—ǰȱ ‘Ž›Žȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ Š›ž–Ž—œȱ Š›Žȱ in no way explicitly distinguished from each other and listeners have to ›Ž•¢ȱ ˜—ȱ Œ˜—Ž¡ȱ Š—ȱ ˜›•ȱ ”—˜ •ŽŽȱ ’—ȱ ˜›Ž›ȱ ˜ȱ ŽěŽŒ’ŸŽ•¢ȱ ’—Ž›™›Žȱ œŽ—- tences, are really rare. The single case in which the core arguments appear in these languages is called the , which is always unmarked. An example of a language that does not distinguish between subjects and ob- “ŽŒœȱŠ—¢–˜›Žȱ’œȱŒ˜Ĵ’œ‘ȱ ŠŽ•’ŒǰȱŠœȱŒŠ—ȱ‹ŽȱœŽŽ—ȱ’—ȱǻşȬŗŖǼǯȱ‘Ž›ŽȱŠ›Žǰȱ˜ȱŒ˜ž›œŽǰȱ –Š—¢ȱ •Š—žŠŽœǰȱ ’—Œ•ž’—ȱ —•’œ‘ȱ Š—ȱ žŒ‘ǰȱ ˜›ȱ ’—œŠ—ŒŽǰȱ ‘Šȱ ‘ŠŸŽȱ Ž’- ther partly or entirely abolished their case systems and do not overtly mark their arguments morphologically, but most of these languages employ other strategies in order to distinguish their arguments, such as the rig-

15 RU:ts 1

’ȱ ȱ ˜›ȱ ˜›Ž›ȱ ˜ȱ —•’œ‘ȱ ˜›ȱ ‘Žȱ žœŽȱ ˜ȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ –Š›”Ž›œȱ ˜›ȱ œž‹“ŽŒœȱ Š—ȱ ˜‹“ŽŒœȱ ˜—ȱ ‘Žȱ ŸŽ›‹ǰȱ œžŒ‘ȱ Šœȱ ‘˜œŽȱ ’—ȱ ’–Šœȱ ǻŠ—ȱ ŸŽ›‹›˜ŽŒ”ǰȱ ŘŖŖřǼǯ

(9) ruith mi run.PST 1 ȃ ȱ›Š—ǯȄȱǻ˜‹’—œ˜—ǰȱŘŖŖŞǼ

(10) chunnaic mi an cat see.ѝѠѡ 1 the cat ȃ ȱœŠ ȱ‘ŽȱŒŠǯȄȱǻ˜‹’—œ˜—ǰȱŘŖŖŞǼ

‘Žȱꗊ•ȱ™˜œœ’‹•ŽȱŠ•’—–Ž—ȱ¢™Žȱ ˜ž•ȱ‘Ž—ȱ‹ŽȱŠ•’—–Ž—ȱ ‘Ž›Žȱ‹˜‘ȱŠ›- guments of transitive sentences are marked in the same way, whereas the ar- ž–Ž—œȱ˜ȱ’—›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱœŽ—Ž—ŒŽœȱŠ›Žȱ–Š›”Žȱ’쎛Ž—•¢ǰȱœ˜ȬŒŠ••Žȱ›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱ alignment. This type of alignment is extremely rare, but it does occur. One of the few languages to have this type of alignment is Rushani, albeit only in the past tense, resulting in a split alignment system. In the present tense, the language displays a typical nominative-accusative system, but the case form that is used to mark transitive objects in the present tense is also used for transitive subjects in the past tense, as can be seen in (11-12), making it a in this tense.

(11) az-um pa Xara㸌 sut 1-Ѡє to Xorog go.ѝѠѡ “I went to Xorog.” (Payne, 2002)

(12) mu ¬ wunt 1.ѡџ 2.ѡџ see.ѝѠѡ “I saw you.” (Payne, 2002)

Figure 1.ȱ‘Žȱ꟎ȱ™˜œœ’‹•ŽȱŠ•’—–Ž—ȱ¢™Žœ

œ’Žȱ ›˜–ȱ ‘Žȱ ꟎ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ œ¢œŽ–œȱ –Ž—’˜—Žȱ Š‹˜ŸŽǰȱ ‘’Œ‘ȱ Š›Žȱ œ‘˜ —ȱ’—ȱ’ž›ŽȱŗȱŠ‹˜ŸŽǰȱ‘Ž›ŽȱŠ›ŽȱŠ•œ˜ȱ–Š—¢ȱ•Š—žŠŽœȱ‘Šȱ’œ™•Š¢ȱŠȱŒ˜–- ‹’—Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱŠ•’—–Ž—œȱŠŒ›˜œœȱ’쎛Ž—ȱœ’žŠ’˜—œǰȱœ˜ȬŒŠ••Žȱœ™•’ȱŠ•’—–Ž—ǯȱ This split can be conditioned by a multitude of variables. Some languages, for instance, distinguish between multiple types based on ’쎛Ž—ȱ œŽ–Š—’Œȱ ›˜•Žœȱ ˜›ȱ ‘Ž’›ȱ ›Žœ™ŽŒ’ŸŽȱ Š›ž–Ž—œȱ Š—ȱ –Š›”ȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ Š›- guments distinctly. This split is strongly related to volition and the degree to which the subject of the verb is a true agent. In languages with this type of split, verbs that have very volitional and agentive subjects, so-called ac-

16 RU:ts 1 tive or unergative verbs, mark their arguments similarly to transitive sub- jects, whereas verbs that have less agentive subjects, so-called stative or un- accusative verbs, mark these subjects like transitive objects (Duranti, 2004). Another type of split alignment is a split based on tense or aspect. Some languages, such as Georgian, show a split based on tense. In the present tense, Georgian has nominative-accusative alignment, whereas in the past Ž—œŽǰȱ’ȱ‘ŠœȱŽ›Š’ŸŽȬŠ‹œ˜•ž’ŸŽȱŠ•’—–Ž—ȱǻ˜–›’ŽǰȱŗşŝŞǼǯȱȱœ™•’ȱ•’”Žȱ‘’œȱŒŠ—ȱ also be conditioned by aspect, like in many Indo-Iranian languages, where a nominative-accusative system is found in the imperfective aspect, while Š—ȱ Ž›Š’ŸŽȬŠ‹œ˜•ž’ŸŽȱ œ¢œŽ–ȱ ’œȱ ŠĴŽœŽȱ ’—ȱ ‘Žȱ ™Ž›ŽŒ’ŸŽȱ Šœ™ŽŒȱ ǻž‹Ž—’”ǰȱ ŗşŞşǼǯȱœȱŒŠ—ȱ‹ŽȱœŽŽ—ȱ’—ȱǻŗŗȬŗŘǼǰȱžœ‘Š—’ȱŠ•œ˜ȱ‘ŠœȱŠȱœ™•’ȱŠ•’—–Ž—ȱœ¢œŽ–ȱ like this, namely a split based around tense similar to the one in Georgian. Animacy is also often an important factor in the alignment of languag- es. Generally, in languages with this type of split, less animate subjects are more likely to be receive an ergative marker, while more animate subjects are usually unmarked. Conversely, some languages tend to mark only high- •¢ȱ Š—’–ŠŽȱ ˜‹“ŽŒœȱ ’‘ȱ Š—ȱ ŠŒŒžœŠ’ŸŽȱ –Š›”Ž›ǯȱ ‘Žȱ ™ŠĴŽ›—œȱ œ‘˜ —ȱ ŠŒ›˜œœȱ languages with animacy splits led to the formulation of the animacy hier- archy (Song, 2014). This hierarchy ranks referents according to their degree of animacy, where referents with a lower animacy are placed further down in the hierarchy. At the top of this hierarchy are personal pronouns, with ꛜȱ Š—ȱ œŽŒ˜—ȱ ™Ž›œ˜—ȱ ™›˜—˜ž—œȱ ‹Ž’—ȱ –˜›Žȱ Š—’–ŠŽȱ ‘Š—ȱ ‘’›ȱ ™Ž›œ˜—ȱ pronouns, followed by proper nouns and then common nouns referring to humans, other animate referents and inanimate referents, in that order. In fact, the place a referent receives in the hierarchy is not just based on anima- cy, but on an interaction between certain factors, such as animacy, person Š—ȱ›ŽŽ›Ž—’Š•’¢ȱǻ›˜ǰȱŘŖŖřǼǯȱŠŒ˜›œȱ‘ŠȱŠ›ŽȱŠ•œ˜ȱ˜ŒŒŠœ’˜—Š••¢ȱŒ•Š’–Žȱ˜ȱ ‹Žȱ’—Ÿ˜•ŸŽȱ’—ȱ‘Žȱ˜›–Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ‘’Ž›Š›Œ‘¢ȱŠ›Žȱ—ž–‹Ž›ȱŠ—ȱŽę—’Ž—Žœœǯ

3. Explanations

As was discussed above, languages are not only able to mark gram- –Š’ŒŠ•ȱ ›Ž•Š’˜—œȱ Šȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ •’—ž’œ’Œȱ •ŽŸŽ•œǰȱ ‹žȱ ‘Ž¢ȱ Š•œ˜ȱ ’œ™•Š¢ȱ Œ˜—- œ’Ž›Š‹•Žȱ ’ŸŽ›œ’¢ȱ ’—ȱ ‘Žȱ œ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ œ›ŠŽ’Žœȱ ‘Ž¢ȱ Ž–™•˜¢ȱ Šȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ ’- ferent levels. An example of this can be seen in case marking, where •Š—žŠŽœȱŒŠ—ȱžœŽȱ‘ŽȱœŠ–ŽȱŒŠœŽœȱ˜›ȱ’쎛Ž—ȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱŠ—ȱŽŸŽ—ȱ’쎛- Ž—ȱ ŒŠœŽœȱ ˜›ȱ ‘Žȱ œŠ–Žȱ Š›ž–Ž—œȱ ’—ȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ œ’žŠ’˜—œǯȱ ‘’œȱ ›Žœž•œȱ ’—ȱ Šȱ ŸŠ›’Ž¢ȱ ˜ȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ Œ˜–‹’—Š’˜—œȱ ˜ȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ ŒŠœŽœǰȱ œ˜–Žȱ ˜ȱ ‘’Œ‘ȱ Š›Žȱ Šȱ •˜ȱ –˜›ŽȱŒ˜––˜—ȱ‘Š—ȱ˜‘Ž›œǯȱ‘’œȱ›Š’œŽœȱ‘ŽȱšžŽœ’˜—ȱ ‘¢ȱ‘ŽœŽȱœ™ŽŒ’ęŒȱ™Š- terns are more common than the rest. The following will provide explana- ’˜—œȱ˜›ȱŠœȱ–Š—¢ȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ˜‹œŽ›ŸŽȱ™ŠĴŽ›—œȱŠ—ȱ‘Ž’›ȱ›ŽšžŽ—Œ’ŽœȱŠœȱ™˜œœ’‹•Žǯ ȱ—˜’˜—ȱ‘Šȱ’œȱŽœœŽ—’Š•ȱ˜ȱ’—Œ˜›™˜›ŠŽȱ ‘Ž—ȱ›¢’—ȱ˜ȱŽ¡™•Š’—ȱ‘ŽœŽȱꗍ- ings is the notion of economy, which means that speakers should not specify ž——ŽŒŽœœŠ›¢ȱ’—˜›–Š’˜—ȱ’—ȱ‘Ž’›ȱžĴŽ›Š—ŒŽœȱǻŠ—ȱ Ž•Ž›Ž—ǰȱŘŖŖŚǼǯȱ‘’œȱŽěŽŒ- tively explains why in most languages, intransitive subjects are usually un- marked, as this single argument does not need to be overtly distinguished from any other core arguments. It also explains the fact that nominative-accu- sative and ergative-absolutive languages are by far the most common types of alignment and tripartite or transitive systems are really rare, as it is only

17 RU:ts 1

necessary to distinguish transitive subjects from transitive objects, but it is not necessary to distinguish either of these from intransitive subjects. This means that only one of the two arguments of transitive clauses needs to be explicitly marked in order to distinguish it from the other argument, while the other argument can simply remain unmarked, just like intransitive subjects. This does not yet explain, however, the preference of either nominative-ac- cusative or ergative-absolutive alignments. As was stated before, nomina- tive-accusative systems are much more frequent than ergative-absolutive systems. This appears to make sense, as nominative-accusative systems use ‘ŽȱœŠ–ŽȱŒŠœŽȱ˜›–ȱ˜›ȱ ˜ȱ’쎛Ž—ȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱ‘Šȱ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱœ’–’•Š›•¢ȱœ¢—- tactically, both functioning as the subjects or topics of sentences and usual- ly, in the case of active verbs at least, also as agents of the verb. This makes one wonder, however, why ergative-absolutive systems exist altogether. The occurrence of ergative-absolutive alignment has been linked to discourse and information structure and the fact that intransitive subjects and transi- tive objects appear to behave similarly in that they very frequently introduce new referents in the discourse through full noun phrases, whereas transitive subjects appear in the form of full noun phrases considerably less frequently ǻžȱ˜’œǰȱŗşŞŝǼǯȱ•‘˜ž‘ȱ‘’œȱ‘Ž˜›¢ȱ‘Šœȱ‹ŽŽ—ȱŒ›’’Œ’£Žȱ‹ŠœŽȱ˜—ȱ‘Žȱꗍ- ’—ȱ‘Šȱ•Ž¡’ŒŠ•ȱ’—›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱœž‹“ŽŒœȱ˜ȱ—˜ȱ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱ’—ȱ›ŽšžŽ—Œ¢ȱ ’‘ȱŽ’‘Ž›ȱ lexical transitive subjects or objects, this dispreference for lexical transitive œž‹“ŽŒœȱ Š™™ŽŠ›œȱ ˜ȱ ‘ŠŸŽȱ ‹ŽŽ—ȱ Œ˜—ę›–Žȱ ŠŒ›˜œœȱ •Š—žŠŽœȱ ǻŸŽ›ŽĴǰȱ ŘŖŖşǼǯ Although the explanation of split alignment systems based on semantic roles seems quite straightforward, as it makes sense for the subjects of ac- ’ŸŽȱ ŸŽ›‹œȱ ˜ȱ ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱ ’—ȱ ‘Ž’›ȱ œŽ–Š—’Œȱ ›˜•Žȱ Œ‘Š›ŠŒŽ›’œ’Œœȱ ’‘ȱ ›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱ œž‹“ŽŒœȱ Š—ȱ ˜›ȱ ‘Žȱ œž‹“ŽŒœȱ ˜ȱ œŠ’ŸŽȱ ŸŽ›‹œȱ ˜ȱ ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱ ’‘ȱ ›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱ ˜‹- jects, explanations for the other two split alignment types mentioned above, Š•‘˜ž‘ȱ •Žœœȱ œ›Š’‘˜› Š›ǰȱ ŒŠ—ȱ Š•œ˜ȱ ‹Žȱ ™›˜™˜œŽǯȱ ˜›ȱ ’—œŠ—ŒŽǰȱ ˜›ȱ œ™•’ȱ alignments based on tense or aspect, one can clearly see a cross-linguistic ™ŠĴŽ›—ǯȱŠ—žŠŽœȱ‘Šȱ‘ŠŸŽȱœžŒ‘ȱŠȱœ™•’ȱŠ• Š¢œȱ‘ŠŸŽȱŠȱ—˜–’—Š’ŸŽȬŠŒŒžœŠ- tive system in the present tense or imperfective aspect and an ergative-ab- œ˜•ž’ŸŽȱœ¢œŽ–ȱ’—ȱ‘Žȱ™ŠœȱŽ—œŽȱ˜›ȱ™Ž›ŽŒ’ŸŽȱŠœ™ŽŒȱǻœž—˜ŠǰȱŗşŞŗǼǯȱ‘’œȱ ™ŠĴŽ›—ȱŒŠ—ȱ‹ŽȱŽ¡™•Š’—Žȱ‹¢ȱŠœœž–’—ȱ‘Šȱ‘ŽȱŒŽ—›Š•ȱ™Š›’Œ’™Š—ȱ˜ȱŠ—ȱŠŒ- tion or the participant whose viewpoint is taken has a tendency to be un- marked in languages. In the imperfective aspect, one could arguably say that the subject is the most central participant, whereas the object is more central ’—ȱ‘Žȱ™Ž›ŽŒ’ŸŽȱŠœ™ŽŒǰȱ‹ŽŒŠžœŽȱ‘Žȱ˜‹“ŽŒȱ’œȱ–˜›ŽȱŠěŽŒŽȱ‹¢ȱ‘ŽȱŠŒ’˜—ȱ˜ȱ ‘ŽȱŸŽ›‹ȱ’—ȱ‘Žȱ•ŠĴŽ›ȱŒŠœŽȱ‘Š—ȱ’—ȱ‘Žȱ˜›–Ž›ȱǻŠ•Œ‘ž”˜ŸȱǭȱŽȱ ˜˜™ǰȱŘŖŗŗǼǯ When looking at (11-12) again, Rushani also seems to follow this pat- tern of marking the subject only in the past tense, which might suggest ‘Šȱ ‘’œȱ •Š—žŠŽȱ ˜›’’—Š••¢ȱ ‘Šȱ Šȱ ž••¢Ȭ̎Žȱ œ™•’ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ œ¢œŽ–ȱ with ergative-absolutive alignment in the past tense, which has started to decay through the increased marking of objects in the past tense. If one now looks at the fact that young speakers of Rushani are starting to occa- sionally leave subjects in the present tense unmarked in spoken language, ˜—Žȱ ꗍœȱ ŽŸ’Ž—ŒŽȱ ‘Šȱ ‘Žȱ ˜ȱ ›Š—œ’’ŸŽȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ žœ‘Š—’ȱ ’œȱ ’œ™•Š¢- ing might in fact be an unstable transition stage from split alignment to common nominative-accusative alignment, which is in line with the rar- ity and apparent illogicalness of this type of alignment (Payne, 2002).

ŗŞ RU:ts 1

’—Š••¢ǰȱ œ™•’ȱ Š•’—–Ž—œȱ ›ŽŸ˜•Ÿ’—ȱ Š›˜ž—ȱ Š—’–ŠŒ¢ȱ ŒŠ—ȱ ‹Žȱ Ž¡™•Š’—Žȱ ’—ȱ terms of frequency and economy. Since highly animate referents are much more likely to be agents and subjects and less animate referents are more likely to be patients and objects, it would make sense economically to leave these arguments unmarked when they appear in a semantic role both speak- ers and listeners would expect them to be in, whereas arguments that ap- pear in unexpected semantic roles could be marked overtly in order to make this semantic role explicit (Aissen, 2003). This can be clearly seen in languages with this type of split alignment, such as Yankunytjatjara, which only marks objects that appear in the form of proper names and pronouns, which are at the top of the animacy hierarchy, with an accusative case, ‘’•Žȱ žœ’—ȱ ‘Žȱ Ž›Š’ŸŽȱ ŒŠœŽȱ ˜›ȱ Š••ȱ ¢™Žœȱ ˜ȱ œž‹“ŽŒœȱ Ž¡ŒŽ™ȱ ˜›ȱ ꛜȱ Š—ȱ œŽŒ˜—ȱ ™Ž›œ˜—ȱ ™›˜—˜ž—œǰȱ ‘’Œ‘ȱ Š›Žȱ Š•œ˜ȱ ‘’‘•¢ȱ Š—’–ŠŽȱ ǻ ˜Š›ǰȱ ŗşŞśǼǯ

4. Discussion

The languages of the world show considerable variation in the strate- ’Žœȱ ‘Ž¢ȱ žœŽȱ ’—ȱ ˜›Ž›ȱ ˜ȱ –Š›”ȱ ›Š––Š’ŒŠ•ȱ ›Ž•Š’˜—œǯȱ ’‘’—ȱ ‘Žȱ ’쎛- ent case marking systems of the world, all logically possible alignment ¢™Žœȱ ˜ŒŒž›ǰȱ Šœȱ Ž••ȱ Šœȱ ŸŠ›’Š’˜—œȱ ˜ȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ œ¢œŽ–œȱ ‘Šȱ Š›Žȱ Œ˜—’’˜—Žȱ by all sorts of linguistic factors. This makes one wonder why there is so much variation in this otherwise very basic feature of language and rais- es the question why languages would favor certain alignment types over others. Seeking explanations in terms of economy, frequency and oth- Ž›ȱ ™›ŽŽ›Ž—ŒŽœǰȱ ‘Žȱ ˜ŒŒž››Ž—ŒŽȱ ˜ȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ ¢™Žœȱ Šœȱ Ž••ȱ as languages’ motivations to choose one over another are analyzed. œȱ ŒŠ—ȱ ‹Žȱ œŽŽ—ǰȱ –˜œȱ ˜ȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ Œ›˜œœȬ•’—ž’œ’Œȱ ™ŠĴŽ›—œȱ ˜ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ ŒŠ—ȱ be explained quite nicely, even though these explanations might not be so œ›Š’‘˜› Š›ȱ˜›ȱ˜‹Ÿ’˜žœȱŠȱꛜȱœ’‘ǯȱ‘Žȱ ˜ȱ–˜œȱŒ˜––˜—ȱŠ•’—–Ž—ȱ types, nominative-accusative and ergative-absolutive alignment, are the most economical, because they only distinguish the arguments of transitive verbs, which are the only arguments that necessarily require this distinc- tion, as opposed to the arguments of intransitive verbs, which do not need ˜ȱ‹Žȱ’œ’—ž’œ‘Žȱ›˜–ȱŠ—¢ȱ˜‘Ž›ȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱ’—ȱ‘ŽȱœŽ—Ž—ŒŽǯȱž›‘Ž›–˜›Žǰȱ most split alignment types can be explained by varying degrees of nuanc- Žœȱ˜›ȱœ‘’œȱ‹¢ȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱ‹Ž ŽŽ—ȱœŽ–Š—’Œȱ›˜•Žœǯȱ¡Š–™•Žœȱ˜ȱ‘’œȱŠ›Žȱ‘Žȱ shift of viewpoint from agent to patient in alignment systems split by tense ˜›ȱŠœ™ŽŒȱŠ—ȱ‘Žȱ’쎛Ž—ȱœŽ–Š—’Œȱ›˜•Žœȱ˜ȱŠŒ’ŸŽȱŠ—ȱœŠ’ŸŽȱŸŽ›‹œȱ’—ȱŠ•’—- –Ž—ȱœ¢œŽ–œȱœ™•’ȱ‹¢ȱŸŽ›‹ȱ¢™Žǯȱ’—Š••¢ǰȱŽŒ˜—˜–¢ȱŠ•œ˜ȱœŽŽ–œȱ˜ȱ™•Š¢ȱŠ—ȱ’–- portant role in various cases, such as the general preference of languages for nominative-accusative and ergative-absolutive alignment and the use of zero marking for arguments that appear in expected semantic roles, such as ani- mate nouns appearing as subjects and inanimate nouns appearing as objects. ‘ŽœŽȱꗍ’—œȱœ‘˜ ȱ‘ŠȱŽ¡™•Š—Š’˜—œȱŒŠ—ȱ‹Žȱ™›˜Ÿ’Žȱ˜›ȱ‘Žȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ alignment systems of languages across the world. These explain both the preferences for certain individual alignment types over others, such as the preference for nominative-accusative or ergative-absolutive alignment over other alignment types, and the occurrence of split alignment systems, such as splits conditioned by animacy or verb tense. In order to clarify the occur-

19 RU:ts 1

›Ž—ŒŽȱ ˜ȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ ¢™Žœǰȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ Ž¡™•Š—Š’˜—œȱ ŒŠ••ȱ ˜—ȱ œŽŸŽ›Š•ȱ distinct notions, namely economy, frequency and cognitive preferences. ȱŒ˜ž›œŽǰȱ‘Žȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ—˜’˜—œȱžœŽȱ‘Ž›Žȱ˜ȱŽ¡™•Š’—ȱ‘ŽȱŸŠ›’Š’˜—ȱ’—ȱŠ•’—- –Ž—ȱœ¢œŽ–œȱ’œ™•Š¢ȱ’—Ž›ŠŒ’˜—ȱŠ—ȱ’ȱŒŠ—ȱ‹Žȱ’ĜŒž•ȱ˜ȱŒ˜››ŽŒ•¢ȱŠĴ›’‹žŽȱ ™˜›’˜—œȱ˜ȱŠ—ȱ˜‹œŽ›ŸŠ’˜—ȱ˜ȱŠ—ȱ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ—˜’˜—ǯȱœ™ŽŒ’Š••¢ǰȱŽ’‘Ž›ȱŒ˜—’’ŸŽȱ ™›ŽŽ›Ž—ŒŽœȱ˜›ȱ›ŽšžŽ—Œ¢ȱŒŠ—ȱ™Š›•¢ȱ˜›–ȱ‘Žȱ‹Šœ’œȱ˜›ȱŽŒ˜—˜–¢ȱŽěŽŒœǰȱ’—ȱ‘Žȱ sense that the marking of a certain argument might be a more economical choice for speakers if they have certain cognitive preferences relating to this Š›ž–Ž—ȱ’—ȱŒŽ›Š’—ȱŒ˜—Ž¡œȱ˜›ȱ’ȱ‘ŽœŽȱŠ›ž–Ž—œȱŠ›ŽȱŸŽ›¢ȱ›ŽšžŽ—•¢ȱŠĴŽœ- ed in certain contexts. The notion of cognitive preferences is also somewhat ŸŠžŽȱŠ—ȱ‘Š›ȱ˜ȱŽę—ŽȱŠ—ȱ˜—ŽȱŒ˜ž•ȱŠ•Ž›—Š’ŸŽ•¢ȱŽę—Žȱ‘Ž–ȱ˜ȱŽ—Œ˜–- ™Šœœȱ‹˜‘ȱŽŒ˜—˜–¢ȱŠ—ȱ›ŽšžŽ—Œ¢ȱŽěŽŒœǰȱ‹žȱ‘Ž¢ȱŒŠ—ȱ‹ŽȱŒ•ŽŠ›•¢ȱœŽŽ—ȱŠȱ™•Š¢ȱ as a distinct factor, for instance in alignment systems split by tense or aspect. As grammatical relations and alignment form an intriguing aspect of the core structure of languages, they have always been an intensively stud- ied topic within linguistics, in the form of both descriptions of the align- –Ž—ȱ œ¢œŽ–œȱ ˜ȱ ’—’Ÿ’žŠ•ȱ •Š—žŠŽœȱ ǻžŽǰȱ ŗşŞŜDzȱ ˜‹’—œ˜—ǰȱ ŘŖŖŞǼȱ Š—ȱ typological comparisons of wide arrays of languages (Nichols, 1993; Bickel ǭȱ’Œ‘˜•œǰȱŘŖŖşǼǯȱŽŸŽ›Š•ȱ™Š™Ž›œȱ‘ŠŸŽȱŠ•œ˜ȱœ˜ž‘ȱ˜ȱ˜ěŽ›ȱŽ¡™•Š—Š’˜—œȱ˜›ȱ ‘Žȱ ™ŠĴŽ›—’—ȱ ˜ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ œ¢œŽ–œȱ ǻž‹Ž—’”ǰȱ ŗşŞşDzȱ ŸŽ›ŽĴǰȱ ŘŖŖşDzȱ Š•- Œ‘ž”˜ŸȱǭȱŽȱ ˜˜™ǰȱŘŖŗŗǼǯȱ‘’œȱ™Š™Ž›ȱ‘˜™Žœȱ˜ȱŒ˜–‹’—Žȱ‘Žȱ”—˜ •ŽŽȱ˜ȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ ¢™Žœȱ ˜ȱ œž’Žœȱ ˜—ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ ‹¢ȱ ˜ěŽ›’—ȱ ‹˜‘ȱ ¢™˜•˜’ŒŠ•ȱ descriptions of alignment systems found in languages across the world and —ž–Ž›˜žœȱ Ž¡™•Š—Š’˜—œȱ ˜›ȱ ‘Žȱ ˜ŒŒž››Ž—ŒŽȱ ˜ȱ ‘ŽœŽȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ œ¢œŽ–œǯȱ ‘’œȱ Š¢ǰȱ‘ŽȱŒž››Ž—ȱ™Š™Ž›ȱŠœ™’›Žœȱ˜ȱŒ˜—›’‹žŽȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱꎕȱ˜ȱ•’—ž’œ’Œȱ¢™˜•- ˜¢ǰȱ —˜ȱ ˜—•¢ȱ ‹¢ȱ ˜ěŽ›’—ȱ Šȱ Œ•ŽŠ›ȱ ˜ŸŽ›Ÿ’Ž ȱ ˜ȱ ‘Žȱ ŸŠ›’˜žœȱ ¢™Žœȱ ˜ȱ œž’Žœȱ ™Ž›˜›–Žȱœ˜ȱŠ›ȱ˜—ȱ‘’œȱ˜™’Œǰȱ‹žȱŠ•œ˜ȱ‹¢ȱ’Ÿ’—ȱŠȱŽę—’’ŸŽȱŠ—œ Ž›ȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ Œ˜–™•Ž¡ȱ šžŽœ’˜—ȱ ‘¢ȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ œ¢œŽ–œȱ Š›Žȱ ‘Žȱ Š¢ȱ ‘Ž¢ȱ Š›Žǯ

5. Conclusion

Grammatical relations constitute a core feature of the languages of the world and a richly studied topic within linguistic typology. One of the pos- sible strategies languages can turn to in order to codify grammatical rela- tions, case marking, leaves languages with several logical options for mor- phosyntactic alignment, all of which occur in the languages of the world. ‘Žȱ ’쎛Ž—ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ ¢™Žœȱ Š—ȱ Œ˜–‹’—Š’˜—œȱ ˜ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ œ¢œŽ–œȱ Œ›Ž- ŠŽȱ ’—›’ŒŠŽȱ ™ŠĴŽ›—œȱ ŠŒ›˜œœȱ •Š—žŠŽœȱ Š—ȱ ˜—Žȱ –’‘ȱ ˜—Ž›ȱ ‘Šȱ –˜- tivations languages could have for preferring certain alignment types over ˜‘Ž›œǯȱ ‘’œȱ ™Š™Ž›ȱ ‘˜™Žȱ ˜ȱ Œ˜—›’‹žŽȱ ˜ȱ ‘Žȱ ꎕȱ ˜ȱ •’—ž’œ’Œȱ ¢™˜•˜¢ȱ ‹¢ȱ˜ěŽ›’—ȱŠȱŒ•ŽŠ›ȱ˜ŸŽ›Ÿ’Ž ȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ’쎛Ž—ȱ¢™Žœȱ˜ȱŠ•’—–Ž—ȱŠ—ȱ™˜œœ’- ble explanations for their occurrence or prevalence in languages across the world. Through careful consideration of previous studies on a variety of lan- žŠŽœǰȱ Š—Š•¢œŽœȱ Š—ȱ Ž¡™•Š—Š’˜—œȱ ˜›ȱ ŽŸŽ›¢ȱ Š•’—–Ž—ȱ ¢™Žȱ Ž›Žȱ ˜ěŽ›Žǯ ‘ŽœŽȱŠ—Š•¢œŽœȱŠĴŽ–™Žȱ˜ȱ™›˜Ÿ’ŽȱŒ•ŽŠ›ȱŠ—œ Ž›œȱ˜ȱŸŽ›¢ȱ›Ž•ŽŸŠ—ȱšžŽœ- tions concerning grammatical relations, an important and interesting topic within linguistic typology. Most importantly, it was found that, resorting to the notions of economy, frequency and cognitive preference, explana- ’˜—œȱŒ˜ž•ȱ‹Žȱ˜ěŽ›Žȱ˜›ȱ‘Žȱ’쎛Ž—ȱŠ•’—–Ž—ȱœ¢œŽ–œȱ˜›ȱŒ˜–‹’—Š’˜—œȱ

20 RU:ts 1

˜ȱ œ¢œŽ–œȱ ˜ž—ȱ ŠŒ›˜œœȱ ‘Žȱ •Š—žŠŽœȱ ˜ȱ ‘Žȱ ˜›•ǰȱ ŽěŽŒ’ŸŽ•¢ȱ Š—œ Ž›’—ȱ some intriguing questions regarding the basic structure of human language.

References

’œœŽ—ǰȱ ǯȱǻŘŖŖřǼǯȱ’쎛Ž—’Š•ȱ˜‹“ŽŒȱ–Š›”’—DZȱ Œ˜—’Œ’¢ȱŸœǯȱŽŒ˜—˜–¢ǯȱNatural Language & Linguistic Theory, 21ǻřǼǰȱŚřśȬŚŞřǯ Ayer, M. (2014). ••Ž—ȱŠ—ȱ ›ŽŽ—˜ž‘ȂœȱŽ ȱŠ’—ȱ ›Š––Š›ȱ˜›ȱŒ‘˜˜•œȱŠ—ȱ˜••ŽŽœ. Carlisle: Dickinson College Commentaries. ’Œ”Ž•ǰȱǯǰȱǭȱ’Œ‘˜•œǰȱ ǯȱǻŘŖŖşǼǯȱŠœŽȱ–Š›”’—ȱŠ—ȱŠ•’—–Ž—ǯȱ‘Žȱ¡˜›ȱ‘Š—‹˜˜”ȱ˜ȱ case, 304-321. ž‹Ž—’”ǰȱǯȱǻŗşŞşǼǯȱ—ȱ’—Ž›™›ŽŠ’˜—ȱ˜ȱœ™•’ȱŽ›Š’Ÿ’¢ȱ’—ȱ —˜Ȭ ›Š—’Š—ȱ•Š—žŠŽœǯȱ Diachronica,6ǻŘǼǰȱŗŞŗȬŘŗŘǯ ˜–›’ŽǰȱǯȱǻŗşŝŞǼǯȱ›Š’Ÿ’¢ǯȱ —ȱǯȱǯȱŽ‘–Š——ȱǻŽǯǼǰȱ¢—ŠŒ’Œȱ¢™˜•˜¢DZȱœž’Žœȱ’—ȱ‘Žȱ phenomenology of language. Austin: University of Texas Press. Croft, W. (2003). Typology and universals. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer- sity Press. Dowty, D. R. (1991). Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language, 67(3), 547-619. žȱ˜’œǰȱ ǯȱǯȱǻŗşŞŝǼǯȱ‘Žȱ’œŒ˜ž›œŽȱ‹Šœ’œȱ˜ȱŽ›Š’Ÿ’¢ǯȱLanguage, ŜřǻŚǼǰȱŞŖśȬŞśśǯ Duranti, A. (2004). Agency in language. A companion to linguistic anthropology, 451- 473. ŸŽ›ŽĴǰȱǯȱǻŘŖŖşǼǯȱȱ›ŽŒ˜—œ’Ž›Š’˜—ȱ˜ȱ‘Žȱ–˜’ŸŠ’˜—œȱ˜›ȱ™›ŽŽ››ŽȱŠ›ž–Ž—ȱœ›žŒ- ture. ž’Žœȱ’—ȱŠ—žŠŽ, řř(1), 1-24. Šž•‘Š‹Ž›ǰȱǯȱǻŘŖŗŗǼǯȱŽ›‹ȱŸŠ•Ž—Œ¢ȱ™ŠĴŽ›—œDZȱȱŒ‘Š••Ž—Žȱ˜›ȱœŽ–Š—’ŒœȬ‹ŠœŽȱŠŒŒ˜ž—œ (Vol. 71). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. ˜Š›ǰȱǯȱǻŗşŞśǼǯȱȱ›Š––Š›ȱ˜ȱŠ—”ž—¢“Š“Š›Š. Alice Springs: Institute for Aborigi- nal Development. King, A. R. (1994). ‘ŽȱŠœšžŽȱ•Š—žŠŽDZȱȱ™›ŠŒ’ŒŠ•ȱ’—›˜žŒ’˜—. Reno: University of Nevada Press. ’ŒŽ›Šœǰȱ ǯȱǯǰȱǭȱNJ£ǰȱǯȱǻŗşşşǼǯȱ˜™’ŒȬ›˜™ȱŸŽ›œžœȱ™›˜Ȭ›˜™DZȱ—ž••ȱœž‹“ŽŒœȱŠ—ȱ ™›˜—˜–’—Š•ȱœž‹“ŽŒœȱ’—ȱ‘Žȱ™Š—’œ‘ȱŘȱ˜ȱ‘’—ŽœŽǰȱ—•’œ‘ǰȱ›Ž—Œ‘ǰȱ Ž›–Š—ȱŠ—ȱ Japanese speakers. ŽŒ˜—ȱ•Š—žŠŽȱ›ŽœŽŠ›Œ‘, 15(1), 1-40. Š•Œ‘ž”˜ŸǰȱǯȱǯǰȱǭȱŽȱ ˜˜™ǰȱ ǯȱǻŘŖŗŗǼǯȱŽ—œŽǰȱŠœ™ŽŒǰȱŠ—ȱ–˜˜ȱ‹ŠœŽȱ’쎛Ž—’Š•ȱ case marking. Lingua, 121(1), 35-47. Š•Œ‘ž”˜ŸǰȱǯȱǯǰȱŠ Šǰȱǯǰȱǭȱ’Ž ’Ž›œ”ŠǰȱǯȱǻŘŖŗŗǼǯȱ˜ Š›œȱŠȱ¢™˜•˜¢ȱ˜ȱ’–™Ž›- sonal constructions. Impersonal Constructions, 19-56. Š£ž”Šǰȱǯǰȱǭȱ ˜‘ǰȱ ǯȱǻŗşşśǼǯȱŠ—ȱ Š™Š—ŽœŽȱœ™ŽŠ”Ž›œȱ‹Žȱ•Žȱ˜ —ȱ‘ŽȱŠ›Ž—ȱ path. Japanese sentence processing, 295-329. ’Œ‘˜•œǰȱ ǯȱǻŗşŞŜǼǯȱ ŽŠȬ–Š›”’—ȱŠ—ȱŽ™Ž—Ž—Ȭ–Š›”’—ȱ›Š––Š›ǯȱLanguage, 56- 119. ’Œ‘˜•œǰȱ ǯȱǻŗşşřǼǯȱ›Š’Ÿ’¢ȱŠ—ȱ•’—ž’œ’ŒȱŽ˜›Š™‘¢ǯȱAustralian Journal of Linguis- tics, ŗřǻŗǼǰȱřşȬŞşǯ Payne, J. R. (2002). Language universals and language types. In An Encyclopedia of Language (pp. 166-195). Abingdon-on-Thames: Routledge. ˜‹’—œ˜—ǰȱǯȱǻŘŖŖŞǼǯȱ›ŽŽ–Ž—ȱ’—ȱŒ˜Ĵ’œ‘ȱ ŠŽ•’Œ (Doctoral dissertation, University College Dublin).

21 RU:ts 1

žŽǰȱǯȱǻŗşŞŜǼǯȱ˜™’ŒŠ•’¢ǰȱ›Š—œ’’Ÿ’¢ǰȱŠ—ȱ‘Žȱ’›ŽŒȱ˜‹“ŽŒȱ’—ȱŽ£ȱŽ›ŒŽǯȱInterna- tional Journal of American Linguistics, 52(2), 124-153. Sinnemäki, K. (2010). Word order in zero-marking languages. ž’Žœȱ’—ȱŠ—žŠŽǯȱ International Journal sponsored by the Foundation “Foundations of Language”, řŚ(4), ŞŜşȬşŗŘǯ Song, J. J. (2014). ’—ž’œ’Œȱ¢™˜•˜¢DZȱ˜›™‘˜•˜¢ȱŠ—ȱœ¢—Š¡. Abingdon-on-Thames: Routledge. œž—˜ŠǰȱǯȱǻŗşŞŗǼǯȱ™•’ȱŒŠœŽȬ–Š›”’—ȱ™ŠĴŽ›—œȱ’—ȱŸŽ›‹Ȭ¢™ŽœȱŠ—ȱŽ—œŽȦŠœ™ŽŒȦ–˜˜ǯȱ Linguistics, 19ǻśȬŜǼǰȱřŞşȬŚřŞǯ Š—ȱŸŽ›‹›˜ŽŒ”ǰȱǯȱǻŘŖŖřǼǯȱŠ—žŠŽȱ¢™Žȱ›ŽšžŽ—Œ¢ȱŠ—ȱ•ŽŠ›—Š‹’•’¢ȱ›˜–ȱŠȱŒ˜——ŽŒ- tionist perspective. Linguistic Typology, 7(1), 1-50. Š—ȱ Ž•Ž›Ž—ǰȱǯȱǻŘŖŖŚǼǯȱGrammaticalization as economy (Vol. 71). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. ’••’Š–œǰȱǯȱǻŗşŞŗǼǯȱ›ž–Ž—ȱœ›žŒž›ŽȱŠ—ȱ–˜›™‘˜•˜¢ǯȱThe linguistic review, 1(1), ŞŗȬŗŗŚǯ

Glossing abbreviations:

ŗȱƽȱꛜȱ™Ž›œ˜— 2 = second person 3 = third person юѐѐ = accusative case юѠѝ = aspect ђџє = ergative case іћѡџ = intransitive ћќњ = nominative case ѝђџѓ = perfect tense ѝѓѣ = perfective aspect ѝѠѡ = past tense Ѡє = singular ѡџ = transitive

22