University of California Santa Cruz Case, Agreement, And

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

University of California Santa Cruz Case, Agreement, And UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA SANTA CRUZ CASE, AGREEMENT, AND SENTENCE PROCESSING IN GEORGIAN A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in LINGUISTICS by Steven Foley June 2020 The Dissertation of Steven Foley is approved: Professor Matt Wagers Professor Sandy Chung Professor Amanda Rysling Professor Maziar Toosarvandani Quentin Williams Acting Vice Provost and Dean of Graduate Studies Copyright © by Steven Foley 2020 Table of Contents List of Figures vi List of Tables viii Abstract x Dedication xii Acknowledgments xiii 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Overview . 1 1.2 A snapshot of Georgian morphology & syntax . 9 1.3 Clarifications and caveats . 13 1.3.1 Terminological notes . 13 1.3.2 Glossing and transliteration conventions . 17 1.3.3 Data transparency . 18 2 Harmonizing animacy & Split Ergativity 23 2.1 Case alignment and incremental processing . 23 2.2 Case alignment in Georgian . 31 2.3 Experiment 1a: Humans in Transitive Clauses . 40 2.3.1 Method . 40 2.3.2 Results . 44 2.3.3 Discussion . 48 2.4 Experiment 1b: Inanimates in Transitive Clauses . 53 2.4.1 Method . 54 2.4.2 Results . 56 2.4.3 Discussion . 59 2.5 General discussion . 63 iii 3 Processing relative & correlative clauses 66 3.1 Introduction . 66 3.2 Previous research . 74 3.2.1 Prenominal relative clauses and the subject gap advantage . 75 3.2.2 The Subject-Gap Advantage in Ergative languages . 80 3.3 Background on Georgian . 89 3.3.1 Case alignment . 90 3.3.2 Case processing in Georgian . 91 3.3.3 Relativization strategies . 93 3.3.4 Predictions . 100 3.4 Experiment 2a: Processing relatives & correlatives with rom ........ 105 3.4.1 Method . 105 3.4.2 Results . 109 3.4.3 Discussion . 109 3.5 Experiment 2b: Processing more (cor)relatives with rom .......... 113 3.5.1 Method . 113 3.5.2 Results . 116 3.5.3 Discussion . 117 3.6 Experiment 3a: Processing wh-relatives . 118 3.6.1 Method . 118 3.6.2 Results . 122 3.6.3 Discussion . 124 3.7 Experiment 3b: Processing more wh-relatives . 125 3.7.1 Method . 125 3.7.2 Results . 128 3.7.3 Discussion . 129 3.8 General discussion . 133 4 Balancing subject and object agreement 137 4.1 Introduction . 137 4.2 South Caucasian agreement . 143 4.3 Accounting for the generalizations . 152 4.3.1 Accounting for tense–aspect–mood suffixes . 153 4.3.2 Accounting for plural suffixes . 156 4.3.3 Accounting for prefixal agreement . 157 4.3.4 Another look at dative-subject constructions . 161 4.4 Filtration and Minimal Compliance . 170 4.4.1 Motivating Postsyntactic Filtration . 171 4.4.2 Motivating the Principle of Minimal Compliance . 177 4.4.3 The Analysis Illustrated . 185 4.5 Conclusion . 194 iv 5 Conclusion 198 5.1 Summary of key findings & conclusions . 198 5.2 Directions for future research . 201 v List of Figures 2.1 Architecture of eADM . 28 2.2 Reading times (Experiment 1a) . 44 2.3 Reading times (Experiment 1b) . 57 3.1 Incremental gap disambiguation in a wh-relative . 102 3.2 Reading times (Experiment 2a) . 110 3.3 Reading times (Experiment 2b) . 117 3.4 Reading times (Experiment 3a) . 123 3.5 Reading times (Experiment 3b) . 130 3.6 Effect of animacy on RTs (Experiment 3b) . 132 4.1 The South Caucasian verbal template . 143 4.2 Deriving 1≫2 agreement . 186 4.3 Deriving 2.dat≫1 agreement . 188 4.4 Deriving 1≫3 agreement . 190 4.5 Deriving 3≫2pl agreement . 192 vi 4.6 Deriving 3≫1pl agreement . 193 vii List of Tables 1.1 Transliteration scheme . 19 2.1 Georgian case alignment . 37 2.2 RT model for the NP2 region (Experiment 1a) . 45 2.3 RT model for the verb region (Experiment 1a) . 46 2.4 RT model for the first spillover region (Experiment 1a) . 46 2.5 RT model for the verb region; data culled (Experiment 1a) . 47 2.6 RT model for the verb region; special contrast coding (Experiment 1a) . 48 2.7 Comprehension data (Experiment 1a) . 49 2.8 RT model for the verb region (Experiment 1b) . 57 2.9 RT model for the NP1 region; special contrast coding (Experiment 1b) . 58 2.10 RT model for the verb region; data culled (Experiment 1b) . 59 2.11 Comprehension data (Experiment 1b) . 59 3.1 Predictions of three theories of the SGA . 72 3.2 Monotransitive case frames in Georgian . 92 viii 3.3 RT model for the coargument region (Experiment 2a) . 110 3.4 RT model for the verb region (Experiment 2a) . 111 3.5 Reading times (Experiment 2b) . 118 3.6 RT model for the adjective region (Experiment 3a) . 122 3.7 RT model for the verb region (Experiment 3a) . 124 3.8 RT model for the verb region (Experiment 3b) . 129 3.9 Effect of animacy on RTs at the verb (Experiment 3b) . 131 3.10 Effect of animacy on RTs at the head noun (Experiment 3b) . 132 ix Abstract Case, agreement, and sentence processing in Georgian by Steven Foley This dissertation examines a few dimensions of morphosyntactic complexity in Georgian. Central are the language’s split-ergative case system, whereby clausal arguments are assigned different case morphology across different tense–aspect–mood categories, and its verbal agreement paradigm, in which j-agreement morphemes interact in complex but systematic ways. Three pairs of self-paced reading experiments probe the ramifications of Georgian split ergativity for online sentence processing, in ordinary transitive root clauses and also relative clauses. The Georgian comprehender is often faced with arguments whose case morphology does not unambiguously indicate their syntactic roles. Results show that comprehenders navigate these temporary ambiguities by harmonically aligning animacy and syntactic role — all else being equal, human arguments are parsed as transitive subjects, and inanimates as direct objects. Interpreted as garden path effects, the specific distribution of reading-time disruptions add nuance to this parsing heuristic, suggesting that comprehenders have fine-grained by not unlimited access to their abstract grammatical knowledge. Moving to relative clauses, I find that priorities are subtly readjusted during the comprehension of filler–gap dependencies. With both Accusative- and Ergative-aligned relative clauses that can either precede or follow their head nouns, Georgian is uniquely well equipped to disentangle theories of filler–gap processing. And in relative clauses of all x stripes, cues that eliminate the possibility of a subject-gap parse regularly lead to process- ing difficulty. This observation lends support to theories in which the structural distance between fillers and gaps — rather than the linear distance, or the informativity of ambient morphological cues — is the primary predictor of relative-clause processing difficulty. The thesis is rounded off with a detailed formal investigation of argument–verb agreement in Georgian and its sibling languages. I identify a few generalizations that re- veal key systematicities within superficially complex paradigms. These motivate an analy- sis deriving agreement patterns from the interaction of narrow-syntactic and post-syntactic mechanisms. Specifically, a syntactic principle independently motivated by non-agreement phenomena in other languages permits syntactic locality constraints to be loosened in cer- tain circumstances; resulting derivational indeterminacy is obscured by morphological con- straints that filter out all but the most expressive and economical combinations of agreement morphemes. xi To Jim Foley xii Acknowledgments Without the help, friendship, and boundless generosity of Marina Rcheulishvili, Ana Chutkerashvili, Elene Kadagishvili, Mariam Matiashvili, Mariam Rukhadze, Ana Kol- khidashvili, Salome Kobalia, Natia Botkoveli, and especially Irma Miminoshvili & Mariam Navadze, I could not have conducted my research on Georgian. Gmadlobt, da gagimarǰot. I will always be grateful to the people of Santiago Laxopa, who welcomed me into their community, shared their beautiful language, and ensured that I didn’t die of appendicitis in the mountains of northern Oaxaca. They include Fe Esperanza Silva Robles, Raquel Díaz, Vianey López, Olivia Maldonado, Amelia Vasquez Reyes, Sara Reyes, Venustiano López, Evaristo López Velázquez, Nereida Robles Vásquez, Raúl Díaz, and the presidents of Laxopa in the summers of 2018 and 2019, Celestino Robles Ramírez and Óscar Benites Celis. Duxklenle. Finally, I wish to acknowledge that land where I lived and worked during gradu- ate school is the unceded territory of the Awaswas-speaking Uypi Tribe. The Amah Mut- sun Tribal Band, comprising the descendants of Indigenous people taken to missions Santa Cruz and San Juan Bautista during Spanish colonization of the Central Coast, is today work- ing hard to restore traditional stewardship practices on these lands and heal from historical trauma. xiii Chapter 1 Introduction 1.1 Overview This thesis investigates a set of interconnected questions about the mental repre- sentation of morphological and syntactic grammars. The meat of that investigation com- prises a set of self-paced reading experiments and an in-depth formal analysis. Engaging primarily with one particular language, Georgian, these studies focus on three phenomena prominent in its syntax and morphosyntax — Split-Ergative case alignment, relativization, and argument–verb j-agreement. The experiments interface most readily with theories of syntactic processing, and the formal analysis with theories of abstract grammar. Together, I hope they provide some insight into the psycholinguistic and formal consequences mor- phosyntactic complexity, both in a single language and more generally. The primary goals and findings of these projects are summarized in the rest of this subsection. Chapter 2 investigates the role animacy plays in the online comprehension of 1 temporary syntactic ambiguities, using as a lens Georgian’s Split Ergativity. Consider a verb-final language — or more precisely, a language where verb-final clauses predominate, preverbal argument order is flexible, and argument omission is frequent.
Recommended publications
  • The Strategy of Case-Marking
    Case marking strategies Helen de Hoop & Andrej Malchukov1 Radboud University Nijmegen DRAFT January 2006 Abstract Two strategies of case marking in natural languages are discussed. These are defined as two violable constraints whose effects are shown to converge in the case of differential object marking but diverge in the case of differential subject marking. The strength of the case bearing arguments will be shown to be of utmost importance for case marking as well as voice alternations. The strength of arguments can be viewed as a function of their discourse prominence. The analysis of the case marking patterns we find cross-linguistically is couched in a bidirectional OT analysis. 1. Assumptions In this section we wish to put forward our three basic assumptions: (1) In ergative-absolutive systems ergative case is assigned to the first argument x of a two-place relation R(x,y). (2) In nominative-accusative systems accusative case is assigned to the second argument y of a two-place relation R(x,y). (3) Morphologically unmarked case can be the absence of case. The first two assumptions deal with the linking between the first (highest) and second (lowest) argument in a transitive sentence and the type of case marking. For reasons of convenience, we will refer to these arguments quite sloppily as the subject and the object respectively, although we are aware of the fact that the labels subject and object may not be appropriate in all contexts, dependent on how they are actually defined. In many languages, ergative and accusative case are assigned only or mainly in transitive sentences, while in intransitive sentences ergative and accusative case are usually not assigned.
    [Show full text]
  • A Crosslinguistic Approach to Double Nominative and Biabsolutive Constructions
    A Crosslinguistic Approach to Double Nominative and Biabsolutive Constructions: Evidence from Korean and Daghestanian∗ Andrei Antonenko1 and Jisung Sun2 Stony Brook University1,2 1. Introduction Distribution of case among distinct grammatical relations is one of the most frequently studied topics in the syntactic theory. Canonical cases are, in accusative languages, subjects of both intransitive and transitive verbs being nominative, while direct objects of transitive verbs are usually marked accusative. In ergative languages, subjects of intransitive verbs share properties with direct objects of transitive verbs, and are marked absolutive. Subjects of transitive verbs are usually ergative. When you look into world languages, however, there are ‘non-canonical’ case patterns too. Probably the most extreme kind of non-canonical case system would be so-called Quirky Subject constructions in Icelandic (see Sigurðsson 2002). This paper concerns constructions, in which two nominals are identically case-marked in a clause, as observed in Korean and Daghestanian languages. Daghestanian languages belong to Nakh-Daghestanian branch of North Caucasian family. Nakh-Daghestanian languages are informally divided into Nakh languages, such as Chechen and Ingush, spoken in Chechnya and the Republic of Ingushetia, respectively; and Daghestanian languages, spoken in the Republic of Daghestan. Those regions are located in the Caucasian part of Russian Federation. Some Daghestanian languages are also spoken in Azerbaijan and Georgia. This study focuses on Daghestanian languages, such as Archi, Avar, Dargwa, Hinuq, Khwarshi, Lak and Tsez, due to similar behaviors of them with respect to the described phenomenon. 2. Ergativity in Daghestanian Aldridge (2004) proposes that there are two types of syntactically ergative languages, based on which argument is performing functions typical for subjects.
    [Show full text]
  • The Progressive and Split Ergativity: the Case of Basque
    In Johns, A. Massam, D. & Ndayiragije J. (Eds.) Ergativity.Emerging Issues, Dordrecht: Springer, pp. 173-196 ITZIAR LAKA† DERIVING SPLIT ERGATIVITY IN THE PROGRESSIVE* The Case of Basque 1. INTRODUCTION: THE PHENOMENON, THE ACCOUNT AND SOME CONSEQUENCES. This paper explores the relationship between Aspect and case in ergative grammars, and the syntactic structure of sentences with a progressive meaning. It suggests an explanation for aspectually driven split-ergativity phenomena, based on an account of progressive sentences in Basque. The contrast between canonical transitive sentences and their progressive equivalents found in Basque is shown in (1), where glosses are deliberately vague for the moment: 1 1) a. emakume-a-k ogi-a jaten du woman-DET-E bread-DET eating has ‘The woman eats (the) bread’ b. emakume-a ogi-a jaten ari da woman-DET bread-DET eating PROG is ‘The woman is eating (the) bread’ As we can see by comparing (1a) and (1b), a change to progressive aspect induces a change in case-assignment, and a change of inflected auxiliary. (1a) is an example of an imperfective transitive sentence, where the external argument emakumea ‘the woman’ carries ergative case (morpheme –k). The internal argument ogia ‘(the) bread’ receives absolutive case, marked zero2. In contrast, (1b) has no ergative-marked argument, despite the fact that there is an event of eating, whose agent is the woman. The progressive marker ari has an effect on the case assigned to the subject: it does not show ergative case; it is assigned absolutive/zero case. The pair in (1) is reminiscent of similar phenomena in other ergative languages, where aspectual variations induce changes in the case assigned to the transitive † University of the Basque Country-Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea; [email protected] 173 A.
    [Show full text]
  • Dative (First) Complements in Basque
    Dative (first) complements in Basque BEATRIZ FERNÁNDEZ JON ORTIZ DE URBINA Abstract This article examines dative complements of unergative verbs in Basque, i.e., dative arguments of morphologically “transitive” verbs, which, unlike ditransitives, do not co-occur with a canonical object complement. We will claim that such arguments fall under two different types, each of which involves a different type of non-structural licensing of the dative case. The presence of two different types of dative case in these constructions is correlated with the two different types of complement case alternations which many of these predicates exhibit, so that alternation patterns will provide us with clues to identify different sources for the dative marking. In particular, we will examine datives alternating with absolutives (i.e., with the regular object structural case in an ergative language) and datives alternating with postpositional phrases. We will first examine an approach to the former which relies on current proposals that identify a low applicative head as case licenser. Such approach, while accounting for the dative case, raises a number of issues with respect to the absolutive variant. As for datives alternating with postpositional phrases, we claim that they are lexically licensed by the lower verbal head V. Keywords Dative, conflation, lexical case, inherent case, case alternations 1. Preliminaries: bivalent unergatives Bivalent unergatives, i.e., unergatives with a dative complement, have remained largely ignored in traditional Basque studies, perhaps due to the Journal of Portuguese Linguistics, 11-1 (2012), 83-98 ISSN 1645-4537 84 Beatriz Fernández & Jon Ortiz de Urbina identity of their morphological patterns of case marking and agreement with those of ditransitive configurations.
    [Show full text]
  • How Do Young Children Acquire Case Marking?
    INVESTIGATING FINNISH-SPEAKING CHILDREN’S NOUN MORPHOLOGY: HOW DO YOUNG CHILDREN ACQUIRE CASE MARKING? Thesis submitted to the University of Manchester for the degree of Doctor in Philosophy in the Faculty of Medical and Human Sciences 2015 HENNA PAULIINA LEMETYINEN SCHOOL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES 2 Table of Contents LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................................... 6 LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................................................... 7 ABSTRACT ................................................................................................................................. 8 DECLARATION .......................................................................................................................... 9 COPYRIGHT STATEMENT .......................................................................................................... 9 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .......................................................................................................... 10 Chapter 1: General introduction to language acquisition research ...................................... 11 1.1. Generativist approaches to child language............................................................ 11 1.2. Usage-based approaches to child language........................................................... 14 1.3. The acquisition of morphology .............................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Morphological Causatives Are Voice Over Voice
    Morphological causatives are Voice over Voice Yining Nie New York University Abstract Causative morphology has been associated with either the introduction of an event of causation or the introduction of a causer argument. However, morphological causatives are mono-eventive, casting doubt on the notion that causatives fundamentally add a causing event. On the other hand, in some languages the causative morpheme is closer to the verb root than would be expected if the causative head is responsible for introducing the causer. Drawing on evidence primarily from Tagalog and Halkomelem, I argue that the syntactic configuration for morphological causatives involves Voice over Voice, and that languages differ in whether their ‘causative marker’ spells out the higher Voice, the lower Voice or both. Keywords: causative, Voice, argument structure, morpheme order, typology, Tagalog 1. Introduction Syntactic approaches to causatives generally fall into one of two camps. The first view builds on the discovery that causatives may semantically consist of multiple (sub)events (Jackendoff 1972, Dowty 1979, Parsons 1990, Levin & Rappaport Hovav 1994, a.o.). Consider the following English causative–anticausative pair. The anticausative in (1a) consists of an event of change of state, schematised in (1b). The causative in (2a) involves the same change of state plus an additional layer of semantics that conveys how that change of state is brought about (2b). (1) a. The stick broke. b. [ BECOME [ stick STATE(broken) ]] (2) a. Pat broke the stick. b. [ Pat CAUSE [ BECOME [ stick STATE(broken) ]]] Word Structure 13.1 (2020): 102–126 DOI: 10.3366/word.2020.0161 © Edinburgh University Press www.euppublishing.com/word MORPHOLOGICAL CAUSATIVES ARE VOICE OVER VOICE 103 Several linguists have proposed that the semantic CAUSE and BECOME components of the causative are encoded as independent lexical verbal heads in the syntax (Harley 1995, Cuervo 2003, Folli & Harley 2005, Pylkkänen 2008, a.o.).
    [Show full text]
  • Materials of the Riga 3Rd International Conference on Hellenic Studies
    Materials of the Riga 3rd International Conference on Hellenic Studies Latvijas Universitāte Humanitāro zinātņu fakultāte Klasiskās filoloģijas katedra Hellēnistikas centrs HELLĒŅU DIMENSIJA Rīgas 3. starptautiskās hellēnistikas konferences materiāli Sastādītāji: Brigita Aleksejeva Ojārs Lāms Ilze Rūmniece Latvijas Universitāte University of Latvia Faculty of Humanities Chair of Classical Philology Centre for Hellenic Studies HELLENIC DIMENSION Materials of the Riga 3rd International Conference on Hellenic Studies Editors: Brigita Aleksejeva Ojārs Lāms Ilze Rūmniece University of Latvia UDK 930(063) He 396 The book is financially supported by the Hellenic Republic Ministry of Culture and Tourism and the University of Latvia Grāmata izdota ar Grieķijas Republikas Kultūras un tūrisma ministrijas un Latvijas Universitātes atbalstu Support for Conference Proceedings by ERAF Project Support for the international cooperation projects and other international cooperation activities in research and technology at the University of Latvia No. 2010/0202/2DP/2.1.1.2.0/10/APIA/VIAA/013 IEGULDĪJUMS TAVĀ NĀKOTNĒ Editorial board: Gunnar de Boel (Belgium) Igor Surikov (Russia) Thanassis Agathos (Greece) Kateřina Loudová (The Czech Republic) Valda Čakare (Latvia) Ojārs Lāms (Latvia) Ilze Rūmniece (Latvia) Nijolė Juchnevičienė (Lithuania) Tudor Dinu (Romania) Language editing Normunds Titāns Translating Rasma Mozere Cover design: Agris Dzilna Layout: Andra Liepiņa © Brigita Aleksejeva, Ojārs Lāms, Ilze Rūmniece, editors, 2012 © University of Latvia, 2012 ISBN 978-9984-45-469-6 CONTENTS / SATURS Introduction 8 Ievads 10 I ANCIENT TIMES SENLAIKI 11 Vassilis Patronis ECONOMIC IDEAS OF ANCIENT GREEK PHILOSOPHERS: ASSESSING THEIR IMPACT ON THE FORMATION OF THE WORLD ECONOMIC THOUGHT 12 Sengrieķu filozofu idejas par ekonomiku: izvērtējot ietekmi uz pasaules ekonomiskās domas veidošanos Nijolė Juchnevičienė HISTORIOGRAPHIC SCIENTIFIC DISCOURSE AND THE TRADITION OF GEOGRAPHY 22 Zinātniski historiogrāfiskais diskurss un ģeogrāfijas tradīcija Igor E.
    [Show full text]
  • Ergative As Accusative Case: Evidence from Adiyaman
    ERGATIVE AS ACCUSATIVE CASE: EVIDENCE FROM ADIYAMAN KURMANJI ÜMİT ATLAMAZ ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! BOĞAZİÇİ UNIVERSITY 2012 ERGATIVE AS ACCUSATIVE CASE: EVIDENCE FROM ADIYAMAN KURMANJI Thesis submitted to the Institute for Graduate Studies in the Social Sciences in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Masters of Arts in Linguistics by Ümit Atlamaz ! ! Boğaziçi University 2012 ! ! Thesis Abstract Ümit Atlamaz, “Ergative as Accusative Case: Evidence from Adıyaman Kurmanji” This study aims to investigate the nature of ergativity in Adıyaman Kurmanji within the premises of the Minimalist Program. Adıyaman Kurmanji displays two alignment patterns depending on the tense. In non-past structures nominative alignment is observed whereas the past tense requires an ergative alignment. Based on these two types of alignments many linguists like Haig (2004), Thackston (2006), and Gündoğdu (2011) argue that Kurmanji is a split ergative language. Accordingly, the major aim of this study is to investigate the structure of the ergative pattern in Adıyaman Kurmanji. In this study, the initial step was to compare the ergative and nominative subjects in terms of certain tests like binding, scope and EPP to determine the phrase structure and where the subjects reside on the structure. Additionally, voice properties of the language were inspected as a background to the major claim. Based on the results of the tests applied and the motivation obtained from the data, it was argued that what has been called ergative in Adıyaman Kurmanji is, indeed, a passive structure diachronically reanalyzed as the past tense. According to Trask’s (1979) typology of ergative languages, there are two types of ergative languages, which labels as Type A and Type B.
    [Show full text]
  • Verb Agreement and Case Marking in Burushaski
    Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session Volume 40 Article 5 1996 Verb agreement and case marking in Burushaski Stephen R. Willson SIL-UND Follow this and additional works at: https://commons.und.edu/sil-work-papers Part of the Linguistics Commons Recommended Citation Willson, Stephen R. (1996) "Verb agreement and case marking in Burushaski," Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session: Vol. 40 , Article 5. DOI: 10.31356/silwp.vol40.05 Available at: https://commons.und.edu/sil-work-papers/vol40/iss1/5 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by UND Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Work Papers of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of North Dakota Session by an authorized editor of UND Scholarly Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Verb Agreement and Case Marking in Burushaski Stephen R. Willson 1 Burushaski verb agreement and case marking phenomena are complex and have not been described adequately by any current theory ofsyntax. In particular, no explanation has yet been given as to why a variety of nominals can trigger agreement in the verbal prefix. In some cases the apparent subject triggers this agreement, in others the direct object appears to do so, in others the indirect object, in others the possessor of the direct object, in others a benefactive or source nominal. Also, the constraints on the usage of ergative, absolutive and oblique case, and other indicators ofgrammatical relations on nominals, have been insufficiently characterized in the literature on Burushaski.
    [Show full text]
  • Lesson 7 | Ntgreek in Session
    Second Declension Nouns (Module B) Feminine and Neuter Nouns 7 Morphology: The Nominal System (Part 2) Overview 7.0 Introduction, 7-1 7.1 Second Declension Feminine Nouns, 7-2 7.2 Second Declension Neuter Nouns, 7-7 7.3 Second Declension Paradigm Overview, 7-13 7.4 Review of Second Declension Nouns, 7-14 7.5 Flow Chart For Second Declension Nouns, 7-21 7.6 NTGreek Language Study Tools, 7-22 7.7 Vocabulary Study, 7-23 Study Guide, 7-25 7.0 Introduction The acquisition of a new language is difficult. This difficulty is compounded if it is not heard and spoken on a regular basis. Because of these special challenges, it will take repeated exposures to new elements of NTGreek grammar before it is understood. Make no mistake about it; acquiring a new language and the skills required is a daunting task! Any grammar positing anything different is selling something. Many enthusiastic students soon realize they are swimming—if not perhaps drowning—in information. Out of frustration, they blame themselves and think they are not intelligent enough or too old to tackle Greek. What they do not realize is that this is the natural learning process. Babies are at first engulfed in a sea of meaningless noise before they gradually learn to detect and recognize meaningful sounds as words. Instead of frustration, the proper response is to continue to be enthusiastic and inquisitive about what you are learning just like a child! © 1996 – 2014 by William Ramey The Nominal System (Part 2) NTGreek In Session Lesson 7: The Second Declension (Module B) 7-2 Feminine and Neuter Nouns ________________________________________________________________ Those studying NTGreek on their own may become unsure what they are learning, especially when there is no authoritative source in their immediate vicinity to ask questions and gain needed reassurance that they are on the right path.
    [Show full text]
  • SPLIT-ERGATIVITY in HITTITE Petra Goedegebuure (University of Chicago)
    Published in: Zeitschrift für Assyriologie und vorderasiatische Archäologie. Volume 102, Issue 2, Pages 270–303, ISSN (Online) 1613-1150, ISSN (Print) 0084-5299, DOI: 10.1515/za- 2012-0015, January 2013 1 SPLIT-ERGATIVITY IN HITTITE Petra Goedegebuure (University of Chicago) “it is possible that all languages show ergativity on some level” (McGregor 2009, 482) 1. Introduction2 As a highly heterogeneous phenomenon ergativity remains a conundrum for linguistic theory. The ergative case has been treated as a structural case, an inherent/lexical case, or rather as a mix (Butt 2006). Split-ergativity is thought to arise as an epiphenomenon, as ‘collateral damage’ of diachronic change after reinterpretation of passive constructions with instrumentals (Dixon 1994) or through reanalysis of transitive null-subject clauses with inanimate instrumentals (Garrett 1990b). Alternatively, case assignment and therefore also split-ergativity ultimately depends on synchronic structural properties of the clause (Merchant 2006). It has been claimed that only 25% of the world’s languages shows ergativity (Van de Visser 2006), or that “all languages show ergativity on some level” (McGregor 2009, 482). Irrespective of the correct ratio, split-ergativity seems to be the norm among languages that show ergativity. When the ergative split is based on semantic features of noun phrases, it is generally assumed that animacy plays a major role. Silverstein (1976) has shown that pronouns and nouns can be hierarchically arranged based on semantic features such as person, number, or grammatical gender. The strength of this hierarchy is that if agent marking is attested for the first time at a certain point in the hierarchy, all nominals lower in the hierarchy will carry agent marking as well.
    [Show full text]
  • AN INTRODUCTORY GRAMMAR of OLD ENGLISH Medieval and Renaissance Texts and Studies
    AN INTRODUCTORY GRAMMAR OF OLD ENGLISH MEDievaL AND Renaissance Texts anD STUDies VOLUME 463 MRTS TEXTS FOR TEACHING VOLUme 8 An Introductory Grammar of Old English with an Anthology of Readings by R. D. Fulk Tempe, Arizona 2014 © Copyright 2020 R. D. Fulk This book was originally published in 2014 by the Arizona Center for Medieval and Renaissance Studies at Arizona State University, Tempe Arizona. When the book went out of print, the press kindly allowed the copyright to revert to the author, so that this corrected reprint could be made freely available as an Open Access book. TABLE OF CONTENTS PREFACE viii ABBREVIATIONS ix WORKS CITED xi I. GRAMMAR INTRODUCTION (§§1–8) 3 CHAP. I (§§9–24) Phonology and Orthography 8 CHAP. II (§§25–31) Grammatical Gender • Case Functions • Masculine a-Stems • Anglo-Frisian Brightening and Restoration of a 16 CHAP. III (§§32–8) Neuter a-Stems • Uses of Demonstratives • Dual-Case Prepositions • Strong and Weak Verbs • First and Second Person Pronouns 21 CHAP. IV (§§39–45) ō-Stems • Third Person and Reflexive Pronouns • Verbal Rection • Subjunctive Mood 26 CHAP. V (§§46–53) Weak Nouns • Tense and Aspect • Forms of bēon 31 CHAP. VI (§§54–8) Strong and Weak Adjectives • Infinitives 35 CHAP. VII (§§59–66) Numerals • Demonstrative þēs • Breaking • Final Fricatives • Degemination • Impersonal Verbs 40 CHAP. VIII (§§67–72) West Germanic Consonant Gemination and Loss of j • wa-, wō-, ja-, and jō-Stem Nouns • Dipthongization by Initial Palatal Consonants 44 CHAP. IX (§§73–8) Proto-Germanic e before i and j • Front Mutation • hwā • Verb-Second Syntax 48 CHAP.
    [Show full text]