Florida State University Libraries

Electronic Theses, Treatises and Dissertations The Graduate School

2017 Redefining ADHD in an Adult Population: Should Inattention Be Viewed as a Separate Dimension from Cognitive and Physiological Activity Level? Nathan Andrew Miller

Follow this and additional works at the DigiNole: FSU's Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected] FLORIDA STATE UNIVERSITY

COLLEGE OF EDUCATION

REDEFINING ADHD IN AN ADULT POPULATION: SHOULD INATTENTION BE

VIEWED AS A SEPARATE DIMENSION FROM COGNITIVE AND PHYSIOLOGICAL

ACTIVITY LEVEL?

By

NATHAN ANDREW MILLER

A Dissertation submitted to the Department of Educational Psychology and Learning Systems in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

2017

© 2017 Nathan Andrew Miller Nathan Andrew Miller defended this dissertation on April 24, 2017. The members of the supervisory committee were:

Frances A. Prevatt Professor Directing Dissertation

Lee P. Stepina University Representative

Angela I. Canto Committee Member

S. Kathleen Krach Committee Member

The Graduate School has verified and approved the above-named committee members, and certifies that the dissertation has been approved in accordance with university requirements.

ii

Dedicated to Jess, my reason for all of this and for everything else.

iii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

No degree is ever earned nor dissertation ever written alone, and that is certainly the case here. I would like to make a permanent (even if rarely read) record of my gratitude for the many people who hold me aloft. To my wife who has been my best friend for ten years and never once showed any regret for the time spent, I say thank you. To my children, all five of you, who have challenged me to be a better man, a good student, and a motivated provider, I couldn’t have done this without your love and kindness. To my parents, who, despite the bitter teenage years (and let’s be honest the preteens as well as a few of the twenties) still feel duty-bound and love-bound to support and advise me, I say that it may still be a few years before I can pay you back with the job market as it is, but I will not give up trying. And thank you. I sincerely thank you. Dr. Prevatt, as my major professor you perhaps did more to bring about this document and the degree it goes with than I did. Seeing something in me that I rarely do, you contacted me, smoothed my path toward a fellowship, and counseled me when I struggled the most. You challenged my assumptions and weathered my stubbornness until we were both satisfied with this document, and I’m proud to have been your student. I am also grateful to those who have offered their valuable time to serve as members of my committee during the last few years: Dr. Angela Canto, Dr. Lee Stepina, Dr. Deborah Ebener, and Dr. Kathleen Krach; it was an honor to work with each of you. My friends from the program also deserve credit. Stasia, Rachel, Jen, and Sarah, I wanted to thank you for your kindness and for giving the Stone Building and friendlier face. I’m certain you are all destined for success, so I’m glad I knew you when we were just starting out as wee little grad students. Jordy and Megan, I already miss origami time. You kept me sane and made my hobby not seem so peculiar. I would be remiss if I neglected to thank my practicum supervisors as well: Dr. Swanbrow-Becker, Dr. Garis, Dr. Kubiak, Dr. Osborn, Dr. Wonder, Bob Carton and Melvina MacDonald (you both certainly deserve a place here in this list), and finally, last in chronology but first in impact, Dr. Anika Fields. Thank you all for trusting me with your good names and your offices, and for showing the passion you each have for your clients and your specializations. To these and many more go my thanks.

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

List of Tables ...... vii List of Figures ...... viii Abstract ...... ix CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION ...... 1 Significance ...... 7 Diagnostic Dissent ...... 10 DSM Incorporation ...... 10 Prevalence ...... 11 Study Benefits ...... 12 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW ...... 13 The ADHD and SCT Constructs ...... 13 Terms and Process ...... 13 History ...... 14 Construct Development ...... 18 Symptoms ...... 21 A Pattern of Heterogeneity in ADHD? ...... 23 SCT and ADHD-H/I ...... 23 SCT and ADHD-I ...... 24 SCT and ADHD-C ...... 24 Summary ...... 25 Onset and Development ...... 25 Comorbidity and Correlate Differences ...... 26 Possible Etiology ...... 31 Treatment ...... 36 Latent Structure Research of ADHD and SCT ...... 40 Summary of Evidence ...... 41 CHAPTER 3 METHODS ...... 44 Introduction ...... 44 Research Question ...... 44 Scale Development ...... 45 Procedures ...... 46 Participants ...... 48 Rational for Splitting the Sample ...... 49 Measures ...... 51 Activity Level ...... 51 Inattention ...... 51 Demographics ...... 51 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS ...... 53 Data Description/Statistical Assumptions ...... 53 Power ...... 53 Normality ...... 53 Hypothesis 1 ...... 54 Hypothesis 2 ...... 57

v

Outlier Characteristics ...... 59 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION ...... 61 Internal Consistency ...... 61 Curvilinearity ...... 63 Theoretical Implications ...... 65 New Model...... 65 Structure ...... 65 Categorization ...... 66 Clinical Implications ...... 67 Limitations ...... 70 Future Research ...... 71 APPENDICES ...... 73 A. STUDY SURVEY FORM ...... 73 B. TABLE OF SPECIFICATIONS ...... 81 C. HUMAN SUBJECTS APPROVAL ...... 82 References ...... 84 Biographical Sketch ...... 100

vi

LIST OF TABLES

1 Sample and US Population Demographic Comparison and Z-test Significance ...... 50

2 Full Sample and ADHD Sample Demographic Comparison and Z-test Significance ...... 50

3 Descriptive Statistics for Sample Groups ...... 54

4 Curve Estimation Sample Groupings...... 59

5 Comparison of Sample and Outlier Group Means ...... 60

vii

LIST OF FIGURES

1 Illustration of relationship between ADHD subtypes and SCT as presently constituted...... 2

2 Illustration of relationship between ADHD subtypes and SCT accounting for correlation between ADHD-I and SCT...... 2

3 Illustration of relationship between ADHD subtypes and SCT accounting for correlation between ADHD-I and SCT and between ADHD-H/I and ADHD-I...... 3

4 Illustration of relationship between ADHD subtypes and SCT accounting for correlation between ADHD-I and both SCT and ADHD-H/I, as well as the negative correlation between ADHD-H/I and SCT...... 3

5 Alternative illustration of Figure 4, including a continuum of Activity Level...... 5

6 Graph illustrating the proposed relationship between a continuum of activity level, from sluggish to hyperactive, and a measure of inattention...... 6

7 Illustration of proposed curvilinear relationship between inattention and activity level with demarcation for sluggishness-related inattention ...... 19

8 Linear illustration of correlations between externalizing behaviors and both SCT and ADHD- H/I, viewing the latter two constructs as endpoints of a continuum of activity level...... 29

9 Linear illustration of correlations between internalizing behaviors and both SCT and ADHD- H/I, viewing the latter two constructs as endpoints of a continuum of activity level ...... 29

10 Illustration of proposed curvilinear relationship between inattention and activity level, with theoretical demarcations encapsulating areas of function and dysfunction ...... 33

11 Illustration of inferred linear relationship between treatment outcomes and activity level based on correlational data. Results for three therapeutic approaches superimposed...... 39

12 Histogram of the Activity Level Frequency Distribution. Full Sample...... 55

13 Histogram of the Activity Level Frequency Distribution. ADHD Sample...... 55

14 Scatterplots and interpolation lines produced by curve estimation, illustrating the curvilinear relationship found in both sample groups between Inattention and Activity Level...... 60

viii

ABSTRACT

The accepted structure of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has changed repeatedly and significantly over its history, with many symptoms being added, changed or dropped. Despite this, many questions remain about the nature of the disorder and the potentially related set of symptoms known by most researchers as (SCT). This study sought to investigate the latent structure of ADHD and SCT by exploring the relationship between SCT, hyperactivity, and inattention as reported by adults. This study proposed that some of the structural issues found in ADHD may be due to the assumption of linearity, and proposed that two significant changes, if supported by the data, would offer some resolution. The first proposed change would be to view hyperactivity and sluggishness as a single continuum of activity level, rather than individual syndromes, and the second proposed change would be to view symptoms of inattention as a separate dimension from the hyperactivity and sluggishness. It was also proposed that evidence of the validity of this dimensional restructuring might be seen as a quadratic curvilinear, or U-shaped, regression line between inattention and the continuum of activity level, where inattention was highest toward the extremes of activity level and lowest at the midpoint of activity level.

For the current study, symptoms of hyperactivity and sluggishness were matched by topic to form a continuum from low levels to high levels of activity. This measure, along with a symptom checklist for ADHD inattention, was included in a survey and provided to a sample of

1,398 adults throughout the United States, collected through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk.

Participants demographics were similar to the population demographics with few variations.

Measures of internal scale consistency and of normality were used to analyze the new

Activity Level scale produced by matching hyperactivity and sluggishness, and curve estimation

ix was conducted to analyze whether a quadratic regression model was a significant predictor of the relationship between activity level and inattention.

Results of the analyses revealed that the new Activity Level scale was unimodal and within commonly accepted limits of internal consistency for both the full sample of participants and for the portion of the sample that endorsed a diagnosis of ADHD. Furthermore, results of the regression analysis indicated that a quadratic model of activity level and inattention accurately explained a small but significant portion of the variance in both the full sample of participants and the in the ADHD sample. Implications are discussed for the both theory and practice. Lastly, limitations of the study and directions for future research are included.

x

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

The goal of this study was to evaluate the relationship between attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT). Specifically, this study sought to determine if the construct of predominantly inattentive subtype of ADHD

(ADHD-I) represents two types of inattentive symptoms: one associated with hyperactivity, and one associated with cognitive sluggishness, which fall on the extremes of a distribution of physiological and cognitive activity level. While this manuscript adopts the terminology and definitions used by the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

(DSM-5, APA, 2013) in discussing ADHD, that text does not currently recognize SCT as a unique disorder. In such a case, the terminology and definitions have yet to be standardized, and will be adopted instead from the common parlance of the research literature.

According to the DSM-5, the inattention symptoms that define ADHD-I are often first seen during childhood and adolescence, are described as an inability to properly focus or avoid distraction, and cause deficits in the performance of tasks at school or at home (DSM-5, 2013).

Individuals diagnosed with ADHD-I may be described as “easily distracted,” may do poorly on assignments even if they understand the subject matter, and may be disorganized.

The symptoms of the hyperactive/impulsive subtype of ADHD (ADHD-H/I) are composed of behaviors that impede more context-appropriate behaviors at school, work, or home. These behaviors are thought to derive from an excess of energy or a lack of control

() over mental and physical energy (APA, 2013). In contrast, SCT is thought to impede performance in those areas because of a lack of energy (hypoactivity) or control in mental functioning (apathy; Barkley, 2012; Becker, Marshall, & McBurnett, 2014).

1

When using a Venn diagram to illustrate the presently accepted constructs of ADHD and

SCT, ADHD-I and ADHD-H/I would seem to overlap, forming the third subtype of ADHD-

Combined. SCT would be a distinct circle, apart from ADHD, as shown in Figure 1.

ADHD Combined

ADHD ADHD Inattentive SCT Hyperactive/ Impulsive

Figure 1. Illustration of relationship between ADHD subtypes and SCT as presently constituted.

However, ADHD-I and SCT seem to overlap as well, as they are strongly correlated

(Hartman, Willcutt, Rhee, & Pennington, 2004). That being the case, an adjustment might be made to the above Venn diagram to illustrate the relationship as shown in Figure 2.

ADHD Combined

ADHD SCT Hyperactive/ SCT Impulsive

ADHD Inattentive

Figure 2. Illustration of relationship between ADHD subtypes and SCT accounting for correlation between ADHD-I and SCT.

The relationship can be further illustrated by taking into account the prevalence of ADHD-

Combined type cases and the relativity rarity of purely ADHD-H/I cases, wherein no symptoms of ADHD-I are present. This can be illustrated as in Figure 3.

2

ADHD Combined

SCT SCT

ADHD Hyperactive/ Impulsive ADHD Inattentive

Figure 3. Illustration of relationship between ADHD subtypes and SCT accounting for correlation between ADHD-I and SCT and between ADHD-H/I and ADHD-I.

This third diagram would seem to imply that the ADHD-Inattentive type population would be almost entirely accounted for by the ADHD-H/I and the SCT populations, and that

ADHD-H/I and SCT should overlap significantly as well. This, however, is not the case. In fact,

ADHD-H/I and SCT appear to be negatively correlated (Penny et al., 2009). The Venn diagram would have to be changed significantly to account for this and for the relative prevalence of the subtypes, as illustrated in Figure 4.

ADHD Combined

SCT

ADHD Hyperactive/ Impulsive ADHD Inattentive

Figure 4. Illustration of relationship between ADHD subtypes and SCT accounting for correlation between ADHD-I and both SCT and ADHD-H/I, as well as the negative correlation between ADHD-H/I and SCT.

In this final diagram, it now appears that both ADHD-H/I and SCT may be subsets of the

ADHD-Inattentive type, meaning that both hyperactivity and sluggishness are forms of inattention. Alternatively, it may be that both ADHD-H/I and SCT (or hyperactivity and sluggishness) can cause symptoms of inattention.

3

As ADHD has been a disorder primarily diagnosed in childhood during much of the construct’s development, the symptom statements are designed to be overt characterizations which are visible to a teacher or parent, rather than exploring the internal experiences of the individual being rated. As such, they offer no clarification on whether the inattention can be perceived by the patient as being associated with either hyperactivity or with sluggishness. The following example makes the implications of this dilemma clearer. The nine symptoms of inattention are as follows (DSM 5; APA, 2013):

1. Fails to give close attention to details or makes careless mistakes.

2. Has difficulty sustaining attention.

3. Does not appear to listen.

4. Struggles to follow through on instructions.

5. Has difficulty with organization.

6. Avoids or dislikes tasks requiring a lot of thinking.

7. Loses things.

8. Is easily distracted.

9. Is forgetful in daily activities.

In reviewing these nine symptoms, it is possible to see how each might be involved with either a mental state of hyperactivity or a mental state of sluggishness. As an example of the first symptom above (fails to give close attention to details or makes careless mistakes), suppose that two students are given an assignment to complete, and one student struggles to focus because of a rush of thoughts about weekend plans or competing interests (i.e. hyperactivity), while the other student struggles to focus because of a general feeling of apathy and slowness of thought

(i.e. sluggishness). Although the explanations for their distraction and carelessness differ greatly

4 from each other, both would likely be rated as having the same symptom of inattention on a rating scale. In other words, the same outward behavior of failing to give close attention can be explained by two differing internal experiences.

This possible causal relationship might be better illustrated by a different method than a

Venn diagram, one which takes into account the symptom severity, as illustrated in Figure 5.

Inattention

Sluggishness Hyperactivity

Activity Level

Figure 5. Alternative illustration of figure 1.4, including a continuum of Activity Level.

This illustration places the hyperactivity of ADHD-H/I and the sluggishness of SCT on opposite ends of a continuum, labeled here as Activity Level, and increasing in symptom severity from the middle toward the endpoints. Viewed in this linear fashion, inattention is forced into two separate domains, one related to hyperactivity, and one related to sluggishness. While consistent with the research, this splitting of inattention might be described as an inelegant theoretical solution. This inelegance may be resolved by viewing these constructs in a multidimensional and curvilinear rather than a unidimensional and linear way. This is best illustrated in a similar fashion to the Yerkes-Dodson inverse-U-shaped curve (1908) (originally designed to describe the relationship between performance and arousal), but which now would describe the relationship between inattention and activity level, as illustrated in Figure 6.

As shown in Figure 6, ADHD may be better characterized as a relationship between two constructs: inattention and activity . In turn, activity can be described on a continuum from low activity (e.g., sluggishness) to high activity (e.g., hyperactivity). For example, someone with high

5 inattention might have very high symptoms of sluggishness OR very high symptoms of hyperactivity.

Figure 6. Graph illustrating the proposed relationship between a continuum of activity level, from sluggish to hyperactive, and a measure of inattention.

Alternately, someone with low inattention is likely neither sluggish nor hyperactive. This hypothesis poses a problem of finding the accurate terminology for describing this construct, as there is currently no universally recognized term. The ADHD terminology uses activity to describe the behavioral aspects of the disorder, but the cognitive symptoms of hyperactivity are merely inferred from those behaviors. SCT terminology uses sluggish tempo to describe the cognitive symptoms, and low energy to describe the behavioral deficit associated with the disorder. Arousal encompasses both aspects, but currently has many other definitions, and is perhaps too broad a term. In order to describe both the cognitive and behavioral aspects of the second construct being proposed, this paper uses the phrase cognitive and physiological activity level, or activity level when shortened. While not semantically perfect, it will do until a better

6 term is chosen. This paper will further hypothesize that the U-shape described by the Yerkes-

Dodson law of performance and arousal (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908) would similarly describe the quadratic curvilinear relationship between inattention and activity level. Specifically, activity level could be conceptualized as a predictive factor, on a continuum ranging from low activity level (similar to SCT) to high activity level (similar to hyperactivity). Activity level would be expected to predict inattention (ranging from low inattention to high inattention). The relationship would be expected to be curvilinear, such that either low activity levels or high activity levels would be associated with high inattention. Alternately, moderate levels of activity

(the absence of SCT or hyperactivity) would be associated with low inattention. This hypothesized curvilinear relationship can be seen again in Figure 6.

The present study hypothesized that the pattern described above and identified in Figure

3 better describes the construct of ADHD. It was further hypothesized that the presence of this pattern could be verified statistically using simple curvilinear regression. If this new pattern is verified, then it would solve two major issues: one, the current categorization of inattentive, hyperactive/impulsive, or combined has not had strong empirical support; and two, much research suggests that sluggishness needs to be included in the conceptualization of ADHD.

Significance

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is perhaps one of the most commonly diagnosed psychological disorders among children, with as many as 1 in every 20 youth in the

United States receiving the diagnosis (APA, 2013). Despite this prevalence it has also been one of the more controversial pediatric disorders (Stubbe, 2000) due in large part to public perception of its symptoms as “typical childhood behavior,” and to the common method of treatment using dopaminergic, stimulant drugs. Various editions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of

7

Mental Disorders have been published to compensate for new knowledge about mental disorders

(DSM; APA, 1952, 1968, 1974, 1980, 1987, 1994, 2000 & 2013), and throughout that time, the construct of ADHD has been revised multiple times. The name, developmental progression, and symptom makeup of the construct have all changed in an effort to better represent the nature of the disorder.

Because of this, some go so far as to say that “ADHD is unlikely to exist as an identifiable disease” (Furman, 2008, p. 775) due in part to the heterogeneity of the ADHD construct. Part of this heterogeneity is the result of subtypes within the ADHD population, predominantly inattentive type (ADHD-I), predominantly hyperactive/impulsive type (ADHD-

H/I), and combined type (ADHD-C) as categorized in the 5 th edition of the Diagnostic and

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; APA, 2013). However, these subtypes are individually heterogeneous as well (Barkley, 2013; Moruzzi, Rijsdijk, & Battaglia, 2013), do not match the factor structure of the symptoms (Willcut et al., 2012), and overlap each other to a high degree (DSM-5). Additional variability has been attributed to a different subset of symptoms that has not yet been integrated into the ADHD construct proposed by the DSM-5.

Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) is the interim name given to this subset of symptoms, as it is not currently recognized by the APA as a unique disorder. The SCT construct seems to identify a distinct subset of the inattentive ADHD population (Barkley, 2013), and includes symptoms of mental or functional slowness that are not explained by another condition. The accepted SCT symptom set is still in flux, but symptoms such as daydreaming, lost in thoughts, slow, and drowsy are commonly used to define the aspects of SCT (Barkley, 2012; McBurnett &

Pfiffner, 2005; Penny, Waschbusch, Klein, Corkum, & Eskes, 2009 ).

8

In addition to describing a pattern of impairment in speed of thought, attention, and behavior, these symptoms are also associated with social dysfunction, withdrawn behavior, academic underperformance, unhappiness, internalizing behaviors, depression, anxiety and other disorders in a pattern that appears to be distinct from the other recognized subtypes of ADHD

(Bauermeister, Barkley, Bauermeister, Martínez, & McBurnett, 2012; Carlson & Mann, 2002;

Marshall, Evans, Eiraldi, Becker, & Power, 2014). Although many individuals with ADHD-I may exhibit these characteristics, the symptoms of those with ADHD-H/I tend to be more strongly correlated with aggression and to be comorbid with oppositional defiant disorder

(Lahey, Schaughency, Strauss, & Frame, 1984; Lahey et al., 1994). It is important to note that, although it includes the word cognitive, SCT is not considered a measure of low intelligence or intellectual disability (Becker & Langberg, 2012). Similarly, while some research has found a correlation between childhood symptoms of SCT and adult deficits in processing speed

(Khurshid, Walker, Riley, & Wellington, 2014), other research has found no association between

SCT processing speed (Bauermeister et al., 2012).

The hyperactivity typical of ADHD-H/I and the slow and lethargic symptoms of SCT are intuitively inverse characteristics, and yet both can serve to explain the lack of attention typified by ADHD-I. The symptoms of ADHD-I also do not distinguish between two possible types of inattention that could be causing the disturbance. This conflation of hyperactivity and sluggishness into one set of inattentive symptoms may obscure any differences between them when analyzed statistically, and might result in inaccurate findings. This inaccuracy is significant when considering the impairments suffered by those with ADHD and SCT, and many of those with symptoms of SCT may even be receiving inappropriate clinical treatment better suited for other types of inattention. Certainly, the treatment for cognitive sluggishness would be different

9 from the treatment for cognitive hyperactivity. It may be possible to develop a more accurate construct of ADHD by clarifying the symptoms of the inattentive type; distinguishing inattention due to hyperactivity from inattention due to sluggishness.

Diagnostic Dissent

Presently, researchers have yet to come to a consensus on the validity of the SCT construct. Some researchers, such as Dr. Russell A. Barkley, Dr. Keith McBurnett and others that have given great attention to the matter, have come away convinced of its existence as a discrete disorder worth considerable study (Barkley, 2014; McBurnett, Pfiffner, & Frick, 2001). An opposing viewpoint has been voiced as well, urging caution before adding another pathological label to what may be considered normal childhood behaviors. Dr. Allen Frances, in an interview with the New York Times writer Alan Schwarz, went so far as to accuse SCT researchers of performing a “…public health experiment on millions of kids,” and stated that “I have no doubt there are kids who meet criteria for this thing, but nothing is more irrelevant” (Schwarz, 2014).

Whichever position one may take, further research is needed to guide policy and treatment.

DSM Incorporation

Whether SCT is in actuality a subtype of ADHD or its own disorder, it has been maintained under the umbrella of ADHD in the DSM-IV-TR edition (2000) and in the DSM-5

(2013) as well. Although it is not explicitly mentioned in either edition, its inclusion may be inferred from the DSM-IV-TR inclusion of the diagnosis 314.9 Attention –Deficit/Hyperactivity

Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (ADHD-NOS). The explanation of this diagnostic option includes a notation that individuals with a “behavioral pattern marked by sluggishness, daydreaming, and hypoactivity [italics added]” can be diagnosed.

10

The last three symptoms in the explanation, sluggishness, daydreaming, and hypoactivity , are typical of the SCT symptom set, and are clearly separated from the accepted ADHD symptom constellation by their inclusion under the diagnosis of ADHD-NOS. The DSM-5

(APA, 2013) eliminates the NOS option and replaces it with the two options of Other Specified

ADHD and Unspecified ADHD. The explanations for these two diagnoses do not retain the mention of SCT symptoms that the DSM-IV-TR used, but neither do they disclaim the symptoms as clinically indicative of ADHD impairment. While likely meant to encompass individuals whose symptoms do not fit exactly within the other subtypes, this categorization is a way of including SCT symptoms within ADHD, yet rejecting SCT as a stand-alone construct.

Based on this ambiguity, individuals with symptoms of SCT have been able to receive diagnoses of ADHD for more than two decades. Although this unconventional method of diagnosis permits patient access to affordable treatment and study, this method bends the rules inherent in the diagnostic process. It forces clinicians to assign a potentially inaccurate label in order to prescribe an off-label treatment. This gap between the theoretical and the practical instills a distrust of the diagnostic system and is indicative of a need to reevaluate the way in which the attention disorders have been defined.

Prevalence

According to prevalence rates published in the DSM 5, ADHD generally occurs among children in about 5% of the population, and among adults in about 2.5% of the population (APA,

2013). These rates appear to be consistent in worldwide samples as well (Faraone & Biederman,

2005). Prevalence rates for SCT, however, have not been widely collected and are still tentative, but according to one estimate it may occur in as much as 5.8% of the adult population (Barkley,

2012). These prevalence rates are comparable or greater than those found for ADHD, and if

11 verified, may represent 15.8 million individuals in the United States alone based on calculations using current census estimates of the US population (U.S. Census Bureau, 2014).

Study Benefits

Considering the potentially high prevalence of SCT and ADHD and the associated functional impairments, it is imperative that research accurately identify the relationship between these disorders. Previous research into ADHD that neglected to take into account the heterogeneous nature of the disorder could only give composite associations for treatment efficacy, correlates, heritability, and etiology. The identification of subtypes within ADHD led to the discovery of significantly different correlates for the individual subtypes (Carlson, Shin, &

Booth, 1999). Correctly conceptualizing the subtypes is critical for both diagnosis and treatment.

Until this current construct is expanded to account for the SCT symptoms, both the research conducted and the treatment offered to those in need will be incomplete.

In order to clarify the above confusion, the following research question was explored:

When SCT and Hyperactivity are analyzed together as a continuum of Activity

Level, does the continuum form a curvilinear relationship with Inattention?

This question will be explored in two steps:

1. Create a combined hyperactivity/sluggishness scale and test the scale reliability of that measure to determine its appropriateness in answering the main research question.

2. Utilize multiple curvilinear regression to test the underlying structure of the proposed

ADHD model.

12

CHAPTER 2

LITERATURE REVIEW

In order to identify the relationship between ADHD and SCT, this review will begin by examining the history of the two constructs, how they have changed over time, and for what reasons. Then, this review will examine the pattern of heterogeneity within ADHD and SCT as identified by studies of their symptomology, onset, gender differences, common comorbidities and correlates, etiology, neurological deficits, assessment and diagnosis, and common methods of treatment. This information will be used to build a theoretical model of the relationship between them. Lastly, this review will examine the factor analytic and curvilinear regression research concerning ADHD and SCT in order to identify the methods and findings supporting these two constructs.

The ADHD and SCT Constructs

Terms and Process

The definition of ADHD has been changed over time based on the presumed core symptoms, the presumed etiology, new research, and even loyalty to conventional language

(Hyman, 2010), yet the actual illness itself did not change as a result. The modern ADHD construct includes 18 possible symptoms of hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention (APA,

2015), and ADHD research presumes that the symptom names will be understood by all people in the same way. Yet these labels are simply a heuristic to communicate a larger idea, and are subject to interpretation by patients, families, and even professionals. Despite this, often these heuristics are treated as the disorders (or symptoms) themselves (Kendall & Jablensky, 2003). It may be the case that significant confusion about ADHD is due to this difficulty in representing and labeling the symptoms accurately.

13

History

This reconceptualization of ADHD has occurred repeatedly throughout its history. The first known reference to a disorder of attention appears to be from a text authored by Melchior

Adam Weikard, and published in 1775. Weikard, a physician, noted a pattern of carelessness, extended latency in reading, distractibility and impatience (Barkley & Peters, 2012). Weikard described a single disorder, which he attributed to “irritable fibres.” This description appears to be of a disorder that Weikard found in adults, describing their behaviors as more typical of a child’s inattention.

The next earliest available reference is from a medical textbook by Alexander Crichton, published in 1798. Crichton described two types of attention-deficit disorders, as opposed to

Weikard’s one. Crichton identified one of the two disorders in a similar fashion to current conceptions of hyperactivity or over-arousal of attention, and the other as typified by under- arousal of attention and low energy. Crichton also speculated that the over-arousal was likely to be inherited, while the under-arousal seemed to be acquired from the environment or by trauma.

This early dichotomy set the pattern for future research into attentional disorders, although the initial edition of the DSM (APA, 1952) did not include a parallel to Crichton’s attention disorders. The DSM-II (APA, 1968) identified a single analogous disorder labeled hyperkinetic reaction of childhood. This construct included the symptoms of over-activity, restlessness, distractibility, and short attention span, and was clearly characteristic of hyperactivity and associated behavioral deficits as might be identified by parents or teachers.

This strictly externalizing conceptualization of ADHD also proposed that these visible behaviors tended to diminish in adolescence.

14

While the inattention due to hyperactivity was included within this construct, symptoms of non-hyperactive inattention or sluggishness were not included as unitary constructs in the

DSM-II (1968). They might be inferred from the development of the withdrawing reaction and overanxious reaction constructs, two of the other 6 conditions identified in children at the time.

This early distinction between the external hyperactivity and other disorders allowed for the hyperactive symptoms to take apparent precedence in the research until the early 1970’s.

During the 1970’s, Virginia Douglas’s research into hyperactive children identified a core subset of symptoms, that included inattention and impulsivity, which appeared to account for the bulk of the deficits suffered by the children (Douglas, 1972). Later research would reclassify the impulsivity as more closely related to hyperactivity than inattention (Carlson, 1986), but

Douglas’ focus on inattention helped shape the categorization of attention disorders within the

DSM-III (APA, 1980). While the second edition of the DSM placed inattention as a symptom of hyperactivity, this third edition of the DSM was the first to recognize a subtype that was composed of inattention without hyperactivity. No cause was proposed for this type of inattention, and it was placed in equal standing with hyperactivity as a cause itself, rather than a symptom. The construct was defined as merely not caused by hyperactivity. The two subtypes were labeled accordingly, attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity and attention deficit disorder without hyperactivity, or ADD+H and ADD-H.

Research into these two subtypes began to increase around the time of the DSM-III

(1980) publication, and consequently this period of time marked the beginning of the modern era of research into SCT as well. Studies were conducted designed to identify differences between the two new subtypes of ADD, +H and -H; and a pattern emerged where the hyperactive (+H) youth were more prone to aggression and conduct problems, while the non-hyperactive (-H)

15 youth were more prone to be anxious and withdrawn (Lahey et al., 1984; Pelham, Atkins, &

Murphy, 1981; Hinshaw, 1987). The 1984 paper by Lahey and his colleagues may also be the first to give the name of SCT to a subset of the -H group with daydreamy and sluggish symptoms. The term SCT was likely first created by R. Neeper (Carlson, 1986), a graduate student of Caryn Carlson’s while she worked with Lahey on the 1984 paper. Subsequent studies that replicated the factor distinction between ADD+H and ADD-H, also identified the SCT factor

(Neeper & Lahey, 1986; Lahey et al., 1988). Lahey and his colleagues (1988) found that the best factorial model of ADD was composed of three factors: hyperactivity/impulsivity, inattention/disorganization , and slow tempo . In this same study, however, a Ward’s cluster analysis (Ward, 1963) divided the clinically referred sample into three symptomatic groups, the first cluster with no ADD symptoms, the second cluster with symptoms of sluggishness and inattention, and the third cluster with symptoms of hyperactivity and inattention. This led the researchers to conclude that future ADD diagnoses should be either AD-HD, or undifferentiated attention deficit disorder (UADD).

Critically, other research had also found that the inattentive population could be split using separate symptom categories from various parent rating scales, and that the hyperactivity- related inattention symptoms were composed of sloppiness, carelessness, and irresponsibility, while those unrelated to hyperactivity were described using terms such as confused, lost in thought, unmotivated , and apathy (Barkley, DuPaul, & McMurray, 1990). This research indicated that once inattention had been defined in a less ambiguous way, it tended to lose its apparent coherence as a unitary subtype; and the attention disorders were more clearly split into inattention caused by hyperactivity, and inattention caused by sluggishness. This interpretation of the research, while congruent with Crichton’s (1798) early observations and with the cluster

16 analysis results found by Lahey et al. (1988), was nevertheless discounted in favor of the three subtype factorial model found by Lahey and his colleagues (1988). This division of inattention approach will be revisited later in this paper, however.

The DSM-III revision (APA, 1987) eliminated altogether the division of ADD into subtypes, and instead created one disorder labeled Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

(ADHD). This movement was temporary, as the DSM-IV work group tasked with creating the new categorization for the attention disorders began to reconsider the subtypes for the upcoming edition. Although four symptoms of SCT were proposed to be included as symptoms of inattention (APA Task Force on DSM-IV, 1991), and three of those were tested in the DSM-IV field trials ( often drowsy or sluggish, often daydreams, and forgetful ), only forgetful was included in the DSM-IV (APA, 1994), with the reasoning that it was useful for ADHD as a whole, since it did not distinguish between the hyperactive and non-hyperactive types of inattention (Frick et al., 1994). As will be explored further later, while drowsy , daydreams , and forgetful were each good predictors of general inattention among a combined-subtype sample of youth with ADHD, inattention was often present without drowsy and daydreams (McBurnett et al., 2001). The DSM-IV consequently included only two subtypes, ADHD predominantly hyperactive/impulsive type (ADHD-H/I), and ADHD predominantly inattentive type (ADHD-I).

Some in the field (McBurnett et al., 2001; Milich et al., 2001) thought that the predominantly inattentive subtype would identify a group of youth that was too heterogeneous to represent a single subtype of disorder, and that there was still cause for the study of the SCT symptoms which had essentially been abandoned in the research. Milich and his colleagues

(2001) argued that the predominately inattentive type of ADHD should no longer even be considered a part of ADHD at all; that there was sufficient evidence of a distinction between it

17 and the type of inattention that would soon be known as ADHD-C to define ADHD-I as an entirely separate disorder from ADHD. This argument paralleled the study of SCT, though failed to merge theoretically with it at that time or since.

At this point, research into the validity of SCT diverged from the consensus classification of ADHD as published in the subsequent editions of the DSM. While research into SCT slowly began increasing, the DSM-IV (APA, 1994) instead established the provisional bridge between

ADHD-H/I type and ADHD-I type by creating a combined hyperactive and inattentive subtype

(ADHD-C). Although factor analysis identifies no ADHD-C factor (Span, Earleywine, &

Strybel, 2002; Willcut et al., 2012), this was seen as an expedient way to account for the overlap between the I type and H/I type. It allowed the inclusion of individuals with symptoms of both, but insufficient symptoms for a diagnosis of either. Little has changed in the accepted construct since then, as the DSM-5 (APA, 2014) espoused the categorization of ADHD from the previous editions, again without coming to a conclusion on the meaning of the SCT symptoms.

Construct Development

Another of the dangers in identifying and defining a psychological construct is the tendency to eliminate related symptoms that nevertheless fail to differentiate between our new construct and the previous construct. Once the core symptoms within a construct are chosen, subsequent studies only use those symptoms to define the disorder. Researchers also use that construct to validate any proposed changes to the construct. In other words, any errors in past research are perpetuated, leading to a tautology, or circular research, if you will.

This process is what seems to have occurred leading up to the publication of the DSM-IV and the exclusion of SCT. Despite a group of two symptoms typical of modern SCT constructs, often drowsy or sluggish and often daydreams , showing very high positive predictive power

18

(PPP) for inattention, even when compared to the other symptoms of inattention, they were rejected from inclusion in the ADHD-I construct (Frick et al., 1994). This critical decision in the history of SCT was made based on the fact that the absence of the two SCT symptoms failed to predict the absence of inattention during the field trials; in other words, the SCT symptoms had poor negative predictive power (NPP) for the inattention subset. Although the SCT symptoms were the top first and third predictors of inattention, inattention could also exist without them. In the present study, the proposed structure would explain the poor negative predictive power of

SCT. As seen in the hypothesized relationship, high inattention would be associated with either sluggishness or hyperactivity, and the relationship would not be evident when using a linear model. This sluggish-only inattentive group is seen clearly in the U-shaped figure being

Proposed, marked for emphasis in Figure 7.

Figure 7. Illustration of proposed curvilinear relationship between inattention and activity level with demarcation for sluggishness-related inattention.

19

It should again be noted that the sample included youth with both ADHD-H/I and

ADHD-I (McBurnett et al., 2001), and that the inattention being measured would not distinguish between subtypes of inattention. Any inattentiveness caused by hyperactivity would be equated or conflated with inattentiveness caused by sluggishness: the remaining forgetful symptom was actually predictive of all inattention, while the discarded symptoms of often drowsy or sluggish and often daydreams were in fact only predictive of sluggish inattention. It does not appear that the researchers included these two symptoms when conducting the same predictive analysis on the hyperactive/impulsive symptoms, although the PPP and NPP for that comparison would be valuable now for this paper.

Not until the 2001 revival of interest in SCT was there a renewed push to reconsider the validity of the SCT symptoms as a distinct construct or as a reliable subset of ADHD-I.

McBurnett, Pfiffner, and Frick (2001) proposed that if there were in fact two types of inattention, one caused by hyperactivity/impulsivity and one caused by sluggishness or hypoactivity, as seemed likely, then the apparent poor negative predictive power of SCT was in fact logical and unsurprising. It might, in fact, be considered strong evidence against the necessity of creating the mosaic ADHD- Combined subtype, since an improved definition of inattention might have clarified the distinction between ADHD-H/I and ADHD-I; the phenomenon previously demonstrated by Barkley and his colleagues in 1990. This dual nature of inattention was noted by others as well (Diamond, 2005; Milich et al., 2001), who described how different parts of the inattentive population arguably demonstrated diametrically opposed behavioral and emotional correlates, vastly different comorbidities, and possibly even different neurological pathways.

However, this line of research has failed to find a strong footing in contemporary literature, perhaps because of the difficulty in reversing the momentum of the powerful ADHD

20 movement. While some studies take the incomplete step of contrasting ADHD-C with ADHD-I and conclude that they are indeed different and should remain separate (Bauermeister et al.,

2005; Collings, 2003; Counts, Nigg, Stawicki, Rappley, & Von Eye, 2005; Grizenko, Paci, &

Joober, 2010; Weiss, Worling, & Wasdell, 2003), no studies have yet concluded that the entire

ADHD construct itself should be razed and rebuilt to adequately subdivide the types of inattention. In the meantime, SCT research continues under the assumption that it has already been sufficiently differentiated from ADHD.

Current research continues to experiment with various ways of defining SCT, as there is no current consensus. Some researchers propose that it is a distinct disorder from ADHD and its subtypes, and deserves to be considered as a second (or possibly third) attention disorder

(Barkley, 2014; Milich et al., 2001; Saxbe & Barkley, 2014), perhaps returning to the terminology of Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) to contrast with the ADHD label. Saxbe and

Barkley (2014) counter that suggestion, arguing that the ADD title would likely create confusion, as it recalls the older DSM-III construction. They offer the new name of Concentration Deficit

Disorder as an alternative, noting that it is sufficiently new to avoid confusion and vague enough not to imply more certainty than we can claim about its cause.

Symptoms

While the 18 symptoms included within the subtypes of ADHD have remained constant since the DSM-IV (APA, 1994), the SCT symptoms utilized in research continue to vary from study to study. Symptom sets have ranged from a set of two to a potential 17 symptoms, but a representative set (Barkley, 2013) currently being used in research is as follows:

1. Prone to daydreaming;

2. Has trouble staying awake or alert;

21

3. Mentally foggy or easily confused;

4. Stares a lot;

5. Spacey, their mind seems to be elsewhere and not paying attention to what is going on

around them;

6. Lethargic, more tired than others;

7. Underactive compared to other children;

8. Slow moving or sluggish;

9. Doesn’t seem to understand or process questions or explanations as quickly or as

accurately as others;

10. Seems drowsy or has a sleepy appearance;

11. Apathetic or withdrawn, seems less engaged in activities than others;

12. Gets lost in his or her own thoughts;

13. Slow to complete tasks, needs more time than others; and

14. Lacks initiative to complete work or their effort fades quickly after getting starting.

These symptoms have been subdivided in various ways (Penny et al., 2009; Jacobson et al., 2012); and the symptoms tend to group themselves around the concepts of slowness, sleepiness, sluggishness, and daydreaming. One study, however, also included symptoms of low persistence (Jacobson et al., 2012), and another study has included symptoms of a deficit in working memory (McBurnett, 2014), which typically have been associated with ADHD-I symptomatology. This shift is not terribly surprising due to the overlap and possible duplication of symptoms within ADHD-I and SCT, and perhaps would be further noted if the standard DSM-

5 symptoms of ADHD were reexamined and revised.

22

A Pattern of Heterogeneity in ADHD?

Even though the subtypes of ADHD are labeled to indicate overlap of symptom presentation as evidenced by the use of predominantly (APA, 2013, p.59), the assumption of

ADHD researchers is clearly that their definitions are sufficiently distinct for use in statistical analysis. Consequently, when heterogeneity occurs in correlational or latent structure analysis that is due to this overlap, it is likely to be attributed to the true nature of the disorder, rather than to the error caused by an incomplete construct.

While this accepted overlap may be designed to retain useful but confusing symptoms until a better construct is found (as discussed earlier), extrapolations based on confounded variables inevitably lead to further confusion (or worse, when patient diagnosis and medication are involved). As noted earlier, the subtype of ADHD-C was created in order to cover the cases when the overlap was most prominent, but this subtype does not exist as a true factor: it has no latent equivalent (Span, Earleywine, & Strybel, 2002; Willcut et al., 2012). Strangely, it still accounts for between 32% (Froehlich et al., 2007) and 49% (Volk, Neuman, & Todd, 2005) of the ADHD diagnoses. In other words, while ADHD-C does not exist as latent factor, the two genuine factors (ADHD-H/I and ADHD-I) fail to describe from a third to a half of the individuals who receive the diagnosis of ADHD. In order to identify the true pattern of heterogeneity inherent in ADHD, it is necessary to review the relationships among the current subtypes and SCT as established in broad areas of the ADHD literature.

SCT and ADHD-H/I

The constructs of ADHD-H/I and SCT have been named according to their most prominent traits, as they are currently defined. Consequently, ADHD-H/I can be understood to comprise the deficits caused by hyperactivity or impulsivity, while SCT seems to mean that the

23 problem is a slow rate of thought and behavior, or in other words, hypoactivity (Barkley, 2012;

Becker, Marshall, & McBurnett, 2014). Considering its common placement within the umbrella of attention disorders (Barkley, 2012; Barkley, 2013; Saxbe & Barkley, 2014), we can assume that the slow rate of thought is understood to be affecting attention as well, and may represent the opposite end from hyperactivity on the distribution of inattention. With these assumptions, one might expect there to be little comorbidity between ADHD-H/I and SCT, and possibly even a negative correlation, once the ADHD-I symptoms have been controlled for. This latter possibility has been borne out by research by Penny and her colleagues (2009). In this study, the researchers used regression analyses to examine the relationship between ADHD-H/I and SCT, and did indeed find a negative correlation after controlling for ADHD-I. Accordingly, it is consistent with the research to view hyperactivity and sluggishness as the extremes of a continuum of activity.

SCT and ADHD-I

ADHD-I can be understood to describe clinically significant levels of general inattention.

SCT, a potential explanation for inattention, is also highly correlated with ADHD-I (Hartman,

Willcutt, Rhee, & Pennington, 2004). It is possible that a clearer definition of inattention would eliminate the influence of hyperactive inattention on the construct, and reinforce that correlation further.

SCT and ADHD-C

Although ADHD-C was not intended to correspond with an individual factor of ADHD, it has been used by many studies as a more realistic representation of ADHD-H/I (Brooks, et al.,

2006; Grizenko, Paci, & Joober, 2010; Milich et al., 2001; Øie, Skogli, Andersen, Hovik, &

Hugdahl, 2014). The combined type specifier is given if criteria are met for both H/I and I (APA,

24

2013, p.60), and essentially states that the individual has clinically significant levels of inattention caused by hyperactivity and impulsivity, and also has clinically significant levels of non-specific inattention. Typically, very little association is found between hyperactive symptoms and sluggish symptoms, showing instead a negative correlation (Hartman, Willcut,

Rhee, & Pennington, 2004; Penny et al., 2009; Skansgaard & Burns, 1998) .

Summary

A possible alternative construct that is consistent with the pattern of correlations above would require inattention to be viewed as a separate variable from hyperactivity and sluggishness, and instead allow hyperactivity and sluggishness to represent two opposite extremes of physical and cognitive activity. With this activity variable represented by the x-axis of a line graph, and inattention represented by the y-axis of the line graph, a curvilinear relationship is seen, as seen on page 17.

As might be expected, inattention is high at both high and low levels of physical and cognitive activity level, and inattention is low when the activity level is optimal. The presentations of inattention would logically be different, since, although they may share the ultimate effect of impeding attention and focus, one can imagine how hyperactivity would induce a lack of discrimination between stimuli, while sluggishness would merely induce a lack of attention to most any stimuli.

Onset and Development

Due to the preliminary nature of the SCT construct and the dynamic composition of its symptom set, it is difficult to identify a reliable age range for the onset of SCT. Based on the intermingling of ADHD and SCT within the literature, the methods for sampling the clinical and general populations have likely conflated some of the aspects of both groups when describing

25

ADHD. The broad DSM-5 (APA, 2013) description for ADHD describes it as a developmental disorder, or that the symptoms are likely present at birth in ways that are indistinguishable from typical behavior. The description continues that “excessive motor activity” (p. 62) is often noted early, but may be masked before the age of four by age-appropriate activity levels. With the reduction of overt hyperactivity in adolescence, other symptoms are more prominent, specifically

“restlessness, inattention, poor planning and impulsivity” (p. 62). Estimates of onset comparing the subtypes of ADHD seem to replicate the anecdotal indicators in the DSM-5, showing that children referred for ADHD-C tend to be younger than those referred for ADHD-I (Bauermeister et al., 2005; Carlson et al., 1999). Perhaps hyperactivity is viewed as beginning before inattention on account of the tendency for inattentive symptoms to be less salient in preadolescence due to their internalizing nature and the typically lower expectations for children in matters of personal responsibility.

SCT may be represented by these estimates for ADHD, but not all cases of SCT may develop along the same pattern. Studies bridging the fields of psychology and medicine have found SCT symptoms as likely repercussions of either illness or strenuous treatment, and possibly even fetal exposure to alcohol (Graham, 2013; Reeves et al., 2007; Willard et al., 2013).

Such connections will be explored later in the paper, but the possibility of artificial causes for

SCT may call into question the assumptions and utility of identifying a developmental onset.

Again, further study is needed before a reliable estimate of onset can be given, if such an estimate is appropriate. If ADHD-I and SCT are truly aspects of the same disorder, then current estimates of ADHD onset may be unreliable as well for the time being.

Comorbidity and Correlate Differences

The deficits and disorders that are found to coexist with ADHD or SCT have been seen to vary in predictable ways, and nosological evidence may be gained from studying their 26 relationships. Whether these syndromes are merely comorbid with ADHD and SCT, or if they represent multiple pathways to the same disorders is unknown; but clear patterns have developed in the literature, and a few will be analyzed here.

Externalizing behaviors . Historically, ADHD was regarded as a behavioral disorder; one labeled hyperkinetic reaction of childhood and grouped with other disorders that were thought to be typified by such externalizing behaviors as escape, aggression, and delinquency

(DSM-II; APA, 1968). This association was continued in the DSM-III, where ADHD and conduct disorder were grouped together under the category of behavioral (overt) childhood disorders. Although it was subdivided into ADD with and without hyperactivity, the associated features of “obstinacy, stubbornness, negativism, bossiness, bullying, increased mood lability, low frustration tolerance, temper outbursts, low self-esteem, and lack of response to discipline”

(DSM-III; APA, 1980, p.42) were thought to be equal between the two groups, if milder without the presence of hyperactivity. The association was somewhat less strongly indicated in the DSM-

IV (APA, 1994) and DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) editions, where the chapter sections label both attention deficit and behavior disorders separately, and in the DSM-5 (APA, 2013), the separation is complete. ADHD is currently labeled as a neurodevelopmental disorder, and behavior disorders are found some 402 pages further on in the manual (DSM-5; APA, 2013).

The reasoning behind this change is that, although the symptoms of ADHD involve behaviors that may cause disruption in the home or school environments, they are thought to be secondary to a deficit in impulse control or an excess of energy, not due to oppositionality or willful defiance. Also, even though early research found ADD to be a good predictor of later arrests for delinquent behavior (Satterfield, Hoppe, & Schell, 1982), ADHD does not perfectly correlate with disruptive behavior (Biederman et al., 1996). Instead, research tends to find a

27 strong association only between externalizing behaviors and the H/I subtype of ADHD rather than with the I subtype (Gaub & Carlson, 1997; Graez, Sawyer, Hazell, Arney, & Baghurst,

2001; Lahey et al., 1984; Lahey et al., 1994; Lalonde, Turgay, & Hudson, 1998) or with SCT

(Becker, Luebbe, Fite, Stoppelbein, & Greening, 2014; Becker & Langberg, 2013; Lee, Burns,

Snell, & McBurnett, 2014), with few exceptions (Garner, Marceaux, Mrug, Patterson, &

Hodgens 2010). In fact, SCT predicts lower rates of externalization than even ADHD-I (Lee et al., 2014). This may be due to the symptom overlap between the ADHD-I and ADHD-H/I subtypes, or to the ambiguous ADHD-I symptoms themselves. Once the relationship with

ADHD-I is accounted for, the correlation between ADHD-H/I and SCT becomes negative, as noted before (Penny et al., 2009). That same study found the correlation between SCT and ODD to be negative as well once ADHD-I had been controlled for. This new relationship could be illustrated as shown in Figure 8. Despite these comparisons between subtypes indicating that

ADHD-H/I is still strongly related to externalizing behaviors, the distinction between hyperactivity and behavioral disorders continues to be enforced.

Internalizing symptoms . Internalizing symptoms such as depression and anxiety tend to serve as a counterpoint to the externalizing behaviors in ADHD. As established before, ADHD-

H/I tends to predict externalizing symptoms and ADHD-I tends to predict internalizing symptoms, but the presence of SCT symptoms seems to predict higher rates of internalization than even ADHD-I ( Becker & Langberg, 2013; Lee et al., 2014 ). Although some research has indicated a clear difference between a factor of SCT symptoms and a factor of depression symptoms (Burns, Servera, Bernad, Carrillo, & Cardo, 2013), the authors clearly noted that they had formulated their SCT scale in such a way as to eliminate symptoms that associated the two constructs.

28

Figure 8. Linear illustration of correlations between externalizing behaviors and both SCT and ADHD-H/I, viewing the latter two constructs as endpoints of a continuum of activity level.

Figure 9. Linear illustration of correlations between internalizing behaviors and both SCT and ADHD-H/I, viewing the latter two constructs as endpoints of a continuum of activity level.

29

Typically, sluggishness is predictive of internalization, and of depression most of all

(Becker et al., 2014). T he previous pattern seen with externalizing is apparent here as well with internalization, but reversed, as seen in Figure 9. Internalization is associated most with the low activity level of sluggishness, and associated the least with the high activity level of hyperactivity. Inattention is only moderately associated wi th both internalizing and externalizing, as would be expected if the inattention construct conflated both ends of the hyperactive inattention and sluggish inattention, averaging out the association.

Academic difficulty/learning disorders . ADHD has long been associated with deficits in academic scholarship and intelligence (Menkes, Rowe, & Menkes, 1967), but with the advent of subtyping between hyperactive (ADD/H) and non-hyperactive types (ADD/WO), a pattern of differentiation in those deficits began to emerge. In a 1986 study, the two subtypes were found to differ on scales of intelligence and achievement (Carlson, Lahey, & Neeper). The children with

ADD/H obtained significantly lower FSIQ scores than those without hyperactivity, and lower

Verbal IQ scores as well. While both groups scored significantly worse than controls on measures of reading and spelling achievement, those children categorized as ADD/WO also scored more poorly on math achievement. One sample of children with nonspecific ADHD diagnoses have been found to have comorbid learning disabilities (LD’s) in 70% of the cases, with a deficit in written expression being by far the most common (Mayes, Calhoun, & Crowell,

2000). When broken down into subtypes, the pattern appears again of an association between inattention and math or reading learning disabilities (Marshall, Hynd, Handwerk, & Hall, 1997;

Morgan, Hynd, Riccio, & Hall, 1996; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000) and a less specific association between hyperactivity and the specific learning disabilities.

30

The relationship between ADHD and intelligence appears to be complicated. While

ADHD may be predictive of significantly lower scores on intelligence tests (Carlson et al., 1986;

Kuntsi et al., 2003), ADHD is also found in individuals with high IQ (>120, Antshel et al.,

2007), and symptoms may be alternatively caused by high intelligence (Hartnett, Nelson, &

Rinn, 2004) or masked by it (Milioni et al., 2014). The possibility exists that the ADHD symptoms interfere with the validity of intelligence measures, causing an underestimate of functioning in an otherwise capable individual.

SCT may have such a complex relationship with intelligence as well. While some estimates of SCT seem to show it is negatively associated with scores on both intelligence and achievement measures (Mikami, Huang-Pollack, Pfiffner, McBurnett, & Hangpai, 2007) and similar to ADHD-I in relationship to the reading and math disabilities (Hartman et al., 2004), most estimates of SCT indicate no association between levels of SCT symptoms and academic success (Becker & Langberg, 2013; Carlson & Mann, 2002; Wahlstedt & Bohlin, 2010).

Considering the current renaissance of research into SCT, additional data is likely forthcoming that will clarify this further, but the present cognitive and achievement literature supports a distinction between ADHD-H/I and ADHD-I, as well as a possible overlap between ADHD-I and SCT. A more specific pattern is not clear.

Possible Etiology

While a genetic explanation of ADHD is commonly accepted, a genetic explanation of

SCT has little support thus far. One study seems to have found a shared genetic contribution between them however (Moruzzi et al., 2013). Unfortunately, the areas of the genome that appear to be involved, 16p13 and 17p11 (Comings, Gade ‐Andavolu, Gonzalez, Blake, Wu, &

MacMurray, 1999) appear to be involved in determining the dopaminergic pathways, yet the

31 genes for the dopamine receptors only modestly influence the development of ADHD (Yeh,

Morley, & Hall, 2004).

The underlying problems with identifying a genotype for either disorder are the complex set of endophenotypes that comprise the symptom sets (Castellanos & Tannock, 2002; Doyle et al., 2005), and the fact that the symptoms indicative of the disorders may be caused by a variety of conditions. For example, symptoms of SCT can be caused by depression, a sleeping disorder

(Langberg, Becker, Dvorsky, & Luebbe, 2014), leukemia (Reeves et al., 2007), brain tumors

(Willard et al., 2013), and fetal alcohol exposure (Graham, 2013) to name a few. These diverse associations, if viewed as causes for the SCT-symptoms, may point to SCT as being the natural endpoint of multiple pathological pathways, or multicausal, rather than being associated with its own unique etiology.

In other words, SCT may simply be one of the body’s natural responses to assault or stress, as has been proposed to explain depression (Nesse, 2000). Such an explanation has been proposed for ADHD as well (Furman, 2008; Stolzer, 2009). As many other syndromes can elicit the symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity, including boredom (Kass, Wallace, & Vodanovich,

2003), high intelligence (Hartnett et al., 2004), low intelligence (Kuntsi et al., 2003), impaired adrenal/stress response (Kaneko, Hoshino, Hashimoto, Okano, & Kumashiro, 1993), traumatic brain injury (Konrad, Gauggel, Manz, & Schöll, 2000), and others, it may be that cognitive hyperactivity/impulsivity is also a physiological response to environmental stimuli; stimuli that may contrast those that trigger the sluggish response. In this scenario, inattention could be defined as any behavioral or cognitive response that does not fit with social expectations for attention, and could be caused by either the hyperactive or the sluggish response. This is certainly in line with the U-shaped relationship being proposed, illustrated in Figure 10.

32

Figure 10. Illustration of proposed curvilinear relationship between inattention and activity level, with theoretical demarcations encapsulating areas of function and dysfunction.

Neurophysiology. Neuroimaging techniques are not yet diagnostic of psychopathology as they can only identify patterns across samples, rather than in individual participants (Botteron et al., 2012). Most imaging studies of ADHD continue to suffer from issues of validity and reliability, including small sample sizes and low statistical power (Button et al., 2013), failing to differentiate between participants those with a history of stimulant treatment from those that are medication-naïve (MacMaster, Carrey, Sparkes, & Kusumakar, 2003; Sowell, Thompson,

Welcome, Henkenius, Toga, & Peterson, 2003), or between different subtypes of ADHD or patterns of action by drug type, and a lack of standardization in measurement (Valera, Faraone,

Murray, & Seidman, 2007).

Neurological deficits. The prominent theoretical explanation of ADHD (Barkley, 1997) posits that the core neurological symptoms caused by ADHD are deficits in the executive functions, namely: nonverbal working memory; speech internalization; purposeful regulation of

33 affect, motivation, and arousal; and reconstitution. Barkley proposed that these deficits all likely stem from a lack of behavioral inhibition, and that since these functions are located in the prefrontal cortex of the brain, the primary effects of ADHD must be centered there as well

(Barkley, 1997).

While ADHD does seem to involve the executive functions (EF) of the mind, this association has not been found to be equal among the subtypes. Bauermeister and his colleagues

(2012) found that the domain of inattention predicted such neuropsychological deficits more strongly than either hyperactivity/impulsivity or SCT. While such results may sound conclusive, the definition of SCT used by the researchers did not include such deficits as working memory

(as does one of the more current models of SCT (McBurnett et al., 2014)’), which preemptively minimizes any association between them. Also, using the current, ambiguous inattentive symptoms would be a cause for additional error in such a study, and could account for the dominance of inattention in predicting the EF deficits. Other research indicates that not all aspects of executive function deficits are associated with ADHD (Geurts, Verté, Oosterlaan,

Roeyers, & Sergeant, 2005), that as many as 50% of those diagnosed with ADHD have no deficit on behavioral inhibition tasks, and perhaps 20% of them have no significant impairments on any of the EF measures (Nigg, Willcutt, Doyle, & Sonuga-Barke, 2005).

Studies that use EF deficits as the primary endophenotype of ADHD also demonstrate that it is insufficient as an explanation for ADHD as a whole (Castellanos & Tannock, 2002;

Doyle et al., 2005; Nigg, Blaskey, Stawicki, & Sachek, 2004), even if it may play a part. Studies of neurological structure and ADHD also tend to find a greater association with a decrease in cerebellar volume (Castellanos et al., 2002), than with the prefrontal cortex locations predicted by Barkley’s EF theory (1997).

34

Other neurological functions that were not included in Barkley’s theory, such as processing speed and spatial memory, appear to have differential associations with the subtypes of ADHD as well. While processing speed deficits are associated with ADHD (Shanahan et al.,

2006; Woods, Lovejoy, & Ball, 2002), they appear to be most closely associated with the construct of inattention. Spatial memory deficits have not been analyzed between subtypes, but evidence suggests that it is involved in ADHD in general (Westerberg, Hirvikoski, Forssberg, &

Klingberg, 2004).

As far as SCT is concerned, there does not seem to be as much association with executive function deficits, although SCT does seem to affect the ability to be organized more than does

ADHD (Barkley, 2013). According to another study, SCT was found to be correlated with variability in spatial memory, but to have no correlation with reaction time, a measure used to indicate processing speed (Skirbekk, Hansen, Oerbeck, & Kristensen, 2011). The findings of this study seem to be at odds with the general findings of research examining SCT and processing speed (Derefinko et al., 2008; Jacobson et al., 2012), and indeed with the name and conceptual framework of SCT altogether. The implications of this are unclear, and additional research is needed to explain the discrepancy.

In summary, although the research on the endophenotype of executive functions has not come to a consensus, the symptoms of ADHD and SCT again show distinct patterns of association for each subtype and the inattention construct shares aspects in common with both hyperactivity and sluggishness. While each deficit would interfere in daily functioning, each would do so differently. The overall pattern, though incomplete, still appears to be consistent with the U-shaped relationship between inattention and activity level. As additional research is

35 conducted using more current SCT symptom sets and potentially revised ADHD symptom sets, the true relationship will likely become clearer.

Treatment

Pharmacological. Although much research has been done which highlights the short- term benefits of simulants for some who struggle with ADHD (Castells, Ramos ‐Quiroga, Bosch,

Nogueira, & Casas, 2011; Faraone, Biederman, Spencer, & Aleardi, 2006; Faraone, & Buitelaar,

2010; Fredriksen, Halmøy, Faraone, & Haavik, 2013; MTA Cooperative Group, 2004), the benefits appear to diminish between the two-year and three-year mark, with a significant increase in ADHD symptoms and delinquent behaviors (Molina et al., 2007; Molina et al., 2009; Swanson et al., 2007), seeming to indicate a temporary improvement in attention across both symptomatic and non-symptomatic groups, followed by a return toward baseline. Another study emphasized that the temporary benefits of the stimulants, in this case , were not limited to those with ADHD, and indeed were not actually functionally different between those with and without ADHD (del Campo et al., 2013).

While stimulants are known to be effective for the hyperactive type of ADHD, they haven’t been shown to be especially effective in treating SCT symptoms such as inattention or sluggishness. These stimulants work to either boost extracellular dopamine, or to increase levels of dopamine production. One study conducted more than two decades ago (Barkley et al., 1991), found that 95% those with hyperactive type showed at least moderate benefit from stimulants, but that those with SCT had either no measurable response to the drug, or responded best to the lowest administered dosage level. This result does seem to indicate a differential response to medication between those with ADHD-H/I type and those with ADHD-I type/SCT symptoms.

36

Such an unequal reaction to the same treatment does lend support to the hypothesis that ADHD-I and SCT are closely related constructs, distinct from ADHD-H/I.

While the ineffectiveness of stimulants in treating low activity symptoms of SCT is surprising, it may indicate that the increase in energy does not actually reduce whatever stressors triggered the low-energy state. This supports the idea that the continuum of activity level represents a bodily response to a condition rather than the condition itself. Suffice it to say that, since the stimulant benefits are temporary, the benefits are equal for those with and without a diagnosis of ADHD, and since a large portion of adult ADHD sufferers are resistant to stimulant therapy, perhaps even 50% at the highest estimate (Wender, 1998), the dopamine hypothesis does not fully account for the symptoms of either ADHD or of SCT.

Coaching. Despite being a relatively new area of practice, coaching has already proven to be effective in treating college students with ADHD; increasing the individual’s control over their symptoms as defined by measures of their study and learning strategies, their self-esteem, wellbeing, and satisfaction with their progress (Field, Parker, Sawilowsky, & Rolands, 2013).

Results indicated that the coaching seemed to increase the students’ study skills in all domains, and improve their sense of well-being. What is left unclear by this study is whether the coaching affects one subtype of ADHD differently than another, as no information was gathered about the specifics of the diagnoses. It is likely that some of the students were diagnosed with ADHD-I, and that some of those students suffered from symptoms of SCT, but the results only indicate a mean change across the collapsed subtypes. Nothing can be inferred about SCT specifically.

Perhaps more instructive for the purposes of this paper is the ADHD coaching study by

Prevatt and Yelland (2013) with a sample of 148 individuals that received coaching over an eight-week period. Acknowledging the heterogeneous nature of the ADHD disorder, the

37 researchers collected more specific data concerning subtypes and symptoms, and noted that 55% of the sample reported a diagnosis of ADHD-I. Self-report measures were also given pre and post that assessed symptoms of anxiety and depression, including somatic symptoms such as fatigue and sluggishness. Although no mention was made of SCT, it can be assumed that the umbrella of ADHD-I and these symptoms of fatigue and sluggishness may give us enough of a measure to estimate the general treatment effects on SCT. According to the results, improvements were identified in all areas measured, but reductions in emotional distress , appeared to be mitigated by the initial level of emotional distress. The higher the initial level, the less likely the participant was to make gains in academic skills. The researchers hypothesized that the effect was due to the time required in session to work solely on emotional distress would, of necessity, displace efforts to improve study skills. It may also be that the sluggishness and fatigue so often shared by depression and SCT make it less likely that a participant will follow through at all with goals and between-session assignments. Similarly, as coaching focuses on the symptoms of ADHD rather than seeking to identify a cause .

Psychotherapy. According to a study by Antshel, Faraone, and Gordon (2012), adolescents with ADHD and oppositional defiant symptoms showed little improvement in their symptoms after undergoing CBT, while those adolescents with comorbid ADHD and anxiety/depression showed significant improvement. Based on what is known about the typical externalizing correlates of ADHD-H/I and the typical internalizing correlates of ADHD-I and

SCT, it is not extreme to infer that CBT may be a more successful treatment for adolescents with

SCT than those with ADHD. As SCT and depression/anxiety are so often comorbid and perhaps even share an etiology, effective therapies for depression and anxiety are logical approaches.

Somewhat more successful results have been found when adults with ADHD are treated using

38 cognitive behavioral therapy, especially when used to augment concurrent pharmacotherapy

(Safren et al., 2005). In the study by Safren and his colleagues, results strongly suggested that the group receiving both treatments fared better and in far greater numbers than those treated with pharmacotherapy alone.

Treatment difference overview. To summarize the ways in which ADHD and SCT appear to respond differently to treatment, ADHD-H/I tends to respond reasonably well to stimulants, while SCT and ADHD-I do not; coaching is generally effective for ADHD, but the internalizing symptoms typical of ADHD-I and SCT tend to impede progress; and psychotherapy tends to be much more effective when the individual has internalizing symptoms as in the case of

SCT and ADHD-I as opposed to the externalizations typical of ADHD-H/I. The treatment effectiveness can be graphed and superimposed, producing a curve as seen in Figure 11.

Figure 11. Illustration of inferred linear relationship between treatment outcomes and activity level based on correlational data. Results for three therapeutic approaches superimposed.

39

The U-shaped curve is again visible in the relationship between treatments and activity level, consistent with the proposed theory. The deficits associated with hyperactivity respond very differently to treatment than those associated with SCT and ADHD-I; supporting the conclusion that they are indeed distinct forms of inattention with different underlying causes.

Latent Structure Research of ADHD and SCT

Factor analysis is a statistical technique developed to identify latent structures by reducing items into related groups. It has been used in psychology since the beginning of the 20 th century (Spearman, 1904), and is a prime tool in identifying relationships among symptoms that might represent an underlying disorder or disorders. Factor analysis, as it has consistently been applied to ADHD and SCT throughout their history, presumes a linear model of data and cannot accurately describe a curvilinear or nonlinear model (Tay & Drasgow, 2012; Van Schuur &

Kiers, 1994). The use of linear factor analysis on nonlinear data would inadvertently bias the results, producing discrete factors as expected, though often extracting a general bipolar factor first depending on the rotation being used. This bipolar factor has previously been interpreted as a G-factor or general factor of ADHD, with various explanations presented (Gibbens,Toplak,

Flora, Weiss, & Tannock, 2011; Martell, Van Eye, & Nigg, 2010; Toplak, et al., 2009) yet none of the studies included the symptoms of SCT, and none noted the need for a multidimensional analysis of the data.

Proposed methods for accurately analyzing the dimensionality of this type of data include quadratic factor analysis (Maraun & Rossi, 2001), correspondence analysis (Polak, Heiser, & De

Rooij, 2009), and multidimensional scaling (Tay & Drasgow, 2012). Such methods have not yet been used in analyzing ADHD, and are still being examined for use in the social sciences.

Alternatively, single curvilinear regression should be able to identify a relationship between the

40 two constructs and determine whether the proposed U-shape exists. If the factor of inattention is viewed as a separate construct that is induced by both hyperactivity and sluggishness, rather than its own heterogeneous subtype of ADHD, then the relationship between hyperactivity and sluggishness becomes clearer.

Summary of Evidence

In summary, the construct of ADHD does not appear to currently match the factor structure of its symptoms due to the creation of the ADHD-C subtype (Span, Earleywine, &

Strybel, 2002: Willcutt et al., 2012), and the general heterogeneity of ADHD (Barkley, 2013;

Moruzzi, Rijsdijk, & Battaglia, 2013). The troubling heterogeneity of ADHD is found consistently in genetic studies (McCracken et al., 2000; Rowe et al., 1998; Yeh et al,, 2004); in neuroimaging studies ( Fair et al., 2012 ); and in treatment studies using stimulants (Barkley,

DuPaul, & McMurrey, 1991), psychotherapy (Antshel, Faraone, & Gordon, 2012), and ADHD coaching (Prevatt & Yelland, 2013).

Also, symptoms of cognitive sluggishness which have been known to be associated with

ADHD since 1984 (Lahey et al.) have not yet been integrated into the ADHD construct or any other construct in any official manner. This is despite the strong correlation between SCT and

ADHD-I (Hartman, Willcutt, Rhee, & Pennington, 2004), and even higher risks of depression and anxiety associated with SCT than with ADHD-I (Becker & Langberg, 2013; Lee et al.,

2014).

Hyperactivity and cognitive sluggishness are intuitively opposite in nature from each other, and show a negative correlation with each other after controlling for inattention (Penny et al., 2009). This difference can be seen as well in the inverse correlation they show with both externalizing behaviors (Lee et al., 2014) and internalizing behaviors (Becker & Langberg, 2013;

41

Lee et al., 2014). Therefore, since inattention is correlated with both hyperactivity and cognitive sluggishness, inattention seems to also show a troubling correlation with both more externalizing and less externalizing, and with more internalizing and less internalizing. This contradictory association may indicate that the construct of inattention is more accurately viewed as a separate dimension from hyperactivity, and that hyperactivity and sluggishness mark opposite ends of one continuum of activity, as illustrated previously in Figure 3.

Because of overlap between the early constructs of hyperactivity and inattention, the combined subtype was created for the DSM-IV (APA, 1994) to offset the diagnostic confusion.

This was not an attempt to rectify the factorial issues of heterogeneity. This unresolved construct of inattention is still in use as of the DSM 5 (APA. 2013), which means that any research done utilizing this construct may be subject to that inherent error, and may not have sufficient factorial and structural validity. Additionally, the dimensional nature of the proposed construct would indicate that linear methods of analysis are inappropriate for identifying the structure of ADHD.

It is important that this possibility be explored: that the constructs of inattention, hyperactivity, and sluggishness be analyzed in a way that is appropriate for nonlinear data, and that can determine whether a curvilinear model is correct. Consequently, the following research question was explored:

When SCT and Hyperactivity are analyzed together as a continuum of Activity

Level, does the continuum form a curvilinear relationship with Inattention?

This question was explored in two steps:

1. Creating a combined hyperactivity/sluggishness scale and testing the scale reliability of that measure to determine its appropriateness in answering the main research question.

42

2. Utilizing multiple curvilinear regression to test the underlying structure of the proposed ADHD model.

43

CHAPTER 3

METHODS

Introduction

The literature review explored the history of ADHD research and SCT research and the ways in which the two constructs have been shown to relate. The summary of the review identified two main issues: first, the indeterminate relationship between hyperactivity/impulsivity

(HY), and sluggishness (SL); and second, the inexplicable relationship they have with

Inattention. These two issues would potentially impact not only the categorization of these symptoms, but also have implications about the appropriateness of using the linear model of statistics in all of the past and future research on the subject.

In this chapter the primary research question is presented with the two steps that were used to explore it, and an explanation is given of how they relate to the two issues found in the literature review. As part of that explanation, it is demonstrated how the research question informed the creation of the instrument that was used, and process by which the instrument itself was created. The hypotheses being proposed will are also listed, as well as the analyses that were used to test them.

Research Question

When SCT and Hyperactivity are analyzed together as a continuum of Activity

Level, does the continuum form a curvilinear relationship with Inattention?

This question will be explored in two steps:

1. Create a combined hyperactivity/sluggishness scale and test the scale reliability of that measure to determine its appropriateness in answering the main research question.

44

2. Utilize multiple curvilinear regression to test the underlying structure of the proposed

ADHD model.

Hypothesis 1: The Activity Level scale will produce internal consistency scores

indicating that the construct is unidimensional.

Hypothesis 2: A significant quadratic curvilinear regression line will be found

when Inattention is regressed onto Activity Level.

Scale Development

In order to test the hypotheses, the HY and SL symptoms needed to be offered in a continuous format, and since the disorders of ADHD and SCT include impairments in various distinct areas, it was deemed appropriate to match the symptoms by area of impairment. The rationale behind splitting and matching the symptoms by area was three-fold: first, it might make it easier for participants to gauge their experience with smaller groups of related symptoms than with the full symptoms sets; second, participants may not experience symptoms of hyperactivity or sluggishness in all aspects of their lives; and third, the constructs of ADHD-H/I and SCT have been shown to be heterogeneous (Barkley, 2013; Jacobson et al., 2012; Moruzzi,

Rijsdijk, & Battaglia, 2013; Penny et al., 2009), and symptom subsets might not all behave the same way under analysis. This approach was thought to offer the possibility of analyzing the curvilinearity of the individual symptom groups should that be undertaken in the future.

The HY symptoms were taken straight from the DSM 5 H/I subtype, and the SCT symptoms were pulled mainly from the symptoms used by Penny and her colleagues in 2009.

Symptoms of impulsivity were not extricated from those of hyperactivity for the purposes of this study, as they seem to function as a single factor in factor analysis. Similarly, SCT has previously been broken down separate factors, but will be included as a whole for now. These

45

HY and SCT symptoms were then matched and subdivided based on the apparent topic and apparent relationship, as no similar research could be found which could inform the process. The procedure for doing this is described in the following paragraph.

Symptoms of SL such as slow movement and sluggishness were paired with HY symptoms such as fidget, tap, or squirm a great deal of the time , and were placed on opposite ends of a continuum that appeared to be related to physical movement. This process was conducted until all symptoms had been used, producing seven distinct continua with hyperactive symptoms on one end of each and sluggish symptoms on the other end.

The seven continua included the topics of physical movement, social interaction, patience, subjective level of activity or drive, participation in general conversations, participation when required to learn and retain information, and perceived level of energy. The seven items of the new measure were checked for face validity by an objective authority of ADHD research and evaluation, and approved. A copy of these items can be seen on pp. 77-79 in the appendices. A further description of the finished scale can be found in the Measures section (p. 53).

Procedures

The method used for collecting data was Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk; www.MTurk.com), a crowd-sourced, internet application, used for reaching large numbers of contributors inexpensively and rapidly. MTurk has come into increasingly common use for psychological research. As of 2011, Amazon reports having a database of over 500,000 workers from 190 different countries (Natal@AWS, 2011), and studies indicate that the participant pool is significantly more diverse than what is available from a typical college student population, and can produce data of comparable or better quality (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Krantz

& Dalal, 2000; Gosling et al., 2004). Recently, a study by Wymbs and Dawson (2011)

46 successfully used MTurk to identify adults with ADHD and found prevalence rates comparable to offline methods of analysis.

Some researchers caution that online research such as this can be vulnerable to participants who engage half-heartedly in the tasks, or who develop methods of scamming the researchers (Goodman, Cryder, & Cheema, 2013; Harms & DeSimone, 2015; McCreadie et al.,

2010). Based on the recommendations of these and other studies, various methods were used to ensure the quality of the data being collected. The first method was the sample size itself, as a sample size of around 1,500 participants not only increases the power of the analysis, it avoids the potential pitfall of having dedicated M-Turk users (who are most likely to respond carelessly) compose more than 5 to 10 percent of the sample (Harms & DeSimone, 2015).

The second method for ensuring the quality of the data was the inclusion of two attention-check items or reverse-Turing test questions, in which a verifiable answer is explicitly requested and required in order for the participant’s responses to be included in the analysis

(Kittur, Chi, & Suh, 2008; Oppenheimer, Meyvis, & Davidenko, 2009). Such questions mimicked the format of the items of interest, but had only a single correct answer which was stated in the question. The first attention question alone, being multiple-choice with 7 possible selections, had the potential to identify more than 85% of random responders or programs designed to mimic participants, and the second attention question, with four possible answers, had the potential to identify an additional 11% (Chandler, Paolacci, & Mueller, 2013). A third such check was included as well, in the form of an option for having received a diagnosis of

SCT, which, of course, has not been recognized by the APA as a valid disorder construct.

In order to reach the participants for the study, an advertisement was placed on the

Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) website offering the chance to participate in psychological

47 research about the relationship between hyperactivity, hypoactivity, and inattention. Only those

18 years and older who reside within the United States were eligible to participate, and repeated participation was not allowed. Those interested were offered a link to the online survey on

Qualtrics.com. In accordance with IRB approval and due to the anonymous nature of the data collection process, informed consent was inferred from the participant’s acceptance of the survey after having read a thorough description. Upon completing the survey, the participants were provided with an alphanumeric code to enter into the MTurk website. Those entering the correct code were compensated $0.20, and later the compensation was raised to $0.35 in order to match the survey length. An appended statement explained that the presence or absence of an ADHD diagnosis would not disqualify them for the survey nor impact their reimbursement, and that the questions are merely for the purposes of data analysis.

Participants

The initial sample estimate for this study was a total of 2,000 participants (see power analysis on p. 54) pulled from the general population of MTurk users within the US, with an estimated 100 of those likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD based on accepted prevalence rates of about 5% (DSM 5, APA, 2013). However, initial data collection efforts indicated that it was necessary for a few changes to be made. First, the wage per participant was increased from $0.20 to $0.35 for the 3.5-minute survey in order to match the preferred wage/time ratio. Second, mid- collection analysis of the ADHD prevalence indicated that there were close to three times the expected respondents reporting a diagnosis, and consequently data collection was terminated upon reaching a total of 1,606 responses. Respondents that missed any of the three attention check or validity check questions were eliminated from the analysis, leaving a final total of 1,443

48 respondents to be included in the analysis. Of those, 196 respondents endorsed having received a diagnosis of ADHD, or about 13.5% of the sample.

Rationale for Splitting the Sample

Because this study explored whether ADHD is better suited to construction and study using a dimensional model, it also begged the question of whether the general population and the clinical population differ in the nature of their inattention and activity levels or only in the severity of those symptoms. For that reason, the internal consistency analyses and the regression analyses were conducted using two groups of responses: first, the general population sample

(n= 1,398); and second, the portion of the full sample that endorsed having a diagnosis of ADHD

(n= 190). These two groups will be referred to from here on out as the full sample and the ADHD sample .

A comparison of US population and study sample demographics can be found in Table 1, and a comparison of demographics between the full sample and the ADHD portion of the sample can be found in Table 2. In summary, when compared to the US population, the overall sample had a significantly lower percentage of Hispanic/Latino and Black/African American participants, but a significantly higher percentage of Asian American and American Indian participants. There were comparable percentages of White participants and Hawaiian/Pacific

Islanders, as well as comparable percentages of males and females between the sample and the

US population. Individuals of 65 years and above were not well represented in this sample.

In comparing the full sample with the ADHD sample, there were no significant differences between them in terms of race, ethnicity, or age. However, there was a significantly higher percentage of males represented in the ADHD sample compared to the full sample, and a significantly lower percentage of females with a ratio of 1.28:1. This is in accordance with the

49

Table 1.

Sample and US Population Demographic Comparison and Z-test Significance Full Sample % Population % *p-value Race/Ethnicity White 77.4 77.1 .78 Hispanic/Latino 7.4 17.6 <.01 Black/African American 7.7 13.3 <.01 Asian American 8.8 5.6 <.01 (East Asian) (6.9) - (South Asian/Indian) (1.9) - American Indian 1.8 .9 <.01 Hawaiian/Pacific Is. .3 .2 .51

Sex Male 47.9 49.2 .32 Female 51.9 50.8 .40 (Prefer not to say) .3 -

Age (18+ only) 18-64 98.3 80.7 <.01 65+ 1.7 19.3 <.01 Note. Population statistics taken from US Census (2015), http://quickfacts.census.gov

*Significant at p = .05, however, using populations of this size (321,418,820 US population) can increase the Type I error, or inflate the significance of small differences, identifying statistically significant but practically insignificant differences.

Table 2.

Full Sample and ADHD Sample Demographic Comparison and Z -test Significance Full Sample % ADHD Sample% *p-value Race/Ethnicity White 77.4 80.1 .40 Hispanic/Latino 7.4 9.2 .38 Black/African American 7.7 5.1 .19 Asian American 8.8 8.2 .76 (East Asian) (6.9) (5.6) .49 (South Asian/Indian) (1.9) (2.6) .27 American Indian 1.8 3.1 .23 Hawaiian/Pacific Is. .3 .5 .58

Sex Male 47.9 55.6 <.05 Female 51.9 43.4 <.05 (Prefer not to say) .3 1.0 .11

Age (18+ only) 18-64 98.3 100 .06 65+ 1.7 0 .06 *Significant at p = .05

50 higher prevalence rates of ADHD found among adult males at 2.15:1 (Ramtekkar, Reiersen,

Todorov, & Todd, 2010), although to a lesser extent than might be estimated.

Measures

Activity Level

The seven items of the Activity Level measure were presented as 7-point scaled-response questions (Appendix A). The SL symptoms were placed at the lowest anchor, 1; the HY symptoms were placed at the highest anchor, 7; and the midpoint, 4, was labeled as Neither, I’m about average. As each of the seven items could have a maximum score 7, the possible range of the cumulative scale score was between 7 and 49, where 7 would indicate that the participant had rated themselves as being on the sluggish end of the spectrum the majority of the time in all areas, and 49 would indicate that they had rated themselves as hyperactive/impulsive the majority of the time in all areas.

Inattention

The nine items of the Inattention measure were presented as is typically seen on ADHD rating scales, in the form of 4-point scaled-response questions (Appendix A). The anchors included Never or Rarely at the lowest point of 1, Sometimes at 2, Often at 3, and Very Often at

4, giving a cumulative score that could range from 9 to 36. For this scale, 9 would indicate that the participant hated rated themselves as never experiencing symptoms of Inattention, and 36 would indicate an endorsement of experiencing each of the Inattention symptoms near continuously.

Demographics

The 11 remaining items were composed of demographic questions for collecting data about age, sex, education, and race/ethnicity. The breakdown of race/ethnicity that was used in

51 this study was taken from a research forum about culturally sensitive and appropriate demographic categorizations (Somerville, 2012) which matched fairly well in comparison with

US census procedures. This demographic section also included an inquiry about whether the respondent had received a diagnosis of ADHD and if so which subtype, whether they had been prescribed medication for ADHD, and questions about whether they had been diagnosed with or suspect the presence of other forms of psychopathology.

52

CHAPTER 4

RESULTS

Data Description/Statistical Assumptions

Power

Power analyses were performed using G-Power 3.1.7 (Faul, Lang, & Buchner, 2013).

According to the post hoc power calculations for linear multiple regressions with two predictors and with an observed effect size of f2 = .16, the full sample of n = 1,398 produces a power level of 1.0. The observed effect size was calculated using the R2 produced by the regression analysis.

The same calculations were performed for the ADHD portion of the sample and it was determined that an observed effect size of f2 = .06 and a sample size of n = 190 produce a power level of .86.

It is important to note that while the full sample size ( n =1,398) is drastically larger than what is needed for acceptable statistical power of .80 ( n = 64), the medium effect size of f2 = .16 that was observed indicates that the results remain practically significant. The ADHD sample size ( n = 190) was also larger than that required for statistical power (164), although not to such a drastic degree, and not overly inflating the chance of a Type I error.

Normality

Since regression is not robust to the presence of outliers, they were identified using the method of calculating the Mahalanobis distance squared and comparing it to a Chi Squared distribution. Of the 1,443 responses in the full sample, 46 fit the definition of an outlier, and were not used in the regression analysis and instead marked for further analysis (p. 61). Of the

196 responses in the ADHD sample, 6 fit the definition, and were not used.

53

With those outliers eliminated, measures of skewness, kurtosis, and overall normality of the residuals were conducted. Since the full sample is so large, measures of normality tend to be inaccurate, being prone to a Type II error of identifying abnormality where it is irrelevant.

Alternatively, such a large sample is robust to peculiarities of normality in the data, and will provide interpretable regression results regardless. For the smaller ADHD sample, normality of the residuals is of greater importance. The residuals of this sample were identified by the

Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Shapiro-Wilk tests as being statistically normal ( p = .20 , and p = .14 respectively), despite having a mild positive skew.

Descriptive statistics of the subgroups for the two main measures used in this study can be found in Table 3.

Table 3. Descriptive Statistics for Sample Groups Sample Group Full Sample No ADHD ADHD Variable n M SD n Mean SD n Mean SD Activity Level 1398 27.76 5.60 1220 27.57 5.28 178 29.06 7.31 Inattention 1398 15.95 5.43 1220 15.21 5.01 178 21.02 5.47

Frequency data for the Activity Level scale is presented in Figure 12 for the full sample and in Figure 13 for the ADHD sample. Measures of normality indicate that while both samples show some degree of skewness and kurtosis, they fall within the acceptable levels for psychometric purposes and can be considered normal.

Hypothesis 1

In the first hypothesis, it was proposed that tests of the internal consistency of the seven composite HY/SL items that make up the Activity Level scale would be initial evidence of the unidimensionality of the scale, with Cronbach’s α (Cronbach, 1951), and McDonald’s ω

54

Figure 12. Histogram of the Activity Level Frequency Distribution. Full Sample.

Figure 13. Histogram of the Activity Level Frequency Distribution. ADHD Sample. 55

(McDonald, 1999) being produced as an indicator of internal scale consistency. An α score in excess of 0.7 is not conclusive, but is considered by many to be indicative that the items of a scale have unidimensionality , or are all measuring the same construct. An issue with using α in this manner is that α assumes that each item on the scale gives an equal contribution (a tau- equivalent model), which is not always the case. McDonald’s ω makes no such assumption (a congeneric model) and has recently become a preferred measure to Cronbach’s α (Zinbarg,

Revelle, & Yovel, 2007) as an indicator of internal consistency. Although acceptable cutoff scores vary, an ω score above 0.7 is also considered acceptable for the field of psychology.

These scores were identified using the full sample, and also using the ADHD-only sample, as previously discussed on pp. 49 and 51.

For the ADHD sample, α = .80 and ω = .86 indicated that somewhere between 80% and

86% of the observed variance in the Activity Level scores can be attributed to the general factor of Activity Level, which is within the acceptable range for a psychological measure of a construct. Item-level analysis indicated that the removal of one specific item ( With regard to my patience… ) would raise the overall α slightly to .802, but the deletion of any of the other questions would only serve to lower the internal consistency of the scale.

For the full sample, α = .69 and ω = .79 indicated that somewhere between 69% and 79% of the observed variance in the Activity Level scores can be attributed to the general factor of

Activity Level. The deletion of items did not increase the α generated for this sample. These results, with the exception of the borderline α of .69 for the full sample, are consistent with the predictions of the first hypothesis. The cause of the discrepancy between the two samples on the measures of internal consistency is not known, but may in part be due to the greater heterogeneity of the full sample in terms of psychological disorders. The inclusion of participants

56 with symptoms that cycle between low activity and high activity, such as those typical of bipolar disorder, might affect the score. Similarly, issues of comorbidity between ADHD and anxiety issues may also affect the overall internal consistency for the full sample.

Hypothesis 2

In the second hypothesis it was proposed that a simple curvilinear regression would identify a curvilinear relationship between Activity Level scores and Inattention scores.

Specifically, it proposed that the curvilinear relationship will be U-shaped or parabolic in nature, which is described by a quadratic regression line. In order to identify the relationship between

Activity Level and Inattention, aggregate scores were calculated for each variable. The first analysis used a method of curve estimation to specify the most appropriate type of model equation for interpolating the data, whether it be linear, or quadratic. The Inattention score was used as the dependent or criterion variable, and the Activity Level score was used as the independent or predictor variable.

The predicted curvilinear relationship occurred for both data groupings (Table 4). The quadratic model was found to be significant for the full sample, F = 108.87, p < .001, and also for the ADHD sample, F = 5.68, p = .004, indicating that the model significantly explains a portion of the variance in the relationship between Activity Level and Inattention. The R 2 of the quadratic model, or an estimate of how much of the variance is being explained, varied between the groups (Table 1). For the full sample, representing the general population, the quadratic model explained about 14% of the variance. For the ADHD diagnosis group, representing the clinical population, about 6% of the variance was explained by the quadratic model. As an explanation of the quadratic coefficients seen in Table 4 (labeled Constant , b1 , and b2 ), each b

57 represents a slope, and more than one b indicating a change in slope, or a curve. The formula for the equation can be written from the coefficients provided in this form:

y = Constant + b1 x + b2 x2.

Thus, the equation for the quadratic interpolation line seen in the full sample is this:

y = 48.71 - 2.41x + .04x 2

Figure 10 will be useful for interpreting the following explanation. This quadratic equation contains three distinct parts that describe the behavior of the interpolation line: the constant or y- intercept, the first term, and the second term. The constant indicates that the interpolation line, extended beyond the meaningful confines of the data, crosses the y-axis at 48.71 . The first term,

-2.41x , is a negative slope of the interpolation line, which shows the initial drop in Inattention as the Activity Level increases from Sluggishness toward typical levels of activity. The second, or quadratic term of .04x 2 indicates that the interpolation line is actually a parabolic or U-shaped curve. This means that there is a gradual flattening of the slope, followed by a new positive slope in the interpolation line. This new slope shows that Inattention rises again as Activity Level increases from typical levels toward Hyperactivity.

The equation for the quadratic interpolation line seen in the ADHD sample is this:

y = 33.89 - .96x + .02x 2.

This quadratic equation follows that same pattern as with the General sample , indicating that the relationship between Inattention and Activity Level also follows a parabolic curve to some degree.

The linear model was not significant for the ADHD group, explaining none of the variance ( p = .40, R 2 = .00), and indicating no measurable, overall change in Inattention scores as

Activity Level scores increase or decrease. For the full sample, the linear model was significant

58

(p = .002), but only explained 0.7% of the variance, indicating a very slight drop in Inattention scores overall as Activity Level scores increased. This linear trend indicates that the parabola described by the quadratic equation has an overall tilt to the right. This would indicate a slight difference in the relationship between Inattention and Sluggishness compared to the relationship between Inattention and Hyperactivity. This slight difference would be seen as a steeper increase in Inattention as Activity Levels drop toward sluggishness than when Activity Levels rise toward hyperactivity. These trends can also be seen in Figure 14.

Table 4. Curve Estimation Sample Groupings Full Sample Model Summary Parameter Estimates Equation R2 F df1 df2 p* Constant b1 b2 Linear .007 9.854 1 1396 .002 18.199 -.081 Quadratic .135 108.865 2 1395 .000 48.706 -2.409 .043 ADHD Diagnosis Sample Model Summary Parameter Estimates Equation R2 F df1 df2 p* Constant b1 b2 Linear .004 .703 1 188 .403 22.450 -.041 Quadratic .057 5.677 2 187 .004 33.887 -.959 .017 *p < .05

Outlier Characteristics

The 46 outliers identified initially were analyzed in comparison to the remaining responses in order to identify a pattern. Due to the small sample of outliers and in order to minimize family-wise error, only two comparisons were made using t-tests: mean group score for endorsement of bipolar disorder, and mean group score for the endorsement of mental health symptoms overall. The Bipolar variable was chosen based on the idea that the presence of this type of disorder, which causes a wide variance in energy and activity levels, might obscure the expected relationship between symptoms of HY and symptoms of SL. The No Disorder variable

59 was chosen in order to see how the groups differed overall in levels of reported psychopathology.

Analyses indicated that the outliers differed significantly from the main sample on both variables, as can be seen in Table 5. The t-tests indicated that those individuals in the outlier group tended to endorse bipolar disorder at a higher rate, and also to endorse more diagnoses and symptoms overall. These analyses both demonstrated nonsignificant differences in group variance, medium effect sizes, and sufficient power for identifying the differences in group means, indicating that the differences that were found were both statistically and practically significant.

Figure 14. Scatterplots and interpolation lines produced by curve estimation, illustrating the curvilinear relationship found in both sample groups between Inattention and Activity Level.

Table 5. Comparison of Sample and Outlier Group Means Variable Group n M SD SEM Cohen’s D Power t Sig. (2-tailed)* Bipolar Outlier 46 .3043 .46522 .06859 0.56 .96 3.146 .003 Sample 1398 .0873 .28233 .00755 Med No Outlier 46 .2826 .45524 .06712 -0.57 .97 -3.992 .000 Disorder Sample 1398 .5558 .49706 .01329 Med *p < .001. 60

CHAPTER 5

DISCUSSION

The present study constitutes the first exploration to date of a continuous relationship between the hyperactivity encompassed by ADHD-H/I and the hypoactivity encompassed by

SCT. Additionally, it is the first study found that explores the possibility of a dimensional and curvilinear relationship between a continuum of activity level and the symptoms of inattention encompassed by ADHD-I. Its purpose was to find evidence to either support or reject this new construct, as such a reevaluation of the present construct may have implications for not only the body of ADHD research but for the diagnosis and treatment of ADHD as well. This was done using a large sample of adults across both sexes and from various ethnic and racial groups.

Previous studies have found that ADHD-H/I and SCT have an inverse correlation with each other, while both have a strong, positive correlation with ADHD-I. Those findings suggested that a strictly linear approach would not adequately describe the relationship between these disorders. One alternative explanation is that hyperactivity and sluggishness form opposite ends of a continuum of activity level, where the midpoint would represent an ideal level of activity. This continuum would be expected to have a curvilinear or quadratic relationship with inattention, where inattention is high at both endpoints, and low at the midpoint of activity level.

The following chapter is composed of a summary of this study’s results. A discussion of the practical implications for theory and practice are included as well as interpretive limitations and ideas for future research.

Internal Consistency

The first hypothesis presumes to determine whether hyperactivity and sluggishness are in fact endpoints of a single dimensional construct, activity level, by using measures of internal

61 consistency. Both the Cronbach’s α and the McDonald’s ω methods of measuring internal scale consistency produced scores within the acceptable range for psychological measure, consistent with the first hypothesis. This indicates that the participants responded to the group activity level items in a consistent manner, and that the overall variance appears to be due to a single common factor.

However, further interpretation of these results uncovers some obstacles. The first obstacle pertains to the α and ω scores, whether they are meaningful indicators of unidimensionality; and the second concerns the data itself, whether unidimensionality is even an appropriate expectation for a dimensional, curvilinear scale.

As stated by Barbaranelli, Lee, Vellone, & Riegel, (2015), “alpha is an accurate measure of reliability only if items measure the same construct and are sufficiently correlated.” In other words, Cronbach’s α should only be interpreted if you already know that the scale is unidimensional. Cronbach’s α and other measures based on the classical test theory are indicators of correlation amongst the test items, but not of unidimensionality. Similarly,

McDonald’s ω assumes that the items pertain to a single factor.

Unidimensionality of a measure would indicate a single construct, in this case, activity level. According to Revelle and Zinbarg (2009), unidimensionality is only one of four proposed qualities that a test may have that are related to dimensionality, the others being the presence of a general factor, the proportion of the variance attributable to that general factor, and the proportion of the variance attributable to all common factors. According to their manuscript, two possibly distinct, but highly related domains (in this case symptoms of hyperactivity and sluggishness), will likely not be measured as unidimensional, but instead as having a shared general factor (activity level). Although factor analysis might be the logical tool for measuring

62 this, it would not be useful for this study since the symptoms of hyperactivity and sluggishness have already been matched. Factor analysis would not be able to break down the composite items for analysis.

In summary, the Activity Level scale may not be unidimensional, and Cronbach’s α and

McDonald’s ω are not useful descriptors of unidimensionality unless the scale is already known to be unidimensional. However, unidimensionality may not even be the expected or appropriate characteristic for this data. Ultimately, while the analysis for hypothesis 1 produced the predicted results, the interpretation of the results is inconclusive.

An alternative analysis (one not included in the initial plan for this study) may provide evidence of whether sluggishness and hyperactivity can be validly combined into an Activity

Level scale. As the distribution of responses on the Activity Level scale is both normal and unimodal as opposed to bimodal, then the scale may be considered to describe a single construct as opposed to two separate constructs. This may be seen for both samples in the histograms of

Figures 8 and 9.

Again, Cronbach’s α would not necessarily indicate whether a scale is unimodal, only if the items are all correlated. In this case, Cronbach’s α would not be able to distinguish between whether the responses tended to fall at either end of the scale, or whether they fell primarily toward the middle.

Curvilinearity

The second hypothesis presumes to determine whether the measure of activity level and the measure of inattention have a quadratic curvilinear relationship, as predicted by the proposed model. While the unidimensionality of the activity level measure may be indeterminate, the curvilinear regression did indeed show a significant and meaningful quadratic curvilinear

63 relationship between the two constructs. According to these results there was a general pattern of higher levels of inattention whenever the activity level was closer to the high or low endpoints, and lower levels of inattention whenever the activity level was near the middle. While it is important to keep in mind that much of the variance (86% for the general sample and 96% for the ADHD sample) was not explained by this relationship, and many other factors remain unknown, the curvilinear pattern was nevertheless present as was hypothesized, and there is likely a benefit to be gained from examining the implications. Exploring the implications of this factor of variance should in no way interfere with or deter any exploration of other possible factors for predicting Inattention.

As stated before, this multidimensional arrangement of the ADHD and SCT symptoms is fully congruent with previous findings where inattention was positively correlated with both hyperactivity and sluggishness, despite the negative correlation between the latter two constructs.

Interestingly, the pattern was present both in the overall sample, and in the smaller portion reporting a diagnosis of ADHD.

As both the general sample and the clinical sample demonstrate the same relationship between activity level and inattention, then it seems unlikely that there is anything categorically different about those with ADHD and those without ADHD. An alternative to categorical differences in activity level and inattention would be that they differ only in terms of gradient or severity of symptoms, for which any categorization becomes somewhat arbitrary. This may indicate a general pattern that is not necessarily consistent with ADHD or SCT being distinct disorders, or that ADHD and SCT do not each describe a single pathology. In reviewing the symptoms of both ADHD and SCT, no symptoms are found that are exclusive to someone with a mental illness, as might be indicative of a unique pathology. Instead it may be more indicative of

64 the fact that activity level is influenced by a number of other environmental or genetic factors shared in common by those with ADHD, SCT, and by the population as a whole.

Theoretical Implications

The initial implications of these results are theoretical. The results seem to be consistent with an inactive line of research that found two types of inattention (Barkley et al., 1990;

Diamond, 2005; Lahey et al., 1988; Milich et al., 2001). However, the implications of this study diverge from the past conclusions.

New Model

Rather than splitting inattention into subtypes or distinct disorders, it allows for a multidimensional relationship with hyperactivity and sluggishness. If replicable, these results may dictate a restructuring of ADHD in a fashion that may be a significant departure from any done previously. Such a construct may best be represented by a continuum that does not lend itself easily to categorization, and may require an emphasis on subjective levels of impairment as opposed to quantification of present symptoms. Such a continuum would not necessarily preclude an individual from shifting their position on the continuum at any point, which is congruent with research showing that the ADHD subtypes are unstable over time (Lahey, et al.,

2005) and in fact leaves ample room for the appropriate inclusion of non-pathological fluctuations in energy/activity level.

Structure

The structural implications of this for research are far-reaching as well. Unidimensional and linear analyses, such as factor analysis and correlation, have been used almost exclusively in past ADHD research and particularly in the studies used to devise the various constructs of

ADHD. If the results of this study are an indication of the multidimensional nature of ADHD

65 symptoms, then these analyses are seemingly insufficient to the task of describing ADHD, and would likely give erroneous and deceptive results. While curvilinear regression seems capable of identifying this type of structure, other, more complex analyses may need to be utilized to further describe such constructs. Quadratic factor analysis is one such possibility, as well as multidimensional unfolding and multidimensional scaling. As an example, the multidimensional relationship indicated by this study resolves the 1994 issues with the poor negative predictive power (NPP) of SCT for inattention, which had led SCT symptoms to be excluded from the

DSM-IV construct of ADHD (Frick et al., 1994). These results would explain why sluggishness would likely have positive predictive power (PPP) without NPP, as sluggishness is not the only causal factor for inattention. The results appear to indicate that hyperactivity may also have different PPP and NPP for inattention, which is an avenue for further research.

Categorization

This study likewise has implications for the current view of ADHD in relation to comorbid issues such as behavioral disorders (DSM 5; APA, 2013). Instead of enforcing a distinction between them because of a weak overall correlation between externalizing behaviors and inattention (Lahey et al., 1984; Lahey et al., 1994; Lalonde, Turgay, & Hudson, 1998), inattention can be split between that caused by hyperactivity and that caused by sluggishness. In the past, this splitting of inattention has resulted in a strong, positive correlation between externalizing behaviors and hyperactivity and a strong, negative correlation between externalizing disorders and sluggishness. This could imply that behavioral disorders may be an integral part of the hyperactive end of ADHD, seen as a common result of increased risk-taking behaviors, increasing as activity levels increase toward unmanageable levels. Relatedly,

66 internalizing symptoms such as depression and anxiety may be more integral to the sluggish end of ADHD than has been explored before.

Clinical Implications

The results of this study seem to indicate that not all inattention has the same cause, and therefore that hyperactive and sluggish forms may require different therapeutic approaches to treatment. This is consistent with past research which indicated that hyperactivity and sluggishness may respond differently to non-pharmacological treatment (Antshel, Faraone, &

Gordon, 2012; Prevatt & Yelland, 2013). For the purposes of treatment, these results imply that a diagnosis of ADHD is insufficient for determining the most effective approach to treatment.

Conveniently, the multidimensional construct supported by these results allows for clinical and non-clinical levels of inattention without needing to create distinctions based on etiology. Many emotional, social, biological stressors, and even genetic diatheses could conceivably influence a person’s activity level and associated level of inattention to vary. Rather than assuming a direct link between hyperactivity and a genetic cause, other avenues would need to be explored in therapy for potential stressors that might be elevating risk-taking behaviors.

Similarly, symptoms of sluggishness would then be cause for exploring stressors that lead to energy depletion or to a reduction of healthy risk-taking behaviors.

Unless entirely without merit, these results may also require an abatement of what may be termed the pathologizing of functional bodily responses to environmental cues, akin to the sleep/wake circadian rhythm or the fight/flight arousal of stress. This possibility would align these results with the previously mentioned Yerkes-Dodson law of arousal and performance. An alternative diagnostic structure, analogous to issues with blood pressure, is a set of high and low cut-off points that may shift based on age or circumstance. As with blood pressure, the cut-off

67 points are seen as indictors of other issues, rather than issues in and of themselves. This view would accept some variation in activity level as normal, as variation in blood pressure is normal.

The cutoff points for blood pressure are determined by a consensus of what is an acceptable level of risk, rather than there being anything categorically different between the types of blood pressure just above and just below the cutoff points. High blood pressure, like hyperactivity, may cause problems in functionality, but it is not necessarily seen as the primary issue. Patterns of diet, exercise, and stress are explored as explanations for the high blood pressure and behavioral changes are recommended alongside pharmacological treatments. Similarly, this alternative structure for activity and inattention may indicate that behavioral and environmental causes for both low activity levels and high activity levels may be the better focus for attention and treatment.

Consequently, one thing that these findings do not seem to support is only using dopaminergic or other ADHD medication as the sole approach to treatment. If ADHD is simply a set of normal responses to a wide variety of stressors, a theory that has been proposed before

(Furman, 2008; Stolzer, 2009) and one that appears fully congruent with the continuous rather than categorical approach to viewing activity level, then the underlying issues may differ from case to case. Evidence suggests that current ADHD subtypes do not respond equally well to stimulant treatment (Barkley et al., 1991; Beery, Quay, & Pelham, 2017), and estimates of stimulant treatment resistance go from a tenth to half of those with ADHD (Barkley, 2016;

Wender, 1998), indicating that treatments should be more individually tailored to each client, with significant focus perhaps being placed on the context of the behavior.

A more widely recognized example of an adaptive response to variety of stressors would be the crying of an infant. If the treatment success is only measured by whether the infant has

68 stopped crying, then the crying itself is being treated as the problem, and not the underlying issues of hunger, sleepiness, fright, pain, or any other common complaint. A drug that silences the infant’s cries, such as the opium or whiskey of generations past, may cover up the true problems by only appearing to be a resolution.

If hyperactivity and sluggishness are indeed adaptive bodily responses to stressors, then the addition of stimulant medication may sometimes interfere with the body’s natural functioning. The hyperactivity/impulsivity may be the results of the body’s drive toward risk- taking (Ellis, et al., 2012), and while the stimulants may improve an individual’s ability to direct that impulse, it will have no direct impact on whatever caused the body’s adaptive response in the first place. This temporary success in reducing the inappropriate behaviors may mask any social, behavioral, physical, or other issues that might otherwise be helped through therapy. This result is consistent with the progressive deterioration of the stimulant’s effectiveness over time

(Molina et al., 2007; Molina et al., 2009; Swanson et al., 2007).

If sluggishness is the result of the body’s reduction in risky behavior and lower energy expenditure, then the addition of a stimulant would necessarily be counterproductive to its aims.

Whichever stressors are being dealt with or avoided by reducing risk and activity would persist instead, and the body’s response would be an attempt to counteract the effects of the stimulant.

This would likely be seen as a rapid reduction in the stimulant’s effectiveness due to the brains reduction in its receptor sites, followed by a repeated increase in necessary dosage. This would also likely lead to a dramatic difference in functioning when off of the medication or when withdrawing entirely, as the body would need to readjust its own dopamine production and the prevalence of its receptor sites. Again this is congruent with past research indicating lower effectiveness of stimulants in treating SCT-type inattention.

69

These implications are admittedly prone to subjectivity, and should therefore be vigorously disputed and reexamined in order to merit credibility. Considerations such as these are far from being proven or even fully examined by the current study and are not mentioned in this fashion. Instead, they represent possible avenues of consideration when examining the implications of a new structural model. Various theories exist on the etiology of ADHD, and the results of this study can and should be placed into the context of each in order to see the full implications. Such a job would not be possible for a single researcher to adequately complete, given the limited perspective and expertise.

Limitations

There were significant limitations to consider when assessing the viability and generalizability of this data, including limitations due to sampling, due to participant subjectivity, and possibly due to the formulation of the activity level measure itself.

The sample, while large, was self-selected from among adults with access to and the inclination for prolonged internet use. This inclination may have an impact on their symptomatology, or may in fact be a consequence of their symptoms. There may be a difference in ADHD and SCT symptom manifestation between those who would choose Amazon’s Mechanical Turk for a source of income, and those that would not choose it. The sample was also limited to those who could afford internet access, and who had the education and training to read the questions and operate the computer. In limiting the sample to those with workers with a high dependability rating, many individuals with behavioral issues or high impulsivity may have been excluded.

Individuals with low energy, low motivation, or social anxiety may also have been more reluctant to respond at all or at least with a full disclosure of their experiences. Using an exclusively adult sample of those 18 and older, these results may not represent in any way the

70 experiences of children and adolescents with symptoms of ADHD or SCT, and cannot be assumed to generalize to childhood ADHD.

The limitations caused by subjectivity, while currently unavoidable in ADHD research and in psychology as a whole, are nevertheless significant and should be mentioned. Diagnostics in psychology depend on self-reports, observation, and clinical expertise, and do not benefit from the simplicity of a blood test or even conclusivity through neural imaging. This means that there is much room for error, exaggeration, or even malingering, and this study was susceptible to those as well. There was no method of verifying the ADHD diagnoses reported by the participants nor the qualifications of the professional who gave the diagnosis, even if there was no inherent benefit to be gained by falsifying such a diagnosis for the survey. While precautions were taken to weed out deliberately or unintentionally careless responses, there was no method of standardizing the subjective severity of the symptoms being disclosed.

The activity level measure, created by combining symptoms from ADHD-H/I and from

SCT and matching them by topic, could have been matched and divided in a variety of ways.

There was no research precedent to which the measure could be readily compared, as no other measure has structured hyperactivity and sluggishness in such a fashion. Despite the participation and approval of an expert in adult ADHD and in the evaluation of ADHD, there is no guarantee that the process produced the ideal result. This is another example of an aspect of this study that requires review, replication, and elaboration.

Future Research

Subsequent research is warranted, and a number of avenues can be taken. It would be important to determine if the results are an artifact of the sampling procedures or the predominantly Caucasian sample, so other collection methods could be used to reach participants

71 outside the realm of Mechanical Turk, and participants from other countries, ethnicities and races. As mentioned earlier, other formulations of the activity level measure might be produced which could give different results, and those could be explored as well. Alternatively, the activity level measure could be left to its component ADHD and SCT symptoms, and, along with the symptoms of inattention, be analyzed using multidimensional analyses such as quadratic factor analysis as a way of clarifying the latent structure. Such methods might be used to better match the hyperactive and sluggish symptoms, or to elaborate upon the curvilinear relationship found in this study. If future research finds merit in the ideas proposed in this study, then many historical findings may need to be replicated using multidimensional analyses and with a new understanding of ADHD. This may require new approaches toward measuring correlations with comorbid disorders as well, and even adjustments to the methods of measuring ADHD prevalence. Considering the impact that ADHD and its associated impairments can have on the social, cognitive, emotional, educational, and even occupational development of an individual, it is indisputable that this disorder deserves further study.

72

APPENDIX A

STUDY SURVEY FORM

Attention and Activity - Form 1

A) Thank you for taking the time to complete this survey! It explores the experiences of attention and physical/cognitive activity, and how that relates to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, or

ADHD. As you complete this survey, you will remain anonymous, and your specific answers will be kept confidential. You will also be compensated equally, no matter your experience with

ADHD, so please answer honestly and to the best of your ability.

B1) Have you ever been diagnosed with ADHD or ADD? (Your answer will not end the survey, but is important for analyzing the data.)

ü Yes, I have a diagnosis (1) ü No, but I may have ADHD or ADD (2) ü No, and I don't think I have it (3)

B2) If you have been diagnosed with ADHD, what subtype is it?

ü ADHD Hyperactive/Impulsive type (ADHD) (1) ü ADHD Inattentive type (ADD) (2) ü ADHD Combined type (3) ü Sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT) (4) ü I don't know (5)

73

B3) Have you been prescribed medication for treating ADHD?

ü Yes, and I'm currently taking it (1) ü Yes, but I'm not taking it (2) ü No, but I use it (3) ü No, and I don't use it (4)

B4) Have you ever been diagnosed with:

ß Depression (1) ß Anxiety (2) ß Bipolar Disorder (3) ß Personality Disorder (4) ß Oppositional Defiant Disorder or Conduct Disorder (5) ß PTSD (6) ß Substance Use Disorder (7)

B5) Even if you don't have an official diagnosis, do you believe that you suffer from any of the following:

ß Depression (1) ß Anxiety (2) ß Bipolar Disorder (3) ß Personality Disorder (4) ß Oppositional Defiant Disorder or Conduct Disorder (5) ß PTSD (6) ß Substance Use Disorder (7)

B6 What is your biological sex?

ü Male (1) ü Female (2) ü Prefer not to say (3)

74

B7 What is your race or ethnicity? (Multiple answers can be given.)

ß Black, Afro-Caribbean, or African American (1) ß Non-Hispanic White or Euro-American (2) ß Latino or Hispanic American (3) ß East Asian or Asian American (4) ß South Asian or Indian American (5) ß Middle Eastern or Arab American (6) ß Native American or Alaskan Native (7) ß Hawaiian or Pacific Islander (8) ß Other (9)

B8) What is your age?

ß

B9) What is your level of education?

ß Some high school (1) ß High school graduate or GED (2) ß Some college credit (3) ß Undergraduate degree (4) ß Technical training (5) ß Some graduate level credit (6) ß Graduate degree (7)

C) CRITICAL : The following questions are designed to explore your experiences with the symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). It is important that you respond to

75 these questions in a way that describes your experiences before ever receiving medication for it, and without the effects of any other legal or illegal drugs.

D1) With regard to my movements:

ü I move slowly or sluggishly a great deal of the time (1) ü 2 (2) ü 3 (3) ü Neither, I'm about average (4) ü 5 (5) ü 6 (6) ü I fidget, tap , or squirm a great deal of the time (7)

D2) With regard to my interactions with others:

ü I'm withdrawn or disengaged from others a great deal of the time (1) ü 2 (2) ü 3 (3) ü Neither, I'm about average (4) ü 5 (5) ü 6 (6) ü I intrude on conversations a great deal of the time (7)

D3) With regard to my patience:

ü I'm not in a rush, instead I'm "spacey", lost in my thoughts, or oblivious a great deal of the time (1) ü 2 (2) ü 3 (3) ü Neither, I'm about average (4) ü 5 (5) ü 6 (6) ü I am impatient or have trouble waiting a great deal of the time (7)

76

D4) With regard to my activity level:

ü I feel apathy, am slow to complete tasks, or lack initiative a great deal of the time (1) ü 2 (2) ü 3 (3) ü Neither, I'm about average (4) ü 5 (5) ü 6 (6) ü I feel driven as if by a motor a great deal of the time (7)

D5) In conversations:

ü I get lost in my own thoughts, daydream, or don't participate a great deal of the time (1) ü 2 (2) ü 3 (3) ü Neither, I'm about average (4) ü 5 (5) ü 6 (6) ü I talk excessively a great deal of the time (7)

D6) Just making sure:

ü Select choice #2 to indicate that you are reading this. Sorry for the deception (1) ü 2 (2) ü 3 (3) ü Neither, I'm about average (4) ü 5 (5) ü 6 (6) ü Don't forget to choose #2 on this question (7)

77

D7) When I'm required to listen to someone talk:

ü I'm mentally foggy, or I don't understand or process questions as quickly or accurately as others a great deal of the time (1) ü 2 (2) ü 3 (3) ü Neither, I'm about average (4) ü 5 (5) ü 6 (6) ü I blurt out answers before the questions have been completed a great deal of the time (7)

D8) Regarding my energy level:

ü I'm lethargic, drowsy, or underactive compared to others a great deal of the time (1) ü 2 (2) ü 3 (3) ü Neither, I'm about average (4) ü 5 (5) ü 6 (6) ü I feel very restless, can't stop moving, or can't stay seated a great deal of the time (7)

E1) I fail to give close attention to details or make careless mistakes doing work.

ü Never or Rarely (1) ü Sometimes (2) ü Often (3) ü Very Often (4)

E2) I have difficulty sustaining attention in tasks or fun activities.

ü Never or Rarely (1) ü Sometimes (2) ü Often (3) ü Very Often (4)

78

E3) I don't listen when spoken to directly.

ü Never or Rarely (1) ü Sometimes (2) ü Often (3) ü Very Often (4)

E4) I don't follow through on instructions and fail to finish my work.

ü Never or Rarely (1) ü Sometimes (2) ü Often (3) ü Very Often (4)

E5) I have difficulty organizing tasks and activities.

ü Never or Rarely (1) ü Sometimes (2) ü Often (3) ü Very Often (4)

E6) I avoid, dislike, or am reluctant to engage in work that requires sustained mental effort.

ü Never or Rarely (1) ü Sometimes (2) ü Often (3) ü Very Often (4)

E7) I lose things that are necessary for tasks and activities.

ü Never or Rarely (1) ü Sometimes (2) ü Often (3) ü Very Often (4)

79

E8) I become easily distracted.

ü Never or Rarely (1) ü Sometimes (2) ü Often (3) ü Very Often (4)

E9) I should select "Often" for this question.

ü Never or Rarely (1) ü Sometimes (2) ü Often (3) ü Very Often (4)

E10) I become forgetful in daily activities.

ü Never or Rarely (1) ü Sometimes (2) ü Often (3) ü Very Often (4)

80

APPENDIX B

TABLE OF SPECIFICATIONS

Table of Specifications for the Survey Items

Content # of Items Item #

Demographics 9 B1-B9

Activity Level 7 D1-D5, D7, D8

-- ADHD-H/I D1-D5, D7, D8

-- Hyperactivity D1-D5, D7, D8

-- Impulsivity D2, D3, D5, D7

-- SCT D1-D5, D7, D8

Inattention 9 E1-E8, E10

Attention Check 2 D6, E9

81

APPENDIX C

HUMAN SUBJECTS APPROVAL

Office of the Vice President For Research Human Subjects Committee Tallahassee, Florida 32306-2742 (850) 644-8673 · FAX (850) 644-4392

APPROVAL MEMORANDUM Date: 5/9/2016 To: Nathan Miller Dept.: EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY AND LEARNING SYSTEMS From: Thomas L. Jacobson, Chair Re: Use of Human Subjects in Research Redefining ADHD: Should Inattention be Viewed as a Separate Dimension from Cognitive and Physiological Activity Level?

The application that you submitted to this office in regard to the use of human subjects in the proposal referenced above have been reviewed by the Secretary, the Chair, and one member of the Human Subjects Committee. Your project is determined to be Expedited per per 45 CFR § 46.110(7) and has been approved by an expedited review process.

The Human Subjects Committee has not evaluated your proposal for scientific merit, except to weigh the risk to the human participants and the aspects of the proposal related to potential risk and benefit. This approval does not replace any departmental or other approvals, which may be required.

If you submitted a proposed consent form with your application, the approved stamped consent form is attached to this approval notice. Only the stamped version of the consent form may be used in recruiting research subjects.

If the project has not been completed by 5/8/2017 you must request a renewal of approval for continuation of the project. As a courtesy, a renewal notice will be sent to you prior to your expiration date; however, it is your responsibility as the Principal Investigator to timely request renewal of your approval from the Committee.

You are advised that any change in protocol for this project must be reviewed and approved by the Committee prior to implementation of the proposed change in the protocol. A protocol change/amendment form is required to be submitted for approval by the Committee. In addition, 82 federal regulations require that the Principal Investigator promptly report, in writing any unanticipated problems or adverse events involving risks to research subjects or others.

By copy of this memorandum, the Chair of your department and/or your major professor is reminded that he/she is responsible for being informed concerning research projects involving human subjects in the department, and should review protocols as often as needed to insure that the project is being conducted in compliance with our institution and with DHHS regulations.

This institution has an Assurance on file with the Office for Human Research Protection. The Assurance Number is FWA00000168/IRB number IRB00000446.

Cc: Frances Prevatt, Advisor HSC No. 2016.18104

83

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.) . Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., revised) . Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.) . Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.) . Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

American Psychiatric Association. Task Force on DSM-IV. (1991). DSM-IV options book: Work in progress (7/1/91) . American Psychiatric Press.

Andersen, S. L., & Teicher, M. H. (2000). Sex differences in dopamine receptors and their relevance to ADHD. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews , 24 (1), 137-141.

Antshel, K. M., Faraone, S. V., & Gordon, M. (2012). Cognitive behavioral treatment outcomes in adolescent ADHD. FOCUS: The Journal of Lifelong Learning in Psychiatry , 10 (3), 334-345.

Antshel, K. M., Faraone, S. V., Stallone, K., Nave, A., Kaufmann, F. A., Doyle, A., ... & Biederman, J. (2007). Is attention deficit hyperactivity disorder a valid diagnosis in the presence of high IQ? Results from the MGH Longitudinal Family Studies of ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 48 (7), 687-694.

Barbaranelli, C., Lee, C. S., Vellone, E., & Riegel, B. (2015). The problem with Cronbach's alpha: comment on Sijtsma and van der Ark (2015). Nursing Research , 64 (2), 140.

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.

Barkley, R. A. (2011). Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV) . Guilford Press.

Barkley, R. A. (2012). Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 121 (4), 978.

84

Barkley, R. A. (2013). Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from ADHD in children and adolescents: executive functioning, impairment, and comorbidity. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology , 42 (2), 161-173.

Barkley, R. A. (2014). Sluggish cognitive tempo (concentration deficit disorder?): Current status, future directions, and a plea to change the name. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 42 (1), 117-125.

Barkley, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., & McMurray, M. B. (1990). Comprehensive evaluation of attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity as defined by research criteria. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58 , 775–789.

Barkley, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., & McMurray, M. B. (1991). Attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: Clinical response to three dose levels of methylphenidate. Pediatrics , 87 (4), 519-531.

Bauermeister, J. J., Barkley, R. A., Bauermeister, J. A., Martínez, J. V., & McBurnett, K. (2012). Validity of the sluggish cognitive tempo, inattention, and hyperactivity symptom dimensions: Neuropsychological and psychosocial correlates. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 40 (5), 683-697.

Bauermeister, J. J., Matos, M., Reina, G., Salas, C. C., Martínez, J. V., Cumba, E., & Barkley, R. A. (2005). Comparison of the DSM ‐IV combined and inattentive types of ADHD in a school ‐based sample of Latino/Hispanic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 46 (2), 166-179.

Beck, A. T., & Clark, D. A. (1988). Anxiety and depression: An information processing perspective. Anxiety Research , 1(1), 23-36.

Becker, S. P., & Langberg, J. M. (2013). Sluggish Cognitive Tempo among young adolescents with ADHD relations to mental health, academic, and social functioning. Journal of Attention Disorders , 17 (8), 681-689.

Becker, S. P., Langberg, J. M., Luebbe, A. M., Dvorsky, M. R., & Flannery, A. J. (2014). Sluggish cognitive tempo is associated with academic functioning and internalizing symptoms in college students with and without attention ‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology , 70 (4), 388-403.

Becker, S. P., Marshall, S. A., & McBurnett, K. (2014). Sluggish cognitive tempo in abnormal child psychology: An historical overview and introduction to the Special Section. Journal of abnormal child psychology , 42 (1), 1-6.

Beery, S. H., Quay, H. C., & Pelham Jr, W. E. (2017). Differential response to methylphenidate in inattentive and combined subtype ADHD. Journal of attention disorders , 21 (1), 62-70.

85

Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Milberger, S., Jetton, J. G., Chen, L., Mick, E., ... & Russell, R. L. (1996). Is childhood oppositional defiant disorder a precursor to adolescent conduct disorder? Findings from a four-year follow-up study of children with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 35 (9), 1193-1204.

Biederman, J., & Pliszka, S. R. (2008). Modafinil improves symptoms of attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder across subtypes in children and adolescents. The Journal of Pediatrics , 152 (3), 394-399.

Biederman, J., & Spencer, T. (1999). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) as a noradrenergic disorder. Biological Psychiatry , 46 (9), 1234-1242.

Botteron, K. C., Carter, C., Castellanos, F. X., Dickstein, D. P., Drevets, W., Kim, K. L., ... & Zubieta, J. K. (2012). Consensus report of the APA work group on neuroimaging markers of psychiatric disorders. American Psychiatric Association .

Brookes, K., Xu, X., Chen, W., Zhou, K., Neale, B., Lowe, N., ... & McGuffin, P. (2006). The analysis of 51 genes in DSM-IV combined type attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Association signals in DRD4, DAT1 and 16 other genes. Molecular Psychiatry , 11 (10), 934-953.

Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon's Mechanical Turk a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data?. Perspectives on psychological science, 6(1), 3-5.

Burns, G. L., Servera, M., Bernad, M. D. M., Carrillo, J. M., & Cardo, E. (2013). Distinctions between sluggish cognitive tempo, ADHD-IN, and depression symptom dimensions in Spanish first-grade children. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology , 42 (6), 796-808.

Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience , 14 (5), 365-376.

Capdevila-Brophy, C., Artigas-Pallarés, J., Navarro-Pastor, J. B., García-Nonell, K., Rigau- Ratera, E., & Obiols, J. E. (2012). ADHD predominantly inattentive subtype with high sluggish cognitive tempo: A new clinical entity? Journal of Attention Disorders , 1087054712445483.

Carlson, C. L. (1986). Attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: A review of preliminary experimental evidence. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology, vol. 9 (pp. 153–175). New York: Plenum.

Carlson, C. L., Lahey, B. B., & Neeper, R. (1986). Direct assessment of the cognitive correlates of attention deficit disorders with and without hyperactivity. Journal of Behavioral Assessment and Psychopathology , 8, 69–86.

86

Carlson, C. L., & Mann, M. (2002). Sluggish cognitive tempo predicts a different pattern of impairment in the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, predominantly inattentive type. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31 (1), 123-129.

Carlson, C. L., Shin, M., & Booth, J. (1999). The case for DSM ‐IV subtypes in ADHD. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews , 5(3), 199-206.

Castells, X., Ramos ‐Quiroga, J. A., Bosch, R., Nogueira, M., & Casas, M. (2011). for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults. The Cochrane Library, DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD007813.pub2.

Castellanos, F. X., Lee, P. P., Sharp, W., Jeffries, N. O., Greenstein, D. K., Clasen, L. S., ... & Rapoport, J. L. (2002). Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA , 288 (14), 1740-1748.

Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Milham, M. P., & Tannock, R. (2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 10 (3), 117-123.

Castellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience , 3(8), 617-628.

Caterino, L. C., Gomez-Benito, J., Balluerka, N., Amador-Campos, J. A., & Stock, W. A. (2009). Development and validation of a scale to assess the symptoms of attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adults. Psychological Assessment, 21 , 152–161.

Chandler, J., Paolacci, G., & Mueller, P. (2013). Risks and rewards of crowdsourcing marketplaces. In Handbook of human computation (pp. 377-392). Springer New York.

Cherkasova, M. V., Faridi, N., Casey, K. F., O'Driscoll, G. A., Hechtman, L., Joober, R., ... & Benkelfat, C. (2013). -induced dopamine release and neurocognitive function in treatment-naive adults with ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39 , 1498– 1507.

Collings, R. D. (2003). Differences between ADHD inattentive and combined types on the CPT. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment , 25 (3), 177-189.

Comings, D. E., Gade ‐Andavolu, R., Gonzalez, N., Blake, H., Wu, S., & MacMurray, J. P. (1999). Additive effect of three noradrenergic genes (ADRA2A, ADRA2C, DBH) on attention ‐deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disabilities in Tourette syndrome subjects. Clinical Genetics , 55 (3), 160-172.

Conners, C. K. (1969). A teacher rating scale for use in drug studies with children. American journal of Psychiatry , 126 (6), 884-888.

87

Coppen, A. (1967). The biochemistry of affective disorders. The British Journal of Psychiatry , 113 (504), 1237-1264.

Counts, C. A., Nigg, J. T., Stawicki, J. A., Rappley, M. D., & Von Eye, A. (2005). Family adversity in DSM-IV ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes and associated disruptive behavior problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 44 (7), 690-698.

Crichton, A. (1798). An Inquiry into the Nature and Origin of Mental Derangement: Comprehending a Concise System of the Physiology and Pathology of the Human Mind and a History of the Passions and Their Effects . London: T. Cadell Hr. & W. Davies. Reprinted by AMS Press, New York, 1976.

Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika , 16 (3), 297-334. del Campo, N., Fryer, T. D., Hong, Y. T., Smith, R., Brichard, L., Acosta-Cabronero, J., ... & Müller, U. (2013). A positron emission tomography study of nigro-striatal dopaminergic mechanisms underlying attention: implications for ADHD and its treatment. Brain , 136 (11), 3252-3270.

Derefinko, K. J., Adams, Z. W., Milich, R., Fillmore, M. T., Lorch, E. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2008). Response style differences in the inattentive and combined subtypes of attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 36 (5), 745-758.

Diamond, A. (2005). Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder (with hyperactivity). Development and Psychopathology , 17 (03), 807-825.

Douglas, V. I. (1972). Stop, look and listen: the problem of sustained attention and impulse control in hyperactive and normal children. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne des Sciences du Comportement , 4(4), 259.

Doyle, A. E., Faraone, S. V., Seidman, L. J., Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Waldman, I. D., ... & Biederman, J. (2005). Are endophenotypes based on measures of executive functions useful for molecular genetic studies of ADHD? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 46 (7), 774-803.

Eifert, G. H., Evans, I. M., & McKendrick, V. G. (1990). Matching treatments to client problems not diagnostic labels: A case for paradigmatic behavior therapy. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry , 21 (3), 163-172.

Ellis, B. J., Del Giudice, M., Dishion, T. J., Figueredo, A. J., Gray, P., Griskevicius, V., ... & Wilson, D. S. (2012). The evolutionary basis of risky adolescent behavior: implications for science, policy, and practice. Developmental psychology , 48 (3), 598.

88

Fair, D. A., Nigg, J. T., Iyer, S., Bathula, D., Mills, K. L., Dosenbach, N. U., ... & Milham, M. P. (2012). Distinct neural signatures detected for ADHD subtypes after controlling for micro-movements in resting state functional connectivity MRI data. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience , 6, 80. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00080.

Faraone, S. V., & Biederman, J. (2005). What is the prevalence of adult ADHD? Results of a population screen of 966 adults. Journal of Attention Disorders , 9(2), 384-391.

Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Spencer, T. J., & Aleardi, M. (2006). Comparing the efficacy of medications for ADHD using meta-analysis. Medscape General Medicine , 8(4), 4.

Faraone, S. V., & Buitelaar, J. (2010). Comparing the efficacy of stimulants for ADHD in children and adolescents using meta-analysis. European child & adolescent psychiatry , 19 (4), 353-364.

FDA, (2006). Psychopharmacologic drugs advisory committee (Meeting minutes). Retrieved from http://www.fda.gov/ohrms/dockets/ac/06/transcripts/2006-4212T1-Part1.htm.

Field, S., Parker, D., Sawilowsky, S., & Rolands, L. (2010). College well-being scale. Detroit, MI: Wayne State University College of Education.

Field, S., Parker, D., Sawilowsky, S., & Rolands, L. (2013). Assessing the impact of ADHD coaching services on university students learning skills, self-regulation, and well-being. Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability , 26 , 67-81.

Fredriksen, M., Halmøy, A., Faraone, S. V., & Haavik, J. (2013). Long-term efficacy and safety of treatment with stimulants and in adult ADHD: a review of controlled and naturalistic studies. European Neuropsychopharmacology , 23 (6), 508-527.

Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., Kerdyck, L., Ollendick, T., Hynd, G. W., Garfinkel, B., Greenhill, L., Biederman, J., Barkley, R. A., McBurnett, K., Newcorn, J., & Waldman, I. (1994). DSM-IV field trials for the disruptive behavior disorders: Symptom utility estimates. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry , 33 (4), 529–539.

Froehlich, T. E., Lanphear, B. P., Epstein, J. N., Barbaresi, W. J., Katusic, S. K., & Kahn, R. S. (2007). Prevalence, recognition, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a national sample of US children. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine , 161 (9), 857-864.

Furman, L. M. (2008). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Does new research support old concepts? Journal of Child Neurology , 23 (7), 775-784.

89

Fusar-Poli, P., Rubia, K., Rossi, G., Sartori, G., & Balottin, U. (2012). Striatal dopamine transporter alterations in ADHD: pathophysiology or adaptation to psychostimulants? A meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry , 169 (3), 264-272.

Garner, A. A., Marceaux, J. C., Mrug, S., Patterson, C., & Hodgens, B. (2010). Dimensions and correlates of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and sluggish cognitive tempo. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 38 (8), 1097-1107.

Gaub, M., & Carlson, C. L. (1997). Behavioral characteristics of DSM-IV ADHD subtypes in a school-based population. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 25 (2), 103-111.

Gershon, J., & Gershon, J. (2002). A meta-analytic review of gender differences in ADHD. Journal of attention disorders, 5(3), 143-154.

Geurts, H. M., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). ADHD subtypes: Do they differ in their executive functioning profile? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology , 20 (4), 457-477.

Goodman, J. K., Cryder, C. E., & Cheema, A. (2013). Data collection in a flat world: The strengths and weaknesses of Mechanical Turk samples. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26(3), 213-224.

Gosling, S., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. (2004). Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about internet questionnaires. American Psychologist, 59, 93–104.

Guidi, J., Fava, G. A., Picardi, A., Porcelli, P., Bellomo, A., Grandi, S., ... & Sonino, N. (2011). Subtyping depression in the medically ill by cluster analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders , 132 (3), 383-388.

Graetz, B. W., Sawyer, M. G., Hazell, P. L., Arney, F., & Baghurst, P. (2001). Validity of DSM- IV ADHD subtypes in a nationally representative sample of Australian children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 40 (12), 1410-1417.

Graham, D. M., Crocker, N., Deweese, B. N., Roesch, S. C., Coles, C. D., Kable, J. A., ... & Mattson, S. N. (2013). Prenatal alcohol exposure, attention ‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and sluggish cognitive tempo. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research , 37 (s1), E338-E346.

Grizenko, N., Paci, M., & Joober, R. (2010). Is the inattentive subtype of ADHD different from the combined/hyperactive subtype? Journal of Attention Disorders, 13 (6), 649-657.

Hartman, C. A., Willcutt, E. G., Rhee, S. H., & Pennington, B. F. (2004). The relationship between sluggish cognitive tempo and DSM-IV ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32 (5), 491-503.

90

Hartnett, D. N., Nelson, J. M., & Rinn, A. N. (2004). Gifted or ADHD? The possibilities of misdiagnosis. Roeper Review , 26 (2), 73-76.

Hebebrand, J., Dempfle, A., Saar, K., Thiele, H., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Linder, M., ... & Konrad, K. (2005). A genome-wide scan for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in 155 German sib-pairs. Molecular Psychiatry , 11 (2), 196-205.

Heninger, G. R., Delgado, P. L., & Charney, D. S. (1996). The revised monoamine theory of depression: A modulatory role for monoamines, based on new findings from monoamine depletion experiments in humans. Pharmacopsychiatry , 29 (01), 2-11.

Hinshaw, S. P. (1987). On the distinction between attentional deficits/hyperactivity and conduct problems/aggression in child psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin , 101 (3), 443.

Hyman, S. E. (2010). The diagnosis of mental disorders: The problem of reification. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology , 6, 155-179.

Jacobson, L. A., Murphy-Bowman, S. C., Pritchard, A. E., Tart-Zelvin, A., Zabel, T. A., & Mahone, E. M. (2012). Factor structure of a sluggish cognitive tempo scale in clinically- referred children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 40 (8), 1327-1337.

Kaneko, M., Hoshino, Y., Hashimoto, S., Okano, T., & Kumashiro, H. (1993). Hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal axis function in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders , 23 (1), 59-65.

Kass, S. J., Wallace, J. C., & Vodanovich, S. J. (2003). Boredom proneness and sleep disorders as predictors of adult attention deficit scores. Journal of Attention Disorders , 7(2), 83-91.

Kendell, R., & Jablensky, A. (2003). Distinguishing between the validity and utility of psychiatric diagnoses. American Journal of Psychiatry , 160 (1), 4-12.

Kessler, R., Adler, L., Barkley, R., Biederman, J., Conners, C., Demler, O., ... & Zaslavsky, A. (2006). The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. American Journal of Psychiatry , 163 (4), 716-723.

Kittur, A., Chi, E. H., & Suh, B. (2008). Crowdsourcing user studies with Mechanical Turk. In Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 453– 456). New York, NY: ACM.

Konrad, K., Gauggel, S., Manz, A., & Schöll, M. (2000). Inhibitory control in children with traumatic brain injury (TBI) and children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Brain injury , 14 (10), 859-875.

91

Krantz, J. H., & Dalal, R. (2000). Validity of web-based psychological research. In M. H. Birnbaum (Ed.), Psychological experiments on the Internet (pp. 35–60). New York: Academic Press.

Kuntsi, J., Eley, T. C., Taylor, A., Hughes, C., Asherson, P., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2004). Co ‐occurrence of ADHD and low IQ has genetic origins. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics , 124 (1), 41-47.

Khurshid, K., Walker, K., Riley, A., & Wellington, R. (2014). Evidence for executive and processing speed deficits in adults with childhood sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology , 29 (6), 540-541.

Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., McBurnett, K., Biederman, J., Greenhill, L., Hynd, G. W., ... & Shaffer, D. (1994). DMS-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 151 , 1673-1685.

Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Schaughency, E. A., Atkins, M. S., Murphy, A., & Hynd, G. (1988). Dimensions and types of attention deficit disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27 , 330–335.

Lahey, B. B., Schaughency, E., Strauss, C., & Frame, C. (1984). Are attention deficit disorders with and without hyperactivity similar or dissimilar disorders? Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry , 23 , 302–309.

Lahey, B. B., Schaughency, E. A., Hynd, G. W., Carlson, C. L., & Nieves, N. (1987). Attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: Comparison of behavioral characteristics of clinic-referred children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 26 (5), 718-723.

Lahey, B. B., & Willcutt, E. G. (2010). Predictive validity of a continuous alternative to nominal subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder for DSM–V. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology , 39 (6), 761-775.

LaHoste, G. J., Swanson, J., Wigal, S. B., Glabe, C., Wigal, T., King, N., & Kennedy, J. L. (1996). Dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphism is associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry , 1(2), 121-124.

Lalonde, J., Turgay, A., & Hudson, J. I. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder subtypes and comorbid disruptive behaviour disorders in a child and adolescent mental health clinic. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie , 43 (6), 623- 628.

Langberg, J. M., Becker, S. P., Dvorsky, M. R., & Luebbe, A. M. (2014). Are sluggish cognitive tempo and daytime sleepiness distinct constructs? Psychological Assessment . Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036276

92

Lee, S., Burns, G. L., Snell, J., & McBurnett, K. (2014). Validity of the sluggish cognitive tempo symptom dimension in children: Sluggish cognitive tempo and ADHD-inattention as distinct symptom dimensions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 42 (1), 7-19.

MacMaster, F.P., Carrey, N., Sparkes, S., & Kusumakar, V. (2003). Proton spectroscopy in medication-free pediatric attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 53 , 184–187.

Maraun, M. D., & Rossi, N. T. (2001). The extra-factor phenomenon revisited: Unidimensional unfolding as quadratic factor analysis. Applied Psychological Measurement , 25 (1), 77-87.

March, J., Silva, S., Petrycki, S., Curry, J., Wells, K., Fairbank, J., ... & Severe, J. (2004). Fluoxetine, cognitive-behavioral therapy, and their combination for adolescents with depression: Treatment for Adolescents With Depression Study (TADS) randomized controlled trial. JAMA , 292 (7), 807-820.

Marshall, S. A., Evans, S. W., Eiraldi, R. B., Becker, S. P., & Power, T. J. (2014). Social and academic impairment in youth with ADHD, predominately inattentive type and sluggish cognitive tempo. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 42 (1), 77-90.

Marshall, R. M., Hynd, G. W., Handwerk, M. J., & Hall, J. (1997). Academic underachievement in ADHD subtypes. Journal of Learning Disabilities , 30 (6), 635-642.

Martel, M. M., Von Eye, A., & Nigg, J. T. (2010). Revisiting the latent structure of ADHD: is there a ‘g’factor?. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 51 (8), 905-914.

Maurer, R. G., & Stewart, M. A. (1980). Attention deficit without hyperactivity in a child psychiatry clinic. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry , 41 (7), 232-233.

Mayes, S. D., Calhoun, S. L., & Crowell, E. W. (2000). Learning disabilities and ADHD overlapping spectrum disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities , 33 (5), 417-424.

McBurnett, K. (2010). Kiddie-Sluggish Cognitive Tempo diagnostic interview module for children and adolescents. San Francisco, CA: Author .

McBurnett, K., Pfiffner, L. J. & Frick, P. J. (2001). Symptom properties as a function of ADHD type: An argument for continued study of sluggish cognitive tempo. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29 (3), 207-213.

McBurnett, K., Villodas, M., Burns, G. L., Hinshaw, S. P., Beaulieu, A., & Pfiffner, L. J. (2014). Structure and validity of sluggish cognitive tempo using an expanded item pool in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 42 (1), 37-48.

93

McCreadie, R. M., Macdonald, C., & Ounis, I. (2010, July). Crowdsourcing a news query classification dataset. In Proceedings of the ACM SIGIR 2010 workshop on crowdsourcing for search evaluation (CSE 2010) (pp. 31-38).

McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A unified treatment. L. Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, N.J.

McLoughlin, G., Ronald, A., Kuntsi, J., Asherson, P., & Plomin, R. (2007). Genetic support for the dual nature of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: substantial genetic overlap between the inattentive and hyperactive–impulsive components. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 35 (6), 999-1008.

Mendels, J., Stinnett, J. L., Burns, D., & Frazer, A. (1975). Amine precursors and depression. Archives of General Psychiatry , 32 (1), 22-30.

Menkes, M. M., Rowe, J. S., & Menkes, J. H. (1967). A twenty-five year follow-up study on the hyperkinetic child with minimal brain dysfunction. Pediatrics , 39 (3), 393-399.

Milich, R., Balentine, A. C., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD combined type and ADHD predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 463–488.

Milioni, A. L. V., Chaim, T. M., Cavallet, M., de Oliveira, N. M., Annes, M., dos Santos, B., ... & Cunha, P. J. (2014). High IQ may “mask” the diagnosis of ADHD by compensating for deficits in executive functions in treatment-naïve adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders , 1087054714554933.

Molina, B. S., Flory, K., Hinshaw, S. P., Greiner, A. R., Arnold, L. E., Swanson, J. M., ... & Wigal, T. (2007). Delinquent behavior and emerging substance use in the MTA at 36 months: prevalence, course, and treatment effects. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 46 (8), 1028-1040.

Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 7(3), 134-140.

Morgan, A. E., Hynd, G. W., Riccio, C. A., & Hall, J. (1996). Validity of DSM-IV ADHD predominantly inattentive and combined types: Relationship to previous DSM diagnoses/subtype differences. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 35 (3), 325-333.

Moruzzi, S., Rijsdijk, F., & Battaglia, M. (2014). A twin study of the relationships among inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity and sluggish cognitive tempo problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 42 (1), 63-75.

MTA Cooperative Group. (2004). National Institute of Mental Health Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD follow-up: Changes in effectiveness and growth after the end of treatment. Pediatrics , 113 (4), 762-769.

94

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2011). Mplus User's Guide. Sixth Edition . Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Natala@AWS (2011, January 26). MTurk CENSUS: About how many workers were on Mechanical Turk in 2010? Message posted to https://forums.aws.amazon.com/thread.jspa?threadID=58891.

Neale, M., & Cardon, L. (Eds.). (1992). Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families (No. 67). Springer.

Neeper, R., & Lahey, B. B. (1986). The children’s behavior rating scale: A factor analytic developmental study. School Psychology Review, 15 , 277–288.

Nesse, R. M. (2000). Is depression an adaptation? Archives of General Psychiatry , 57 (1), 14-20.

Nigg, J. T., Blaskey, L. G., Stawicki, J. A., & Sachek, J. (2004). Evaluating the endophenotype model of ADHD neuropsychological deficit: Results for parents and siblings of children with ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 113 (4), 614.

Øie, M., Skogli, E. W., Andersen, P. N., Hovik, K. T., & Hugdahl, K. (2014). Differences in cognitive control in children and adolescents with combined and inattentive subtypes of ADHD. Child Neuropsychology , 20(1), 38-48.

Penny, A. M., Waschbusch, D. A., Klein, R. M., Corkum, P., & Eskes, G. (2009). Developing a measure of sluggish cognitive tempo for children: Content validity, factor structure, and reliability. Psychological Assessment , 21 (3), 380.

Pfiffner, L. J., Yee Mikami, A., Huang-Pollock, C., Easterlin, B., Zalecki, C., & McBurnett, K. (2007). A randomized, controlled trial of integrated home-school behavioral treatment for ADHD, predominantly inattentive type. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 46 (8), 1041-1050.

Pliszka, S. R. (1989). Effect of anxiety on cognition, behavior, and stimulant response in ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 28 (6), 882-887.

Polak, M., Heiser, W. J., & De Rooij, M. (2009). Two types of single-peaked data: Correspondence analysis as an alternative to principal component analysis. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis , 53 (8), 3117-3128.

Pondé, M. P., Cruz-Freire, A. C., & Silveira, A. A. (2011). Relationship between learning problems and attention deficit in childhood. Journal of attention disorders , 1087054711423625.

95

Prevatt, F., & Yelland, S. (2013). An empirical evaluation of ADHD coaching in college students. Journal of Attention Disorders , Published online before print March 18, 2013, doi: 10.1177/1087054713480036.

Qualtrics, P. UT, USA.

R Core Team (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/.

Reeves, C. B., Palmer, S., Gross, A. M., Simonian, S. J., Taylor, L., Willingham, E., & Mulhern, R. K. (2007). Brief report: Sluggish cognitive tempo among pediatric survivors of acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Journal of Pediatric Psychology , 32 (9), 1050-1054.

Rowe, D. C., Stever, C., Giedinghagen, L. N., Gard, J. M., Cleveland, H. H., Terris, S. T., ... & Waldman, I. D. (1998). Dopamine DRD4 receptor polymorphism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 3(5), 419-426.

Safren, S. A. (2005). Cognitive-behavioral approaches to ADHD treatment in adulthood. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry , 67 , 46-50.

Safren, S. A., Otto, M. W., Sprich, S., Winett, C. L., Wilens, T. E., & Biederman, J. (2005). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for ADHD in medication-treated adults with continued symptoms. Behavior Research and Therapy, 43, 831-842.

Satterfield, J. H., Hoppe, C. M., & Schell, A. M. (1982). A prospective study of delinquency in 110 adolescent boys with attention deficit disorder and 88 normal adolescent boys. The American Journal of Psychiatry , 139 (6), 795-798.

Saxbe, C., & Barkley, R. A. (2014). The second attention disorder? Sluggish cognitive tempo vs. attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Update for clinicians. Journal of Psychiatric Practice , 20 (1), 38-49.

Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology , 80 (1), 1-27.

Schwarz, A. (2014). Idea of new attention disorder spurs research and debate. The New York Times .

Skansgaard, E. P., & Burns, G. L. (1998). Comparison of DSM-IV ADHD combined and predominantly inattention types: Correspondence between teacher ratings and direct observations of inattentive, hyperactivity/impulsivity, slow cognitive tempo, oppositional defiant, and overt conduct disorder symptoms. Child & Family Behavior Therapy , 20 (1), 1-14.

96

Skirbekk, B., Hansen, B. H., Oerbeck, B., & Kristensen, H. (2011). The relationship between sluggish cognitive tempo, subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 39 (4), 513-525.

Somerville, W. (2012, February 26). What is the best way to ask study participants about race & ethnicity? Message posted to https://www.researchgate.net/post/What_is_the_best_way_to_ask_study_participants_ab out_race_ethnicity.

Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Welcome, S. E., Henkenius, A. L., Toga, A. W., & Peterson, B. S. (2003). Cortical abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Lancet , 362 (9397), 1699-1707.

Span, S. A., Earleywine, M., & Strybel, T. Z. (2002). Confirming the factor structure of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in adult, nonclinical samples. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment , 24 (2), 129-136.

Spearman, C. (1904). "General intelligence," objectively determined and measured. The American Journal of Psychology , 15 (2), 201-292.

Stolzer, J. M. (2009). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Valid medical condition or culturally constructed myth? Ethical Human Psychology and Psychiatry , 11 (1), 5-15.

Stubbe, D. E. (2000). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder overview. Historical perspective, current controversies, and future directions. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America , 9(3), 469-79.

Swanson, J. M., Hinshaw, S. P., Arnold, L. E., Gibbons, R. D., Marcus, S. U. E., Hur, K., ... & Wigal, T. (2007). Secondary evaluations of MTA 36-month outcomes: Propensity score and growth mixture model analyses. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 46 (8), 1003-1014.

Tay, L., & Drasgow, F. (2012). Theoretical, statistical, and substantive issues in the assessment of construct dimensionality: Accounting for the item response process. Organizational Research Methods , 1094428112439709.

Thayer, J. F., Friedman, B. H., & Borkovec, T. D. (1996). Autonomic characteristics of generalized anxiety disorder and worry. Biological Psychiatry , 39 (4), 255-266.

Todd, R. D., Rasmussen, E. R., Wood, C., Levy, F., & Hay, D. A. (2004). Should sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms be included in the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 43 (5), 588-597.

Toplak, M. E., Pitch, A., Flora, D. B., Iwenofu, L., Ghelani, K., Jain, U., & Tannock, R. (2009). The unity and diversity of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity in ADHD: evidence

97

for a general factor with separable dimensions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 37 (8), 1137-1150.

U.S. Census Bureau. (2014, November 12). State & county Quickfacts: USA, Population, 2013 estimate. Retrieved November 12, 2014, from http://quickfacts.census.gov.

U.S. Census Bureau. (2015). United States Quickfacts. Retrieved July 14, 2016, from http://quickfacts.census.gov.

Valo, S., & Tannock, R. (2010). Diagnostic instability of DSM–IV ADHD subtypes: Effects of informant source, instrumentation, and methods for combining symptom reports. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology , 39 (6), 749-760.

Van Apeldoorn, F. J., Van Hout, W. J. P. J., Mersch, P. P. A., Huisman, M., Slaap, B. R., Hale, W. W., ... & Den Boer, J. A. (2008). Is a combined therapy more effective than either CBT or SSRI alone? Results of a multicenter trial on panic disorder with or without agoraphobia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica , 117 (4), 260-270.

Van Schuur, W. H., & Kiers, H. A. (1994). Why factor analysis often is the incorrect model for analyzing bipolar concepts, and what model to use instead. Applied Psychological Measurement , 18 (2), 97-110.

Volk, H. E., Neuman, R. J., & Todd, R. D. (2005). A systematic evaluation of ADHD and comorbid psychopathology in a population-based twin sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(8), 768-775.

Ward Jr, J. H. (1963). Hierarchical grouping to optimize an objective function. Journal of the American Statistical Association , 58(301), 236-244.

Weinstein, C. E., Schulte, A. C. & Palmer, D. R. (2002). Learning and Study Strategies Inventory. Clearwater, FL: H and H Publishing.

Wender, P. H. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America , 21 (4), 761-774.

Weikard, M. A. (1775). Observationes medicae . impensis Andreaeis.

Wietecha, L., Williams, D., Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., Hooper, S. R., Wigal, S. B., Dunn, D., & McBurnett, K. (2013). Atomoxetine improved attention in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and dyslexia in a 16 week, acute, randomized, double-blind trial. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology , 23 (9), 605- 613.

Weinberg, W. A., & Brumback, R. A. (1992). The myth of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Symptoms resulting from multiple causes. Journal of Child Neurology . 7, 431– 445.

98

Weiss, M., Worling, D., & Wasdell, M. (2003). A chart review study of the inattentive and combined types of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders , 7(1), 1-9.

Westerberg, H., Hirvikoski, T., Forssberg, H., & Klingberg, T. (2004). Visuo-spatial working memory span: A sensitive measure of cognitive deficits in children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychology , 10 (3), 155-161.

Willard, V. W., Hardy, K. K., Allen, T. M., Hwang, E. I., Gururangan, S., Hostetter, S. A., & Bonner, M. J. (2013). Sluggish cognitive tempo in survivors of pediatric brain tumors. Journal of Neuro-oncology , 114 (1), 71-78.

Willcutt, E. G., Chhabildas, N., Kinnear, M., DeFries, J. C., Olson, R. K., Leopold, D. R., ... & Pennington, B. F. (2014). The internal and external validity of sluggish cognitive tempo and its relation with DSM–IV ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology , 42 (1), 21- 35.

Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., ... & Lahey, B. B. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 121 (4), 991.

Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Comorbidity of reading disability and attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder differences by gender and subtype. Journal of Learning Disabilities , 33 (2), 179-191.

Wymbs, B. T., & Dawson, A. E. (2015). Screening Amazon’s Mechanical Turk for adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders , Doi.1087054715597471.

Yang, R., & Li, R. (2014). Could atomoxetine improve sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms? Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology .

Yeh, M., Morley, K. I., & Hall, W. D. (2004). The policy and ethical implications of genetic research on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry , 38 (1 ‐2), 10-19.

Zinbarg, R.E., Revelle, W., and Yovel, I. (2007). Estimating ωh for structures containing two group factors: Perils and prospects. Applied Psychological Measurement , 31(2) , 135–157.

99

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

Nathan Miller is a doctoral candidate in the Combined Doctoral Program for Counseling

Psychology and School Psychology at Florida State University (FSU). Nathan earned his

Bachelor of Science in Psychology with a minor in Spanish from Brigham Young University –

Idaho and his Master of Arts in Psychology from Western Carolina University. Following his

Bachelor’s degree, Nathan worked for three years as a Psychosocial Rehabilitation Specialist with children and teenagers using behavioral techniques to reduce conduct issues. His graduate work brought experiences in various elementary, middle, and high schools around Cullowhee,

NC, as well as clinical work with college students and community members at FSU’s Human

Services Center, FSU’s Career Center, Tallahassee Memorial Hospital’s (TMH) Behavioral

Health Center and Employee Assistance Program, Dr. Nancy Wonder’s private office,

Tallahassee Community College’s (TCC) Mental Health Services, and Florida A&M

University’s (FAMU) Office of Counseling Services.

Nathan’s research interests include the structural analysis of behavioral and attentional disorders, how they develop and overlap, and how to better blend the theoretical aspects of diagnosis with the practical responsibilities of Mental Health Professionals. His clinical interests lie in conducting therapy with college students using cognitive, behavioral, and emotion focused approaches. Nathan will continue his work with the college student population during his APA accredited internship with Texas State University’s Counseling Center in San Marcos, TX.

100