<<

Optimizing Alarm Notification for High Risk Groups Research Project

Summary report

Prepared by: Steven M.V.. Gwynne, Ph.D. Hughes Associates, Inc.

THE RESEARCH FOUNDATION

■■..,. - ..,. y_,- ., .. .,...... --- ■ _.,. -- _.,. ---.....11 __.aw_,_..._, ·-•··----··--- ■ • ■ :------■-• ■ --.._-A...... ----y -11...... • a..-A •-

THE FIRE PROTECTION RESEARCH FOUNDATION ONE BATTERYMARCH PARK QUINCY, , U.S.A. 02169 E-MAIL: [email protected] WEB: www.nfpa.org/Foundation

© Copyright The Fire Protection Research Foundation June 2007 FOREWORD

In April of 2006, the Foundation was awarded a Fire Prevention and Safety Grant by the US Fire Administration to study the effectiveness of alarms for emergency notification of high risk groups. The study's aim was to optimize the performance requirements for alarm and signaling systems to meet the needs of these groups. Elements of the study included: a risk assessment to estimate the potential impact in lives saved of changes in notification effectiveness of smoke alarms for these groups; quantifying the human behavior aspects of the problem; developing benchmark performance criteria for alarm and signaling systems; reviewing current and emerging technologies that address the performance criteria; and assessing the information developed in the above tasks to develop recommendations on notification technology for each target group and the overall impact for the general population. This report is one in a series of four that report on the on the results of the study.

The Research Foundation expresses gratitude to the report author Steven Gwynne of Hughes Associates, Inc.; the Project Technical Panelists listed on the following page; and to the United States Fire Administration, the project sponsor.

The content, opinions and conclusions contained in this report are solely those of the author. Optimizing Fire Alarm Notification for High Risk Groups Research Project

Technical Panel Rose Coniglio, Illinois Department on Aging Stephen DiGiovanni, Clark County (NV) Joshua Elvove, General Services Administration Rita Fahy, NFPA Bruce Fraser, SimplexGrinnell Wendy Gifford, Invensys Controls/Firex C.hantal Laroche, University of Ottawa Arthur Lee, U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission Dana Mulvany Guylene Proulx, National Research Council of Canada Rodger Reiswig, SimplexGrinnell Lee Richardson, NFPA Robert Schifiliti, RP. Schifiliti Associates, Inc. John Woycheese, Worcester Polytechnic Institute

Project Sponsor United States Fire Administration Grant No. EMW-2005-FP-01258

Optimizing Fire Alarm Notification for High Risk Groups

Prepared for:

The Fire Protection Research Foundation 1 Batterymarch Park Quincy, MA 02169-7471

Prepared by:

Steven M.V. Gwynne, Ph.D., Hughes Associates, Inc. 3515 28th St., #307, Boulder, Colorado 80301, USA

Reviewed by: David Boswell, SET and David Tomecek, PE Hughes Associates, Inc. 2 Garden Center, Suite 204 Broomfield, Colorado 80020 USA

June 27, 2007

1

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY

The three key objectives of this project were to identify vulnerabilities; to facilitate research on the effectiveness of notification technologies for addressing these vulnerabilities; and to provide recommendations, given the findings produced. In order to achieve these objectives, a number of novel developments had to be made.

Firstly, in order to identify vulnerabilities, it was felt necessary to develop a vulnerability model (derived inductively, from the bottom-up) rather than adopt a traditional high-level statistical approach. This was required given the difficulties found in reliably recording, storing and interrogating fire data. The development of this model required an exhaustive literary review producing an extensive list of vulnerabilities. As part of the model a metric was developed that enabled the categorization of the vulnerabilities found and improved our understanding of key vulnerability attributes. A similar approach was adopted when examining the notification technologies available.

The current empirical data relating to the effectiveness of notification technologies was examined. This outlined our current understanding of their capabilities. By examining the recommendations for future work made by other researchers and through guidance provided by the Technical Panel, the set of vulnerabilities were further prioritized enabling us to focus on three of them: people with an alcohol impairment; people who are hard of hearing; and the general population in public spaces. A risk assessment was performed in order to establish the potential impact of reducing these three vulnerabilities. However, the assessment was hampered by the analytical (as opposed to statistical) nature of the model employed.

The three data collection activities were conducted. The work conducted by Bruck, Thomas and Ball addressed a domestic situation where the participants were asleep and impaired, either through alcohol or through an existing hearing disability. The work conducted by Gwynne addressed non-domestic situations where those involved were awake.

Bruck, Thomas and Ball established the importance of adopting a low frequency square wave T-3 (LFSW) signal in spaces where people sleep. During this work, the effectiveness of this signal was established for the alcohol impaired and those with a hearing disability. This was consistent with previous research regarding other populations including children and older adults. Other technologies examined were less effective; e.g. strobes and vibrating devices presented in isolation. Bruck, Thomas and Ball identified several key areas for future work: 1. That the technical feasibility of replacing the current high frequency smoke alarm T-3 signal with a low frequency square wave T-3 (LFSW) signal, for the entire population, be investigated. 2. That the T-3 pattern duration (ON/OFF pattern) be further investigated. 3. That the strobe should not be considered as a viable means of independent notification for people asleep. 4. That the combined use of signals be investigated (e.g. auditory T-3 plus one other), in conjunction with the development of a modular kit where combinations of these signals can be delivered.

2

5. That there should be further investigation of an appropriate means of measuring the intensity of bed and pillow shakers and that this inform a new standard.

In public spaces, Gwynne clearly established the importance of both alerting and informing the population. This importance increased where preparation was not possible and the effectiveness of the emergency procedure was not guaranteed. Gwynne identified several key areas of future work: 6. Guidance should be provided on the use of a tone/voice combination in public spaces. This is generally important, but critical when preparation is not possible. The tone employed (e.g. low frequency square wave T-3, swooping tone, broadband, etc.) should be examined along with the content and format of the voice message provided including whether the voice announcement is live or pre-recorded. Clear guidance should be supplied on the information provided in such messages, making reference to literature associated with mass notification systems. 7. The technical feasibility of utilizing existing means of visual (and tactile) notification should be explored for public spaces. The potential for the emergency notification system to exploit existing non-emergency communication (e.g. large screens, televisions, monitors, paging, etc.) should also be examined.

These findings were derived directly from the three data collection activities. Building on these findings, and on the rest of the work conducted during this project, a simple framework was developed recommending the notification systems to be implemented. This was sensitive to the potential for providing information to a population before and during an incident, rather than just to the type of occupancy involved.

3

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This project was conducted for the Fire Protection Research Foundation under a grant from the U.S. Fire Administration. This work reflects a collaborative effort between three principal organizations, Victoria University, the National Fire Protection Association (NFPA) and Hughes Associates, Inc. The author gratefully acknowledges the contribution of the Technical Panel, John Hall, Ian Thomas and Dorothy Bruck. I would also like to thank Dave Boswell and Dave Tomecek from Hughes Associates, Inc. for their assistance in producing this document.

4

CONTENTS

Executive Summary 2 Acknowledgements 4 1. Introduction 8 2. Approach Adopted to Identify Vulnerabilities 10 3. Stage I: Literature Review 15 4. Stage I: Identification of Vulnerabilities 47 5. Stage I: Categorization of the Notification Process 53 6. Stage I: Notification Technology 63 7. Stage I: Recommendations for Stage II Research Activities 72 8. Stage II: Research Activity – Risk Assessment 76 9. Stage II: Research Activity – People who are Alcohol Impaired 87 10. Stage II: Research Activity – People who are Hard of Hearing 87 11. Stage II: Research Activity – Large Groups in Public Spaces 91 12. Discussion 95 13. Conclusions and Recommendations 104 14. References 106 15. Notification Technology Links 120 16. Appendix A: Request for Proposals - Large Groups 122 17. Appendix B: Request for Proposals - Alcohol Impaired 123 18. Appendix C: Request for Proposals - Hearing Impaired 124

5

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1: Architectural/Structural Factors the might influence performance...... 18 Table 2: Scenario/Environmental Factors the might influence performance...... 19 Table 3: Organizational Factors the might influence performance...... 19 Table 4: Procedural Factors the might influence performance...... 20 Table 5: Individual attributes the might influence performance...... 21 Table 6: Potential actions that can be performed in response to an incident...... 21 Table 7: Examples of Notification Systems...... 27 Table 8: Examples of factors that can interfere with the two notification technologies [127]...... 28 Table 9: Results produced by Shields and Boyce [99]...... 35 Table 10: Results produced by Proulx [79]...... 35 Table 11: Results produced by Purser [96]...... 36 Table 12: Results produced by Purser [96]. Categorization modified from original text...... 36 Table 13: Risk Factors Reported by Fahy and Molis, categorized according to Age Group [31]...... 41 Table 14: How respondent was alerted [95] ...... 44 Table 15: Presence, activity and effectiveness of . [95] ...... 44 Table 16: Categorization of the different vulnerability groups...... 54 Table 17: Primary and Secondary impact of vulnerabilities...... 56 Table 18: Recommended topics of further research related to vulnerabilities...... 58 Table 19: Recommended topics of further research related to experimental design...... 59 Table 20: Recommended topics of further research related to addressing the vulnerability...... 59 Table 21: Recommended topics of further research related to issues beyond notification...... 59 Table 22: Empirical data on the effectiveness of notification technologies according to the vulnerabilities examined...... 60 Table 23: Categorization of Notification Technology...... 69 Table 24: Example of the categorization of various notification approaches. Rc indicates the ability to improve receipt and recognition; Rc(adv) Rp indicates the ability to improve ...... 70 Table 25: Summary of the key vulnerabilities identified...... 72 Table 26: Assessment of the potential impact of notification technologies upon vulnerabilities...... 73 Table 27: Data collection scenarios recommended to the Technical Panel...... 74 Table 28: Revised data collection scenarios...... 75 Table 29: Home Fire Deaths for Victims Impaired by Alcohol or Other Drugs,...... 77 Table 30: Sensitivity of ratio to A...... 79 Table 31: Levels of deafness and hearing impairment according to age...... 81 Table 32: Fatalities according to levels of deafness/hearing impairment and age...... 81 Table 33: Data Collected during this activity...... 91 Table 34: Information provisions available to each type of occupancy. Black indicates that the method is available; grey indicates a limited version of this method is available...... 92

6

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1: The method used to develop the vulnerability model...... 11 Figure 2: Hypothetical incident with ‘contributing’ factors. [116]...... 12 Figure 3: Overall approach adopted during this project...... 13 Figure 4: The ASET/RSET comparison, derived from Purser [94,96]...... 16 Figure 5: Sources of influence upon the evacuation process...... 17 Figure 6: Issues with the notification process. [125] ...... 25 Figure 7: The continuum of effectiveness in notification. This ranges from an absence of information or the provision of inaccurate information to the presence of a well-trained and assertive member of staff. In between there are a range of different notification technologies available.[ 127]...... 26 Figure 8: Floor plan of Gothenburg Disco building in which the incident took place. Redrawn from [132]...... 30 Figure 9: Results compiled by Fahy and Proulx [32]...... 34 Figure 10: Intoxication levels produced in the Minnesota and Maryland studies according to age. [3]...... 37 Figure 11: Fatalities according to age and the presence of alcohol. [136]...... 38 Figure 12: Fatalities according to age and the proportion of fatalities due with recorded BAL.[ 136] ...... 38 Figure 13:Risk indicators and risk factors identified by Leth.[ 137] ...... 39 Figure 14: Overall deaths and intoxication categorized by age group [31] ...... 42 Figure 15: Overall deaths and disabilities categorized by age group. [31]...... 42 Figure 16: Combined description of the factors that might influence egress performance...... 45 Figure 17: Simple model of the source, content and influence of surrounding information...... 46 Figure 18: Focus of this analysis...... 52 Figure 19: (a) Primary effect of the vulnerability; (b) Secondary effect of the vulnerability [297] ...... 55 Figure 20: The factors leading to vulnerabilities...... 57 Figure 21: The Modular Approach. If the inclusion of a component is dependent on other factors (such as the level of preparation or availability of technology), then a question mark is included. If the impact of the component is also dependent on these factors, the font size is reduced...... 101

7

1 Introduction

In recent years advances have been made in the development and implementation of notification systems. The developments have had varied impacts, some of which were dependent upon a number of issues beyond the notification system itself; these include the connectivity of the systems in place and issues of maintenance and reliability. In designing and implementing such systems it is critical to first understand their effectiveness at notifying the population, and then focus their technological development toward the needs of the occupants or communities served. As with any varied population, notification systems must address these needs in as broad a scope as possible so as to not favor one section over the remainder. This is especially important given that vulnerabilities can be dynamic and can propagate throughout a population.

This project represents an attempt to identify vulnerabilities and then assess which of the available notification technologies is appropriate to address them. The primary purpose was to identify these vulnerabilities and suggest areas of research required to support these technologies; a secondary purpose was to structure our understanding of the effectiveness of such systems in order to make comparisons between them. From this effort, a number of recommendations are made regarding the application of the notification technologies examined.

The Fire Protection Research Foundation was awarded a Fire Prevention and Safety Grant by the US Fire Administration in order to determine the effectiveness of alarms for emergency notification of high risk groups. The study aims to optimize the performance requirements for alarm and signaling systems to meet the needs of these groups. In order to do this it needs to first identify groups deemed to be vulnerable to fire. This has been achieved by performing a number of tasks: Review current literature of relevant human behavior studies – This established the current understanding of the response of vulnerable populations. Review current and emerging technologies that address the performance criteria – This established the notification methods currently available. For each of these methods, the particular benefit to high risk groups, as well as the general population, was determined. Assess the information compiled to develop recommendations on notification technology for each target group and the overall impact for the general population –this described the current notification tools available and identified the conditions under which their use is most advantageous. Monitoring of a risk assessment exercise in order to estimate the potential impact in lives saved due to changes in notification effectiveness of notification technologies - This risk assessment has been undertaken by the National Fire Protection Associations’ Research Division, directed by Dr. John Hall. Direct recommendations have been provided on the factors expected to influence the effectiveness of alarms, and the manner in which these factors may interact.

The general aims of this project were therefore to: Identify vulnerable populations and establish the behavioral issues underlying these vulnerabilities; Determine the notification systems (and associated practices) that may reduce these vulnerabilities;

8

Synthesize these findings and develop hypotheses to assess the effectiveness of specific notification systems; Develop research strategies to test these hypotheses, which will directly influence the performance of subsequent data collection projects; and Enable technical recommendations to be made on the application of notification systems based on the results of these tests and our review.

A key component of this work was then to identify vulnerabilities, the nature of these vulnerabilities, and how current (and to a lesser extent future) notification technologies address these vulnerabilities. The project was conducted in two stages. In Stage I, sections of the population were examined to establish two criteria: whether they are particularly vulnerable to hazards associated with ; and whether these vulnerabilities could be reduced by particular notification systems. Existing notification technologies were examined to determine whether they were effective in alerting these groups of the existence of an incident and improving their chances of survival when responding to it. Once the vulnerabilities and technologies were identified, an overview of the associated empirical evidence was produced. This, along with guidance provided by the Technical Panel, established where gaps in our understanding existed. The results of Stage I identified key vulnerabilities that required further research. These key vulnerabilities were then examined in Stage II of the project. The findings were then drawn together, and recommendations made.

9

2 Approach Adopted to Identify Vulnerabilities The traditional method employed in order to understand vulnerabilities of a population to fire is to adopt a top-down approach. This involves the examination of the high-level (e.g. national or international) statistical trends in order to establish the most significant vulnerabilities [1-12]. Although this approach has a number of important strengths, it is not adopted here for several reasons.

It was established relatively early on in the work that vulnerabilities are not equally supported by the data available. It was clear that the groups identified as being vulnerable were not equally likely to be represented in the incident statistics collected; i.e. that some vulnerabilities were more likely to be recorded than others. This discrepancy may have been due to difficulties in identifying the presence of a condition (e.g. partial hearing loss in a fatality), recording practices (e.g. whether all the information was recorded at the scene) and also establishing whether a factor actually influenced the fatality (e.g. whether being asleep contributed to the fatality or whether the individual would have died anyway). Therefore, not only were there difficulties in extracting vulnerabilities during the fire investigation and through examining the reported material, but it was also difficult to assess the extent of the actual impact of reported vulnerabilities during an incident.

This became apparent from examining high-level statistics, the methods used to collect and store the supporting information and from a preliminary examination of human behavior in fire literature [13- 115]. These issues may have led to certain vulnerabilities being overlooked or under-reported; i.e. that the differences in the data on vulnerabilities was not just due to the extent of the vulnerabilities themselves, but was also due to confounding factors. It is felt that this issue may mask potentially important vulnerabilities that require further examination.

To avoid these potential discrepancies, a novel approach has been developed to more reliably assess the vulnerabilities present. A vulnerability model was developed that requires vulnerabilities to be derived from the bottom-up (i.e. by identifying vulnerabilities and establishing their fundamental components), rather than the traditional approach of deriving trends from high-level statistics. It is certainly not claimed that the traditional approach is not a useful and important tool; however, it was felt that a method that was more independent of the data collection process allowed for some of the obscured vulnerabilities to be identified. The two approaches are considered complementary.

The approach developed to identify and categorize vulnerabilities and notification technologies is shown in Figure 1. This approach involved a detailed analysis of the available literature, the identification of vulnerabilities and the categorization of these vulnerabilities according to their underlying nature. This allowed them to be prioritized according to whether they could be addressed through the use of a particular notification system.

10

Perform literature Review

Identify Vulnerable Groups

Categorize Notification Process

Categorize Notification Technologies

Establish Effectiveness of Notification Technology

Figure 1: The method used to develop the vulnerability model.

A review of general evacuation literature was performed [13-115]. This was essential as it enabled us to place the vulnerabilities (and the notification process) in context with the overall evacuation process. Once compiled, the notification process itself was broken down into its constituent parts: whether the message/signal is received; whether it is understood; whether the appropriate response is identified; and whether this response is conducted. This information was then re-examined in order to categorize the vulnerabilities according to the phase of the notification process to which it is vulnerable. A metric was produced to clarify this categorization process. This established the nature and extent of the vulnerability, and whether this vulnerability was due to the notification process or from some other concern (e.g. they had mobility issues and were unable to respond).

This iterative process also enabled the identification of vulnerabilities that arose from different sources; i.e. that could not simply be attributed to the individuals involved. This was important as the nature of the vulnerability influenced the likelihood of it being recorded. These sources included

Innate vulnerabilities – vulnerabilities that were an attribute of those involved (and are traditionally easier to identify and record); e.g. the individual was immobile, elderly, etc. Experiential vulnerabilities – vulnerabilities that related to the level of experience, knowledge levels, and procedural awareness; e.g. whether or not an individual is sufficiently trained. Situational vulnerabilities – vulnerabilities that related to the incident scenario; e.g. the individual was asleep or intoxicated, close to the fire, etc.

Vulnerabilities were defined in this manner to better understand how they may arise, persist, develop and propagate. For example, vulnerabilities need not be permanent; i.e., someone that may not otherwise be considered vulnerable may become vulnerable (e.g. intoxication, the presence of background noise). These situations are particularly susceptible to being overlooked in the reporting process and are also less likely to have been prepared for by those involved; e.g. someone with a permanent and serious

11

hearing impairment is more likely to have taken some measures to counteract this condition than someone who is occasionally intoxicated.

Status ? Intoxication ?

Smoking ? Outcome

Event Occu" Alann sounds Evac. Starts Status? Behavioral Response ? Impairment ?

Contributory Attempt to lntox.ication lmpaim1ent? Behavior? tacRle fire

(a) (b) Figure 2: Hypothetical incident with ‘contributing’ factors. [116] An example will help illustrate this problem. Assume that an incident has occurred where fatalities have been recorded. The report of this incident records that several events, factors and behaviors were present during the incident; e.g. the alarm sounded, people were asleep, people were intoxicated, etc. (see Figure 2(a)). However, some factors are missed in the reporting process, given that there was no indication at the scene and there was no previous record of this factor existing; for instance, a fatality had an unrecorded hearing impairment. Therefore, it is possible that an incomplete record exists of the factors that contributed to the number of fatalities. In addition, it is difficult to reliably assess whether the recorded factors actually contributed to the number of fatalities, whether they would have contributed but were dominated by the severity of the incident, or whether they were coincidental (see Figure 2(b)). This is primarily due to the absence of a detailed narrative account describing the incident. Therefore, the chain of events (e.g. causal factors, outcomes, etc.) is difficult to ascertain.

These issues can lead to certain vulnerabilities being misreported and misunderstood; it is possible that high-level statistical trends may occasionally misrepresent the importance of a particular factor, and may miss others entirely. To avoid these discrepancies, the vulnerability model highlighted was developed to complement the existing statistical approach. It is contended that by representing vulnerabilities according to their fundamental components that this model is able to identify factors that may fall through the statistical ‘net’ and may help explain why certain vulnerabilities exist in the first place. Particular care was taken during the literature review to examine work where the chain of events and the key influential factors were established (see Section 3).

A review of the notification technologies was then performed. These were examined in order to establish the capabilities of the current notification technologies and to identify their defining characteristics. The numerous methods available were grouped according to the notification approach adopted (e.g. visual, audible, etc.) and the scope of the technology’s impact. The notification technologies currently available were then re-examined to determine what benefit they might bring, and which aspects of the notification process they address. In doing so it was possible, in general terms, to establish which notification systems might benefit which of the vulnerabilities.

12

,-' ;••-·-·-·-·-·-·-·-·-·-·-· ..... \ ' ' '' Literature ' ' Review ' Evacuation Process : : Actual lncid;,;~i;- ' Pre-Evacuation Data ~ contrived Drills Notification/ Preparation '-· · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · Quantitative/Qualitative Analysis ' Identify Set of ' ' -...... -♦ ...... Vulnerabilities --

w Categorize (.!) Notification Process <( I- (/') t Identify and Examine Notification Tech.

Final Vul nerabi Ii ti es Map Existing and Notification Empirical Research Technologies to be examined t Recommendations -w from Researchers (.!) Performance of <( I- Research (/') Projects Expert Analysis from Technical Panel

Compile findings from Research Projects and from Analytical Process

Figure 3: Overall approach adopted during this project. The existing empirical data relating to the performance of notification technologies was examined to establish the findings, recommendations and omissions in our understanding. The Technical Panel was consulted throughout to establish their opinions on the vulnerabilities and technologies identified. The number of vulnerabilities considered for further research was then reduced, enabling three data collection activities to be outlined. In Stage II of the project, these activities were conducted, along with a risk assessment that examined the potential benefit of this work. The findings of these activities were reported and compiled, with general conclusions drawn and recommendations made. The overall process adopted during this project is outlined in Figure 3.

By adopting this bottom-up approach, vulnerabilities could be identified in a manner less dependent on the process used to record the data at the scene. This allowed vulnerabilities to be highlighted that might

13

otherwise be overlooked. It also allowed the nature of the vulnerabilities to be better understood and suggested a method of potentially addressing the vulnerability in question. This project was primarily conducted in order to direct research activities and possible recommendations; i.e. to establish where there are omissions in our understanding, improve our understanding and generate recommendations. In Stage I of the project, vulnerabilities were examined and three key vulnerabilities identified that required further data collection to support our understanding. In Stage II of the project, data was collected on these three vulnerabilities. Once complete, these findings were compiled and recommendations made.

14

3 Stage I: Literature Review A general literature review was performed in order to support and inform the analysis. It also provided some background information on the types of behaviors and factors that influence evacuee response. This review examines: • the evacuation process, • the notification process, • the pre-evacuation phase, and • data relating specifically to factors that can influence the response to notification systems.

The literature review involved a vast amount of material (see References). A detailed presentation of this material is beyond the scope of this report. Therefore, where required, examples of the research considered are briefly described. This will provide insight into important work but also give an indication of the type of material examined (see previous section). It was critical to this project that a broad review was performed. Given the vulnerability model adopted, a narrow focus may have excluded vulnerabilities from consideration early on in the process.

3.1 Evacuation Process

In the last few decades an increasing effort has been spent in investigating and understanding human behavior in response to fire. Prior to this time, human behavior was disregarded altogether, seen as unfathomable, or simplified according to a few basic assumptions: during a fire people’s behavior was likely to be panic-based; it was likely to be selfish and competitive; and it would involve immediate and direct movement once the incident was discovered. These assumptions had a direct impact on the manner in which emergencies were viewed and how emergency procedures were designed. Effectively, these assumptions pointed to the impossibility of predicting evacuee behavior; managing an evacuation; and providing detailed information to the evacuees for them to better appreciate the situation.

More recently, an improved understanding of human behavior in fire has been developed [97-98,103- 110]. This has been derived from the examination of actual incidents, the collection of empirical evidence and the development of related theories [13-115]. All of this has, to a large degree, refuted the assumptions that had previously dominated. This has allowed engineers, behavioral researchers, procedure designers and evacuation modelers to attempt to take human behavior into account when trying to understand the outcome of an incident.

It is now recognized that the evacuation process is not simply initiating the evacuation and then controlling the ensuing response; instead, it is viewed as a multi-faceted event where people respond in different ways according to the incident scenario and the information available. The problem of understanding human behavior in fire is not the relatively simple process previously assumed; instead, it relies upon a number of factors, which can interact and influence the outcome in different ways. Although the number and type of factors is extensive, some are worthy of further discussion.

An understanding of human performance is required for any performance-based analysis of an incident. In such an analysis, the development of the fire is compared directly against the performance of the population in order to assess whether there is sufficient time for the entire population to evacuate before

15

the conditions become untenable: the Required Safe Egress Time is less than the Available Safe Egress Time (see Figure 4). This type of schematic, which is widely used in current engineering analysis, acknowledges a fundamental finding from the past few decades: the time spent in responding to the event (i.e. the time between the notification of the incident and the population making a purposive movement towards a place of safety) can often be the largest component of the overall evacuation time. This will not always be the case; however, in the vast majority of cases it is critically important to understand the impact that the pre-evacuation phase will have upon the success of the evacuation process. Unfortunately, the effort spent in performance-based analyses is not necessarily spread evenly between the ASET calculations (related to the fire development) and the RSET calculations (related to egress calculations): there is often much more time spent on the fire calculation due both to the familiarity with these calculations in the field and a residual belief that the human variable has limited influence to the overall situation.

Available Safe Egress Time (ASET)­ Time available before untenable conditions are reached

Time to reach safety

Time to commence egress movement

Ti me for al arm to be raised

Ti me for fire to be detected

Required Safe Egress Time (ASET) - Overall time needed to reach safety

Figure 4: The ASET/RSET comparison, derived from Purser [94,96].

The schematic shown in Figure 4 simplifies the evacuation process in order to enable direct comparison to be made. In reality the two behavioral components identified (e.g. the time to commence egress movement and the time to reach safety) are themselves influenced by a large number factors. These have been the subject of a number of research activities over the past three decades [39,42,76-80,85- 89,93,99,112,117]. Figure 5 outlines some of the key influences upon the evacuation process. These are neither consistent, uniform nor static during an incident and can have an impact over all aspects of the evacuation process. These factors include the architecture of the structure; the organization(s) housed within the structure; the nature of the incident; the procedural/technological attempts to ameliorate such an incident; and finally the people that are present during the incident itself. To better understand these primary influences and the associated work performed regarding each of these areas, discussion of how they influence the pre-evacuation and evacuation phases is necessary, as is information to place the notification process in context.

16

AOl'Uil11ization Resident Popurauon Usage or structure Expected activities .. .• .. Individual Members or Resideon Po1mlation Scerml'io Al'chltecture Nature or incident Vlsual access Individual attributes/ill)ainnents Role/Relationship Environmental conditkms Exit capacity Condition of structure Egress routes Familiarity with the structure ation Loss of routes Terrain Access to Inform Experience Decision-Making Process t

Procedunl!! !response Notification Systems Slgnage Suppression Systems Comparrnentalizatlon Egress procedure Staff activity Figure 5: Sources of influence upon the evacuation process.

3.1.1 Architecture / Structure [25,28,35,39,66,105,106,110] The physical limitations imposed by the structure are a critical component of the evacuation process: it constrains the egress capacity of the structure and provides the arena for the evacuation to take place. For a significant period of time this was considered the dominant factor in the evacuation process, and in some situations it certainly is. Undoubtedly, the configuration of the structure has an important impact over the egress performance. This is embodied in the majority of prescriptive codes, which focus on the physical limitations imposed by the structure and its configuration [118]. However, the importance of this factor is highly dependent upon the nature of the scenario. For instance, in large structures with low density populations, the capacity of the egress routes may be less important than in relatively small spaces with high density populations.

Numerous researchers have examined the physical impact of the structure’s configuration upon the evacuation . This has been conducted at the level of the building and at the level of the component [35,67,119,120]. This data is usually presented in the form of flow rates, velocities and populations

17

densities [119].1 These are usually sensitive to a sub-set of the following variables: the nature of the component (e.g. stair, exit, corridor), the nature of the scenario (e.g. emergency or non-emergency), and the direction of use.

However, the configuration of the structure can have an impact on the decision-making process as well as on the physical performance of the evacuating population. The layout of the egress routes may be more or less susceptible to being blocked according to the nature of the scenario; e.g. by the spread of smoke. This will directly influence the routes available to the population during an evacuation.

The design of the structure may encourage the population to adopt specific routes during normal circulation; this may in turn influence the familiarity of the population with the routes available and affect their choice of egress route during an evacuation [28,39,106,110]. The design may be intuitively arranged or be complex, inhibiting wayfinding. The availability and visibility of egress components will also affect occupant choices. This may influence the ability of an unfamiliar population to navigate around the structure successfully, especially when under time pressure. The level of compartmentalization of the structure will influence the level of information to which the population has access. Highly compartmentalized spaces may prevent visual or aural cues reaching isolated populations [110]. Ironically, this compartmentalization may represent an attempt at ; reducing the spread of smoke and, hence, transmission of information about the fire situation.

The structure has a direct impact on the ability of the evacuees to move freely during the evacuation. However, it will also have an impact on the individual’s response to the incident, influencing their access to information (e.g. cues from the incident, notification messages, etc.), and then their affiliation with particular egress routes (see Table 1). Table 1: Architectural/Structural Factors the might influence performance. Factors that might influence egress performance Building Type and Use Physical Dimensions Geometry of enclosure Number and arrangement of egress routes Complexity of space Visual separation Lighting and (non-emergency) signage Extent of Passive Fire Protection

3.1.2 Scenario/Environmental Conditions [17,39,94,114,121-123] The nature of the scenario will influence the type of hazard presented to the population. It may inhibit the use of certain routes. In this respect the scenario can have a direct impact on the effective physical configuration of the structure; e.g. smoke blocking routes, damage to the structure, etc. It can also have a physical impact upon the evacuees. For instance, the evacuees may be physically impaired through exposure to smoke, elevated temperatures [121,123]; this may reduce their travel speeds, make them unconscious, or eventually lead to their death. The presence of smoke and irritant gases can also impair the evacuees’ ability to see, leading to difficulty in wayfinding or moving around the space [121].

1 Although great care is required regarding the units of measurement used.

18

The presence of fire effluent can also influence the decision-making process of the population. The exposure to smoke can act as cue to the existence of a fire; i.e. motivating individuals to respond. However, should the level of smoke (and other fire effluent) become too severe it may inhibit the movement of the evacuees around the structure, blocking off routes and forcing them to redirect and/or using routes that may have otherwise been unfamiliar [114,122]. Ironically the presence can reduce the time spent in the pre-evacuation phase by prompting the evacuees to respond, but prolong the movement phase by blocking of routes, reducing travel speeds and directly influencing the well-being of the evacuee (see Table 2). Table 2: Scenario/Environmental Factors the might influence performance. Factors that might influence egress performance Presence of Fire Effluent Background Pollution (Noise/Sound) Lighting levels Debris Presence of fatalities Structural damage Loss of routes

3.1.3 Organization [25,26,39,45] A more subtle influence exerted by the structure is the nature of the ‘organization’ that is housed within it; i.e. the use of the space. This use may range from a domestic residential settings, where the population is based around a single familial or social group, to a shopping mall, an office or a university, where looser, less familiar social groupings are present. The use of the space will influence the social structures present, the relationship between the population and the structure and the various procedural measures present. In small domestic residencies, the population will be familiar with the structure, the threats present and the people within it. This can cause them to be committed to the well-being of the occupants, the contents and the building itself, leading to the performance of specific behaviors (e.g. re- entry) [25-26]. In some spaces they may show commitment or engagement dependent upon the activities in which they are involved and the resources at stake in remaining in the space; e.g. they are attending an expensive concert or they have already paid for food at a restaurant. In such situations it may be difficult to attract the attention of the occupants and then motivate them to leave.

Where the population is transient and the space is complex (such as a shopping mall), the population’s familiarity with the space may vary; they may be in a variety of different social and familial groups and they may be less familiar with the cues indicating the existence of an incident. In spaces with a formal management system and sufficient resources, staff may be in place to manage the evacuation process. In such situations they may be accompanied by relatively advanced notification technologies. Table 3: Organizational Factors the might influence performance. Factors that might influence egress performance Safety Culture Normal use of structure Security procedures

19

Communication system Existence of social hierarchy

3.1.4 Procedural Response [13,16,34,39,49,50,54,58,122] According to the use of the space, an emergency procedure may be implemented in order to manage the evacuation process. This will be present in nearly all structures and will range from the use of a small tonal alarm and the development of an ad hoc family evacuation plan, to the development of an emergency procedure supported by staff and the implementation of advanced notification and suppression systems (see Table 4).

The procedure may involve a number of components. These can require significant preparation before the event (e.g. procedural design, training, drills, documentation, hiring practices, technical implementation, maintenance, etc.) and then actions in response to the event (e.g. activation of suppression systems, notification systems, staff implementing procedure, provision of information, etc.). The procedural response represents an attempt to extend the amount of time available to the evacuating population (by suppressing the fire and limiting its spread) and reduce the time required by them to reach safety (by proving information on the routes available, the existence and nature of incident, and ensuring the most efficient response).

The procedural response of the population is sensitive to the information provided to them and their interpretation of it. Variables that affect the response include the ability to receive information, the means by which the information is transmitted, the ability of the population to believe and understand the information provided, and the expectations for response.

Where large numbers of people (or vulnerable populations) are involved, the activities of the staff becomes critical. They are a means by which to provide current information to the populations, assuming that they are trained and trusted. The actions of the staff can and should be supported by other methods of providing information, including notification systems. Table 4: Procedural Factors the might influence performance. Factors that might influence egress performance Active Fire Protection Emergency Signage Notification System Emergency Training Emergency Literature Performance of fire drills False alarms Staff/fire wardens

3.1.5 Individual [15, 20,39,122,124] The individual evacuee brings an array of attributes to the event that will directly influence their ability to perceive the incident, determine a response and then enact that response. These attributes are physical (e.g., age, gender, physical fitness), psychological (e.g., cognitive abilities, motivation), sociological (e.g., relationship to those around them, role) and experiential (e.g., training, familiarity, information

20

levels). Some of these attributes can be influenced by the procedures employed within the structure; however, others cannot be changed and will need to be taken into account when determining a strategy (or technology) to address an incident. Some of the variation in the population can be predicted (and legislated for), such as the presence of persons with physical challenges; other aspects of it are less predictable, being transient, unaddressed and/or undeclared (e.g. intoxication, injuries due to the incident, partial hearing impairment, etc.).

The many attributes identified can influence the individual’s ability to respond (see Table 5). The attributes combine to form an idealized view of the components of the individual that are of interest to safety specialists and emergency planners. These attributes (along with the factors highlighted in the previous sections) will also influence the behavioral response of the individual (see Table 6).

Table 5: Individual attributes the might influence performance. Factors that might influence egress performance Factors that might influence egress performance Cognitive abilities Age Culture Gender Exposure to cues Activity Location Social affiliation Fatigue Engagement General health Commitment Sensory impairment Physical abilities Size Proximity to incident Experience Motivation Information levels Status Familiarity Role Responsibility Table 6: Potential actions that can be performed in response to an incident. Potential Actions Investigate / search Wayfind Evacuate ‘Panic’ Re-enter Remain / Delay Fight Fire Collect items Secure item Communicate Process information Provide assistance Non-evacuation behavior

21

3.2 Notification and Preparation in Non-Domestic Settings [39,64,65,122,124,125] The development of an emergency and the response to it can be categorized into three broad phases: Preparation, Notification / Response, and Procedural Implementation. The concept of the emergency procedure is based on the assumption that a population is receptive to information (i.e. that they will not inevitably panic) and that they can act on the information available. These phases pose different concerns and require different procedural, technological and structural actions in order to address them.

3.2.1 Phase I - Preparation: Implementing an Effective Design Extensive preparation is not always possible. This may be due to the resources available (e.g. if the occupancy is a single family dwelling) or the time available in order to prepare (e.g. transient populations will have limited time to prepare). Local preparations can be made; e.g. where a family develops a plan based on the resources and information available to them. However this will still involve them relying on their own actions (rather than the actions of staff) and possibly on a relatively simple ‘stand-alone’ notification systems.

Safety managers and designers employ several techniques to prepare a resident population for an emergency. These include emergency training, documentation, lectures and exercise drills. All of these occur prior to the event and will take place over a period of time. The preparation for the evacuation procedure is a process that requires assessment, design and reinforcement. This requires a sufficiently static population to allow for long-term preparation, significant resources, and a sufficiently formal environment allowing safety management to analyze and design an emergency procedure. In some occupancy types (e.g. residential) the preparation phase is limited and possibly non-existent. The population is then left to establish their own personalized response to an incident. This process may benefit from advice provided by experts, government sources, fire departments and neighbors. However, this is not guaranteed.

Given that there may be a complete absence of procedural preparation, a greater burden is placed upon the notification technology and its ability to quickly effect an evacuation. The initial shock of a serious and very real incident developing in someone's residence will not only require the occupants to be convinced, but to be convinced in sufficient time for them to make decisions, including making mistakes. In such a case valuable time may be wasted. The notification system then has to alert evacuees, and guide them where possible, in order to limit the time taken to respond and to instruct the nature of the response.

Where these procedures are in place (e.g. in high-rise office blocks) these preparatory measures can often be seen by the resident population as a burden. The training is reluctantly attended and the drills occasionally avoided; i.e. actions are taken just before the drill is performed in order to avoid it or minimize the disruption. Where drills are announced, residents often take action to reduce the inconvenience of the event; e.g. preparing for the drill prior to the announcement being made. When drills are unannounced, producing more reliable feedback on performance, there is usually an associated degree of resentment. Unfortunately preparation is not often given the time and attention required as a result.

To successfully prepare a population and for an emergency procedure to be effective, several key objectives need to be achieved:

22

the procedure needs to produce an efficient evacuation; the procedure is instilled into the population, such that they have sufficient knowledge and familiarity to follow it; the population (which may include occupants that are impaired, intoxicated, fatigued, etc.) is able to perform the actions required of them; the population’s familiarity with the procedure needs to be maintained; and the population needs to have sufficient confidence in the procedure in order to trust and enact it.

These objectives are inextricably linked. A well-designed, simple procedure that is sufficiently explained on a regular basis is more likely to be understood, and then followed. However, as already mentioned, there will be situations where the level of preparation is restricted due to the transient nature of the population and/or the resources available.

3.2.2 Phase II - Notification and Response: Making People Aware of the Incident Although the emergency procedure should be implemented once notification has taken place, there will inevitably be a delay in doing so - often a significant delay [32,34,76-81,99]. The delay may be due to a variety of factors and will be dependent of the incident scenario. For example, it may involve the population: Processing notification; e.g. differentiating the message from other emergency and non- emergency announcements. Confirming the nature of the event; e.g. seeking further information, confirming information with colleagues and safety personnel. Performing activities that are not part of the procedure; e.g. collecting bags, finishing meal, etc. Performing role-based activities as part of the procedures; e.g. shut-down activities. Confirming what is required of them. Preparing to perform the actions required of them; e.g. determining their first action.

Initially, notification requires that the population divert their attention away from the activities in which they are engaged. The success of this is initially associated with the clarity of the reception of the alarm signal/message. Members of the population who do not clearly receive it may misinterpret or ignore the signal/message entirely [39,115]. They are then required to comprehend the significance of the signal/message provided; i.e., that they receive it and then accept that it represents an actual event. To attract the attention of the population, the signal/message needs to clearly and believably denote the occurrence of an emergency incident. The population needs to be able to differentiate between the emergency information and other information from adjacent systems (on other floors, from other companies, non-emergency systems, security alarms, background pollution, etc.) [117]. Any difficulty in doing this might significantly hinder their response.

To comprehend the significance of the signal/message, the population must believe that it signifies a real and imminent threat. The accuracy of this perception is influenced by the frequency with which the system is tested, the frequency of malfunctions, and the frequency of false alarms [25,39]. If these events occur too frequently then they may detract from the manner in which occupants respond.

Therefore, notification systems need to be heard, recognized, and believed to the extent that an engaged group stops what they are doing to attend to the information provided to them and then respond to it. Ideally, they should also provide information on the nature of the incident and guidance on the required

23

response, in support of the training provided [64,65]. The provision of accurate and timely information to the population is more likely to convince them of an incident than a signal alone and may aid them during their response. A signal might alert them of an incident, but provide no further information.

It is now widely accepted that ineffective behavior is more likely when the provision of detailed information is delayed, ambiguous, or avoided altogether, causing a delayed response [64,65]. During such a delay, conditions might deteriorate further resulting in less time in which to make a decision and fewer alternatives from which to choose. In contrast, a more informed population is better able to assess the situation and respond accordingly. The same logic is then applied to the evacuee as to the safety manager: timely information helps appropriate actions.

The process of managing an evacuation is also aided by the evacuees knowing what they are doing and why. This response can be compared to attempts at crowd control where a population is coerced into acting in a specific manner without explanation [46]. Given that it is now considered desirable to inform the evacuees of the incident, the question then becomes how to do so most effectively.

The longer that the incident has to develop, the greater the risk posed to the resident population [94]. This may involve the loss of potential routes, the worsening of conditions, and the contraction of the procedural timeline. Therefore it is important for the entire emergency response that the population is notified of the incident as quickly as possible, that they believe this information and that sufficient information is provided to them to encourage an appropriate response (i.e. in support of the procedure). The procedure can then be implemented more quickly. Ironically, this quick response is still important even for those who are initially expected to remain behind. They may still be required to evacuate at some stage, and will then be able to do so earlier in the timeline of the fire, when the egress routes have cleared.

3.2.3 Phase III - Getting People to Follow Instructions Once the population is aware of the incident and have completed their preparatory actions, they are then ready to respond to the incident. Ideally this would involve them following behaviors allocated to them by the procedure (or small-scale plan). This might then involve one of the following: Moving towards a place of safety (outside or to a refuge). Remaining in place until further notice. Performing tasks or duties associated with control of the fire.

There are numerous problems that may arise during this phase of the emergency. It is the most complex and dynamic phase, given that it involves the movement of different sections of the population. If it progresses ineffectually, it is difficult to correct. The problems may be technological, procedural, structural and/or behavioral: Are the occupants physically able to follow the procedure? Are the occupants familiar with the procedure? Do the occupants follow the procedure? Is there sufficient capacity for the egress movement to take place? Are the occupants familiar with routes available? Is the capacity available exploited efficiently? Does the incident impinge upon this capacity (i.e. the egress routes available)?

24

GETTING PEOPLE TO UNDERSTAND THE PROCEDURE

GETTING PEOPLE NOTIFYING TO MOVE GETTING PEOPLE ENSURING THE GETTING PEOPLE PEOPLE OF THE TO USE THE ROUTES HAVE TO REMEMBER EVENT AND REQUIRED SUFFICIENT THE PROCEDURE THEIR EXPECTED GETTING PEOPLE ROUTES CAPACITY RESPONSE TO REMAIN

GETTING PEOPLE TO TRUST THE PROCEDURE

Figure 6: Issues with the notification process. [125]

The research literature indicates that the most effective means of informing an individual of an incident is through the presence of a well-informed, well-trained, assertive and respected member of staff instructing occupants of the incident [28,126-128]. If a member of staff is not present, then other means of influencing the evacuation need to be provided. In their absence it is critical that an information vacuum be avoided. The worst-case scenario would be that, in this information vacuum, the occupants respond after an extended period of time and then evacuate in a disorganized manner, adopting incorrect routes and overloading some egress routes whilst underutilizing others. In contrast, with sufficient information and management, the evacuation can be managed in order to reduce the likelihood of conditions deteriorating (see Figure 6).

It may then reasonably be assumed that the effectiveness of a member of staff provides an upper bound for any other technological solution; i.e. it is unlikely that a technological solution will surpass the effectiveness of a well-trained, well-informed human solution both in terms of initiating and responding to the incident (see Figure 7). The flexibility and credibility of committed staff can then act as a benchmark for the performance of notification technologies.

25

Best Case - Assertive, well­ trained, well-informed, committed members of staff are present to support and guide the evacuee. -~• ­ Daaprl Intermediate Case - Information provided by notification technology Worst Case - Evacuee is provided with no information, or inaccurate ill information

Figure 7: The continuum of effectiveness in notification. This ranges from an absence of information or the provision of inaccurate information to the presence of a well-trained and assertive member of staff. In between there are a range of different notification technologies available.[ 127]

However, there will be situations where staff is unavailable, or where they need support. In these situations, notification technology is a critical tool. Even though it may be less effective, it is still very effective. The evacuation process requires the occupant population to recognize that an incident is sufficiently hazardous to require a change in their behavior, such that they respond (see Figure 7). It is critical that accurate information is provided. The worst situation would be where there was no information available, or where inaccurate information was provided, especially where it came for a source perceived as credible (e.g. a member of staff).

In essence, the provision of information via notification systems compensates for deficiencies in the existing information levels in the occupant population. Visual, audible and tactile notification systems can be employed, as they increase the likelihood of reaching the occupant population given the different conditions that might arise and the abilities of the occupants themselves. The success of these approaches is dependent upon the resolution of a number of questions, prior to the occupant appropriately responding to the incident [64,65]: Is the information produced by the notification system received? Is the information perceived by the occupant as representing an emergency? Does the information lead the occupant to identify an appropriate response? Is the individual then able and willing to perform this response?

26

The information included in Table 7 indicates that the nature of the information provided by a system can vary significantly. The most basic information indicates the occurrence of an incident; e.g. through the sounding of a tone, flashing of a light, or vibrating of a buzzer. The proliferation of tones, alarms and security devices may reduce the association of a particular event with an emergency incident [39]. Particular signals have been developed (e.g. the temporal or T-3 signal) that are more readily associated with an emergency in an attempt to combat this problem [76,80,93,127, 129,130]. Table 7: Examples of Notification Systems. System Nature Information provided Siren, Horn, Bell Audible Indicates that an event has occurred T-3 Signal Audible Indicates that an emergency has occurred, assuming that T- 3 is suggestive of an emergency [129,130] Voice Recording Audible Indicates that an incident has occurred and may also provide response guidance. The nature of the incident and the response will need to fall within the library of stored messages. Directional Noise Audible Indicates that an incident has occurred and provides guidance on the location of exit locations [131] Live Announcement / Audible Indicates that an incident has occurred and may also Paging provide real-time response guidance. Assuming that a procedure exists to determine the response, the method is flexible enough to provide relevant information. Strobe Visual Indicates that an event has occurred LED Textual Signs Visual Indicates that an event has occurred and may allow limited information to be provided. Display Screens Visual Indicates that an event has occurred and may allow information to be provided on the required response. Vibrating Device Tactile Indicates event has occurred

As with audible systems, visual systems range in their ability to initiate the evacuation and then influence evacuee behavior. Strobes are often present in public spaces and, although intended to provide primary notification to those with a hearing impairment, have the ability to grab the attention of other occupants and inform them that an event has occurred. However, even if this information is accepted as indicating an incident, it provides no further information relating to the nature or seriousness of the event, nor information on the required response. Other visual devices can be employed to represent this type of information, including the use of screens, displays, electronic signage, or other such devices. In these cases, it is critical that they are placed in a position where they are visible such that the full message is available to as wide an audience as possible. The information provided by such devices will normally be pre-determined in nature given the difficulty of producing visual instructions in a short period of time. However, these instructions could be more detailed (e.g. including maps) if a screen was employed.

There are a number of technological alternatives available for notification systems (as shown in Table 7). These have different strengths and weaknesses that can be broadly categorized according to: Whether they provide information on the nature of the response or not? Whether this information is pre-determined or adaptive?

In addition, the two approaches are susceptible to a variety of factors that can influence their effectiveness. A selection of these can be seen in Table 8.

27

Table 8: Examples of factors that can interfere with the two notification technologies [127]. Audible Visual Tactile Physical Factors Is there sufficient coverage? Is the information blocked by physical Is the individual exposed to the Does the audible signal/message reach the obstacles; e.g. the design of the structure, ‘signal’; e.g. are they in the required population? Are the sound levels furniture, etc.? designated location, such as a appropriate for this to occur, whilst not bed? being prohibitively loud in certain areas? Environmental Factors Is there background noise inhibiting the Are there non-emergency lighting, screens, receipt of the sound? advertising hoardings, etc. that detract from the emergency information? Does the presence of smoke interfere with the information provided? Individual Factors Are there individuals that are hard of Are there individuals that are visually hearing? impaired? Are there individuals that are cognitively unable to evaluate the information provided?

Situational Factors Is the message perceived to represent a fire emergency? Is the population expecting a visual Is the population expected a message? tactile signal? Is the system susceptible to false alarms? Will this then influence the perceived veracity of the notification system? Do the occupants comprehend the message provided? Is the population focusing elsewhere (e.g. engaged in another activity such as using a PC, watching TV, etc.) and is therefore less likely to receive the information provided?

3.3 Key Notification Issues Given the previous discussion, there are some basic requirements regarding the notification technology irrespective of whether it is visual, audible, tactile or otherwise: 1. It must have the maximum coverage possible. Those that are able to receive the information should have adequate access to it. It should be recognized that when employing a system there will be a proportion of the population that will not be able to receive the information provided. Alternative measures will need to be taken for these individuals (e.g. the intervention of staff, vibrating devices, mobile devices, etc.). 2. The social context of the information should be understood and managed if possible; e.g. will people recognize the information as actually representing a real incident. Technical issues that lead to false alarms, or the misinterpretation of the message as non-emergency, should be addressed. 3. The information levels available (either through the preparation or the notification system) should indicate the need to respond and the required response. This may be targeted to certain populations according to location or proximity to the incident. 4. Irrespective of whether the required response is uniform or not, the information provided should be based on the most current data available and should reflect the emergency procedures in place.

Ideally, a notification system should be formed from technological and human solutions. Not only will these two sources of information act to reinforce the reality of the incident, their combination will increase coverage and introduce much needed redundancy into the system.

28

3.4 Pre-Evacuation Data In the following sections a number of example data-sets are presented. These are vital in understanding the notification process and the impact that it has on the population. These were derived from a number of areas: Actual incidents – especially those where an assessment of the pre-evacuation/notification phase was reported; Contrived studies – where drills or experiments have been conducted and results presented relating to the pre-evacuation/notification phase; Focused Quantitative Analysis – small-scale quantitative analyses that focused on a particular place or a particular aspect of the pre-evacuation/notification phase process; Qualitative Analysis – where an analysis of the pre-evacuation/notification phase has been conducted with access to narrative, anecdotal or detailed reports enabled some explanation as to why the results occurred.

High-level quantitative analysis (i.e. the use of national or general trends) was also examined as part of this work; however this has been reported in detail elsewhere [1-12] and is not repeated here. The findings were used to support the rest of the work conducted, rather than determining the key elements of the work. The reader is referred to a number of important documents presenting high-level analyses, both on the national and international scale [1-12].

The provision of a comprehensive digest of these cases is beyond this project. However, it is important that representative examples are provided to enable the reader to judge the basis for any conclusions made. In each of the different categories, a list of references is provided reflecting the other sources examined as part of this review.

3.4.1 Pre-Evacuation: Real Incidents The impact of notification and the pre-evacuation phase on the results of an evacuation in response to an actual incident can vary. The manner in which factors interact is sensitive to the scenario and formed from many of the factors identified in Section 3. To explore this further, several incidents are discussed in order to assess the various ways in which the pre-evacuation phase can influence the results produced.

The impact of pre-evacuation behavior upon the outcome of a fire is sensitive to the nature of the incident. In some instances, the population responds quickly but, given the nature of structure, the population density and the rapid development of the fire, the population is still affected by the deteriorating conditions [13,25,32,34,39,76-80,111,115, 122,124]. In other cases, the prolonged pre- evacuation time (through procedural, technological, or behavioral factors) is responsible for the problems that arise. It is instructive to examine real-life incidents to establish the factors that lead to these different situations and the impact that the notification technologies that were present had upon the results produced.

3.4.1.1 Macedonian Association, Gothenburg, Sweden , 1998 [132] – Issues of Detection On the evening of October 29 1998, a fire developed in the premises of the Macedonian Association in Gothenburg, Sweden. This was a relatively small, two story structure approved to hold 150 people. On

29

the evening of the fire, up to 400 people were in the structure [132]. Witnesses confirm that the dance hall was very crowded [132].

The fire started in a stairwell adjacent to the main hall. The disco was on the upper floor and was served by two stairwells at each end of the structure (see Figure 8). There were two internal exits, one located at each end of the hall, each equipped with a single door that swung outward (see Figure 8). The main stairway on the northwest end discharged directly to the exterior. The other stairway on the southeast end discharged into a corridor on the ground floor, which then led to the same external exit.

NORTH 35.4 m/116 ft \ 1: 32m/105 ft -i:- ~·~

22.6 m'74 ft

11.s~· m's ft Q.s m/311 J 2.6m'llt

1.5 m This area is a separate occupancy 1.Sm

I; m/10 ~ 1 ~ Figure 8: Floor plan of Gothenburg Disco building in which the incident took place. Redrawn from [132]. The middle landing of the stairwell located in the south-eastern end of the building was used to store chairs. The fire originated in this area among the chairs. This fire developed undetected. Shortly before midnight, some time after the fire started, the door leading to the southeast stairwell was opened, allowing smoke from the fire to enter the main area of the building. This stairway was impassable and therefore not available during the evacuation. The only viable means of egress from the disco was through the north-western exit. This was soon overloaded by the number of people simultaneously attempting to evacuate. Once the smoke and fire entered the disco, the population responded very quickly. In this instance the delay in detection directly influenced the outcome, rather than the delay in the response of the evacuees once they were aware of the incident. Given the limited means of exit, a few people chose to escape through the windows.

During this incident there were 63 fatalities, of which 43 were found ‘piled’ around the internal exit leading to the main stairwell (see Figure 8). In addition 180 people were injured. Fire fighters reported that their access to the hall was blocked by a wall of bodies inside the doorway that reached the top of the doorjamb [132]. Other victims were found in a side room, off of the main hall, where they had apparently sought refuge. The NFPA concluded that the level of overcrowding, the lack of a fire detection and alarm system and the ignition of combustible storage material in the stairwell contributed to the large loss of life in this incident [132]. In this case, the presence of a detection system that was able to notify the population was an issue, rather than them delaying their response.

3.4.1.2 Woolworth’s , Manchester, UK, 1979 [115] – Issues of Procedure, Misinterpretation and Commitment

30

The department store consisted of a ground and 5 upper floors. The fire occurred on the second floor, in a furniture storage area adjacent to a restaurant. Ten people were killed.

The fire started in foam-filled furniture that had been stored on the second floor; as a result the fire spread rapidly, trapping people on upper floors. Some people in the adjacent restaurant were overcome by smoke. Sime performed a statistical analysis of the pattern of escape behavior from this fire [115]. He used witness statements and records of the fire as a data source. The movement and actions of 63 members of the public and 69 staff were analyzed. He found that the staff and customers used different routes out of the structure according to their familiarity levels. Effectively, the staff and customers employed different emergency procedures. In addition, it was reported that those in the restaurant delayed their evacuation, due to their proximity to the kitchen and their commitment to their meal. This group initially only received environmental information directly from the kitchen; i.e. they smelled smoke. However, without the provision of information from the alarm system or from the staff, this was interpreted as being a 'normal' kitchen event and did not motivate them to respond immediately, especially as they wanted to continue with their meal. This group was in particular danger given their proximity to the fire. The population would have benefited from clear information relating to the event and intervention from the staff. This would have reduced their pre-evacuation times and motivated them to leave their meals and evacuate.

3.4.1.3 Beverly Hills Supper Club, Southgate, KY, USA, 1977 [13] – Issues of the Notification System and Ad Hoc Procedural Development

On the evening of May 28, 1977, an electrical fire started in a small, unoccupied enclosed space in the popular . This allowed the fire to develop for an extended period of time prior to its discovery [13]. The two-story club covered approximately 65,500 ft2 and was formed from numerous dining rooms, cabaret rooms, function rooms and lounges. Although it was designed to be occupied by approximately 1500 patrons (being classified as a place of assembly), at the time of the incident between 2400 and 2800 patrons were on the premises.

The absence of a notification system and the complexity of the enclosure constrained the communication process and contributed to the high death toll: 164 people. The dispersion of the population and the relative distance (and visual separation) of a large section of the population from the incident allowed the fire to develop to a far more advanced stage than might otherwise have been the case. Those people isolated from the incident had no source of information other than the staff and the fire itself.

The fire was discovered at approximately 8:45 p.m. by staff who attempted to fight the fire prior to informing the patrons. Some of the patrons who were situated near the fire also became aware at this stage. The majority of the patrons in the other main rooms were notified by employees at or before approximately 9:00 p.m., prior to serious environmental deterioration. Those in the main room, some distance from the fire (the Cabaret room), only became aware of the incident at approximately 9:06 p.m. The Cabaret Room was completely engulfed in smoke by 9:11 p.m., at which stage no tenable means of escape were available.

Of the 164 fatalities, 162 were situated in the Cabaret room, where it was estimated that between 1200 and 1300 patrons were originally located. Due to its relative distance and isolation, the patrons in the

31

Cabaret room were dependent upon the employees for information concerning the incident, as there was no notification system within the structure.

Some insight into the event was gained through the performance of a post-incident survey [13]. The patrons were initially notified by a member of staff: the busboy Walter Bailey. He climbed onto the stage in the centre of the Cabaret room and informed the crowd of the incident. This information was not immediately accepted as being pertinent by many of the patrons. Patrons either believed that Bailey’s actions were part of a drill, that the incident was not particularly serious, or (worst of all) that Walter Bailey was part of the comedy act that had previously been interrupted. One patron estimated that only 30 to 40% of the patrons perceived the information as being serious enough to evacuate immediately. However, after further prompting, the patrons eventually began to move off.

The majority of the patrons (80%) within the Cabaret room cited instructions by a staff member as their reason for leaving. The actions of the busboy enabled many of the patrons to evacuate prior to the degradation of the environmental conditions. He also informed patrons of previously unfamiliar exits that by this stage were the only viable routes of exit. Not only did Walter Bailey point out the existence of the fire exits, but gave instruction as to which sections of the population should use which exit. Even with the early warning provided by the member of staff, the large evacuee load led to sever congestion, causing significant delays to the evacuation of the patrons located in the Cabaret room.

Fatalities were eventually found around the two exits previously unfamiliar to the evacuees. Due to the time constraints, these two fire exits were overloaded. Extensive congestion arose due to the difficulties presented by the structure’s geometry and the decreasing tenability of the environment.

The lack of early warning and sufficient information was a factor here. However, it is also indicative of the value of accurate information on the required response. Even under time pressure, the population was able to receive, process and follow information provided to them.

3.4.1.4 Düsseldorf Airport, Düsseldorf , Germany, 1996 – Issues of Information Provision and Procedural Difficulties [133]

On Thursday, April 11, 1996, a fire developed in a passenger terminal. This killed 17 people and injured 62. Authorities determined that the fire began when a worker ignited insulation used in the void above the ceiling on the first level. The fire began in the vicinity of a flower shop on the first floor. Electrical faults were reported, smoke was seen coming from the vents in the flower shop, and the ceiling began to glow and drop burning members. Personnel were on the scene soon after.

The fire developed rapidly and effluent spread throughout a large area of the first level of the terminal. Seven of the victims died in two elevators. Several people were on the roof observing aircraft and descended when smoke was perceived; the elevators opened into the fire area on the first level. Eight more victims died in a lounge on the third level; this was a mezzanine overlooking the departure level of the building. The location of two victims was not established.

The smoke and flames spread throughout the first level then extended to the second level through unprotected open stairwells and escalator openings. The area where the fire occurred was not equipped

32

with automatic sprinkler systems. The building was equipped with an alarm system that used voice annunciation in German, French, and English. NFPA identified several factors that were deemed to have contributed significantly to the loss of life. Only those that are relevant to our current analysis are described here: The transmission of erroneous information over the voice annunciation system during the first 10 minutes of alarm activation. The ability to shut down the public address system in the lounges. (This system was also used to transmit the emergency voice announcements.) Inadequate means of egress capabilities from the VIP lounge on the mezzanine level Two occupied elevators that opened directly into the fire area Lack of adequate communications capabilities between the command staff and fire fighting units Lack of awareness of the building layout

In this incident, a relatively sophisticated notification system was in place, however, the use of general guidance rather than information tailored to the evolving incident proved detrimental to the evacuation process.

3.4.2 Pre-Evacuation: Contrived Studies In addition to the data collected from real incidents, a number of trials have been conducted to collect pre-evacuation data [32,42,76-81,93,94,99,112,117]. The data is collected for many reasons (e.g. theory development, engineering calculations, procedural testing, specifically for data collection, etc.). It is also employed in a variety of different methods and provides the data to different degrees of detail. Given this, the data available in the literature should be (and was) treated with care. Irrespective of this, the existing data was useful regarding: the organization of trials; the variation in the results produced; the data collection methods employed; the data collected; and the format in which the data should be presented.

33

0 0 00 0 -J tv 0 ... N ~ N ... (I) ,I> u, ,I> . ~ (J ~ u, • :...... ; . • . • . • • • • ► ► ► ' ' 0 ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) l y e e 2) 3) 1 s se s (7) ( (4 ) ( (2 (2 (6 (5 e Ri Rise e e Hote ise ise s s R Rise(2) Rise(3 Ri Ri Rise Rise Rise Store( Store(4 Stor Store(3 Mid Hlgt'i

Shields Shield Shield Shields Figure 9: Results compiled by Fahy and Proulx [32].

Given the variation in background conditions and collection methods employed, it is unsurprising that the results produced can vary so greatly. Fahy and Proulx compiled a database of a large set of evacuations, real and contrived (see Figure 9). The range of results produced is evident.

3.4.2.1 Shields and Boyce - A Study of Evacuation from Large Retail Stores [99] Shields and Boyce conducted four unannounced evacuation from retail stores in the United Kingdom. Three of these stores were three-story structures, while the fourth structure had a single story (see Table 9). The stores were of different sizes and different levels of complexity. The evacuation was recorded using existing CCTV cameras and additional video cameras. A post-incident survey was also conducted to determine the experiences of the evacuating customers. They were able to collect information on the pre-evacuation times and evacuation times generated. All of the trials were conducted with the participation of a single company.

Customers were asked how frequently they visited the store. This could then be used to indicate the level of familiarity. Throughout the stores, the majority of customers were weekly or monthly visitors. They also examined what the customers were doing at the time of the evacuation, their level of commitment to this activity, the egress route adopted and how they were notified; i.e. the researchers were therefore interested in establishing what happened and why.

34

Table 9: Results produced by Shields and Boyce [99]. Building Pre-Evacuation time (sec) Evacuation time (sec) Number of data points Number of data points 1 37 [3-95] 240 122 571 2 31 [4-100] 210 122 616 3 25 [1-55] 165 95 477 4 25 [2-60] 131 71 409

3.4.2.2 Proulx – Evacuation Time and Movement in Apartment Buildings [79] Proulx conducted four evacuation trials involving different mixed occupancy buildings. Each was 6-6 storeys high and contained 80-130 apartments. The trials were recorded on video cameras that were distributed throughout the structures. The study examined the pre-evacuation time, the evacuation time, the movement time and the behaviors of the evacuees; particular attention was paid to the performance of the elderly and the disabled.

In Table 10 a selection of the results collected by Proulx is provided. Interestingly she was able to establish the average time spent moving (i.e. the time between initially responding to the point of arrival). She found that people spent between 11% and 35% of their time moving, while spending between 67% and 88% of their time responding; i.e. in the pre-evacuation phase. Table 10: Results produced by Proulx [79]. Building Average Pre-Evacuation Average Evacuation Average Movement time (sec) time (sec) time (sec) 1 150 185 65 2 502 576 77 3 582 657 75 4 188 278 67

3.4.2.3 Purser – Quantification of Behavior for Engineering Design Standards and Escape Time Calculations [96]

Purser conducted a series of evacuation studies and fire investigations, involving several building types in order to contribute to design standards and egress calculations. He achieved this by monitoring evacuation trials and then formatting the data to suit the twin objectives.

The trials ranged in size and complexity. The simple, small-scale trials enabled his team to focus on the key behavioral components and parse the data collected accordingly. He then applied this method to the other trials examined. Purser categorized the evacuation process into: recognition time (time from the alarm sounding to the first response); response time (time from the first response to purposive evacuation movement);

35

and travel time (time to reach safety). Using these categories in one small-scale trial, he monitored the evacuation of a single room using a hidden camera. The people in the room were unfamiliar with the building and were notified using a voice alarm system. This consisted of an initial sounder and then a message. The evacuees responded quickly, taking between 15 and 20 seconds to perform their first act, but then spent additional time performing other preparatory actions (between 17 and 39 seconds). He was able to provide some indicative values for the behavioral categories identified (see Table 11). Table 11: Results produced by Purser [96]. Person Recognition Response Travel Total Time (sec) Time (sec) Time (sec) time (sec) 1 16 40 5 61 2 15 17 5 37 3 17 20 2 39 4 20 30 3 53 5 16 30 6 52 6 18 39 5 62 7 ? ? 3 67 8 16 30 13 59 9 ? ? ? 55 10 ? ? ? 65 11 ? ? 2 71 12 ? ? 4 51 Average 17 [15-20] 29 [17-39] 5 [2-13] 56 [51-71]

Purser then examined a series of other trials in order to populate the categories defined as contributing to the overall evacuation time. Table 12: Results produced by Purser [96]. Categorization modified from original text. Recognition Response Total Overall Evacuation Time (sec) Time (sec) Pre-Evacuation (sec) Time (sec) Sounder 63 17-94 70-365 Voice 0-69 3-40 10-26 20-538 Bell 540 900

A relatively large amount of research exists examining pre-evacuation behavior through contrived (i.e. pre-planned) trials [96]. The value of this work is influenced by several key factors: its original purpose; the methods used to collect the data; the extent and format of the data presented; and the conditions present during the data collection event. The examination of this material was useful in that it informed our understanding of the pre-evacuation process, the data collection process and the development of the vulnerability model.

3.4.3 Pre-Evacuation: Focused/Quantitative Analysis In this section several small-scale analyses are described. These are a representative sub-set of the work examined [3,11,133,134,136-170]. These tend to be detailed and/or focused on one location, a limited number of vulnerabilities or limited aspects of the notification process. Although the analysis is

36

quantitative, the focused approach provides valuable insight and reduces the ‘averaging’ effect that can occur in broader analyses. There is certainly some overlap between the results in the preceding and proceeding sections; however, it is felt that the data-sets now presented provided greater access to the behavioral issues of interest.

Although quantitative in nature, the fact that these examinations were on a relatively small-scale enabled them to focus in on a particular aspect (or vulnerability) and/or on a particular location. This enabled the researchers to endeavor toward a more detailed quantitative understanding than might otherwise have been the case; it also meant that they combined fewer data-sets when presenting the material and therefore their findings were closer to the original data than might otherwise have been the case.

3.4.3.1 Maryland (1972-1977) and Minnesota (1996-2001) [3,134,135] The impact of alcohol intoxication was examined in two separate studies of fire incidents in Maryland and Minnesota, which can be paired due to their similarities. Blood alcohol levels were established by the coroners' offices and reported. An examination was made of the intoxication levels of fire fatalities. Although it could not be definitively stated that the alcohol levels caused the fatalities, a correlation was established. This work focused on a particular vulnerability evident in fires from a single location.

The adult fatal fire victims were examined. It was found that 46 to 51% of these fatalities were alcohol impaired, based on a 0.1% threshold. This varied according to age (see Figure 10). It is apparent that the largest proportion of fatalities due to alcohol were aged between 30 and 59 years of age. This will certainly have been influenced by drinking patterns and other scenario-specific factors (e.g. smoking habits, location, whether they were alone, etc.), but may also indicate that for these traditionally less vulnerable age groups are made vulnerable through intoxication. Other age groups may not have the same drinking patterns, or may have other existing vulnerabilities. A similar study into the fire fatalities recorded between 1992-1997 in Alabama found 45% of victims aged over 18 had 0.10 or more grams per deciliter of blood .

80

70

60

50

Maryland 40 Minnesota

30

% Deaths Due to Alcohol to Due % Deaths 20

10

0 (-9) 9-19) (-29) (-39) (-49) (-59) (60+) Age Groups

Figure 10: Intoxication levels produced in the Minnesota and Maryland studies according to age. [3]

37

3.4.3.2 Fire Fatalities in Minnesota 1991 [136] In an earlier analysis of the fire fatalities in Minnesota, the unintentional fatalities accounted for 31% of all alcohol-related deaths in the state. Of these 6% stemmed from fires. Of the 255 fire fatalities in Minnesota from 1993-1996, 74 were found to have positive blood-alcohol concentrations. This represented 30% of the total fatalities and 40% of the fatalities over 15 years of age. It was found that 90% of alcohol impaired fatalities had a blood alcohol level of 0.1 or greater (see Figure 11).

30% 27%

25% 23% 21% 20%

15%

10%

501o

0% 0-14 15-24 25-34 35-44 45-54 55-64 65-74 75-84 85+ Age Category

□ BAL Fatalities ■ No BAL Reported

Figure 11: Fatalities according to age and the presence of alcohol. [136] As was evident previously, the age groups with the highest recorded proportion of fatalities with blood alcohol levels, were those traditionally considered to be less vulnerable; e.g. 15-54 years old.

50% -,------,

45%

40%

35%

30%

25%

20%

15%

10%

5%

0-9 10-14 15-24 25-34 35-44 45-54 55-64 65-74 75-84 85+ Age Range

Positive BAL ■ No BAL Reported I

Figure 12: Fatalities according to age and the proportion of fatalities due with recorded BAL.[ 136]

38

3.4.3.3 Fatal Residential fire accidents in Municipality of Copenhagen, 1991-1996 [137] Leth et al examined the fire fatalities in Copenhagen (Denmark). They focused on a single location, but examined a number of different vulnerabilities. They performed a case control study that involved examining 65 fatal house fires and identified a number of variables that they felt influenced the likelihood of survival: location at time of ignition; physical impairment; alcoholism; involvement in a clothing fire; alarm being given by person not at the scene of the fire. They claimed that,

' [The] elderly people may run an increased risk of death because of physical impairment or handicap. Furthermore, many of the elderly people and chronic alcoholics die in fires associated with cigarette smoking.' [137]

They produced a model to relate risk indicators and risk factors to the likelihood of death (see Figure 13).

[ HISK IN DICATO~S ) ( RISK Ft,CTORS )

DEATH 0Y In

P"ychiatri c I

Sµr~ ud of tire Figure 13:Risk indicators and risk factors identified by Leth.[ 137] It is apparent that the outcome of the incident was sensitive to a large number of interrelated factors with proximity and intoxication being important contributing factors.

3.4.3.4 Risk factors for fatal residential fires [138] Using a case-control design, Runyan et al compared 151 fatal fires in single-family dwellings in North Carolina with a sample of nonfatal residential fires over a 13-month period. Runyan focused on a single area and occupancy type. Case fires were identified through the medical-examiner system, and control fires that occurred within a few weeks of the case fires were randomly selected. For each fire, fire officials were interviewed about the dwelling, the fire, the people involved, and the fire-response system. From their findings fatal fires were more likely to have been caused by smoking (31 % of fatal fires against 6 % of nonfatal fires). Mobile homes posed a higher risk of death if a fire occurred, as did the absence of a smoke detector. Smoke detectors were more protective against death in fires involving young children and when no one present was impaired by alcohol or drugs or had a physical or mental

39

disability. The presence of an alcohol-impaired person was the strongest independent risk factor for death in the case of a fire. The risk of death was therefore greatest in fires in mobile homes; in those involving alcohol-impaired persons; and in those in houses without smoke detectors.

3.4.4 Pre-Evacuation: Focused/Qualitative Analysis The research that focused on (or made reference to) individual reports of an incident were termed qualitative analyses [3,,4,13,15,17,20,31,33,45,46,48,49,58,77,90,94,111,114,115,71-174]. These examined the accounts provided by survivors that allowed an understanding of the experience from the perspective of an individual. From this body of material, the researchers usually then performed an additional quantitative analysis, producing numerical statistics in support of their qualitative findings. Importantly, it also enabled a better understanding of the chain of events within the incidents examined. This provided an understanding of the vulnerabilities in the populations examined and how these influenced the outcome.

The qualitative work provides an insight into the underlying vulnerabilities; this would not be available from high-level statistical analyses. This is due to the detail of the reports provided and the ability to establish the link between initial conditions and eventual outcome. Even here this is not always clear cut or unambiguous; however, it at least established a more detailed picture of the chain of events than might otherwise have been the case.

3.4.4.1 Fahy and Molis[31] Fahy and Molis examined the cause of fatalities in home fires where the alarm operated. By doing so they were targeting the very population in which we are interested: those that died when the alarm was in operation. Using the National Fire Incident Reporting System (NFIRS) database,, they estimate that annually there are 770 US fatalities where the smoke alarm was in operation. Using data collected by the NFPA, they examined 218 fires, from the years 1997 and 1998, where there were 277 civilian casualties [REF]. Although this only represents a small proportion of the overall number of fatalities, it is a particularly important piece of work, given the detail in the causes of the fatalities examined and the material employed in their analysis (e.g. fire department reports).

Detailed analysis of these incidents is provided in their research, with the cause of death and an outline of the actions performed prior to death presented for several of the cases. This was established by examining the narrative accounts of the incidents themselves.

Fahy and Molis made several observations regarding vulnerable groups. Children were vulnerable due to both difficulty in waking them up and an inability to act. They stated that, ‘…questions have been raised as to whether this inability to waken has resulted in children in this age group accounting for a disproportionate share of the victims of home fires with operating smoke alarms. An analysis of NFIRS data from 1994 to 1998 did not indicate that this is the case.’ [31]

Those intimate with the fire would not be saved by notification. Their proximity meant that the issue of notification was rarely the primary factor in their death.

40

Age, the presence of a hearing impairments and intoxication hindered waking effectiveness. These factors therefore contributed to the fatalities.

The key data is shown presented in their work is shown in Table 13. Several points are apparent from this table: 17% of victims were intoxicated (see Figure 14). This may be an underestimate due to under- reporting. The adult population (e.g. 21 – 60 years of age) appeared to be the most likely to be intoxicated. 34% of the fatalities were alone. This was apparent in most age groups, with persons over the age of 60 also being susceptible to this factor. 18% of the fatalities were disabled (see Figure 15). Persons over the age of 60 were susceptible to this factor.

Table 13: Risk Factors Reported by Fahy and Molis, categorized according to Age Group [31]. Age Unattended Groups Deaths Intoxicated Alone Children Disabled Under 6 44 0 9 0 (6-10) 18 0 6 0 (11-15) 7 0 0 1 (16-20) 6 1 2 2 (21-30) 20 5 4 1 (21-40) 26 9 11 3 (41-50) 31 14 11 3 (51-60) 25 8 13 4 (61-70) 26 5 15 9 (71-80 37 3 17 15 (81-90) 27 1 14 10 (91-97) 8 0 4 2

From Table 13 it is apparent that a lower percentage of fire victims over the age of 60 years (9 %) were intoxicated compared to the population of adults between 20 and 60 years of age (35 %). It is also suspected that the number of intoxicated victims may be under-reported. Intoxication does not appear to be as significant for older adults, as some of the other risk factors examined. Other factors may be more influential for the elderly.

Overall, 34 % of victims age 16 and older were home alone at the time of the fire. Of those victims younger than 16, 24% were home alone. For adults older than 60, over half (51 %) of the victims examined were alone at the time of the fire. Given the number of elderly living alone, this may be a contributing risk factor, particularly for those who have disabilities or difficulty hearing the alarm.

41

50

45

40

35

30 Total Deaths 25 Intoxicated 20

Number of Deaths 15

10 5 t1-1- 0 (6-10) (11-15) (16-20) (21-30) (31-40) (41-50) (51-60) (61-70) (71-80) (81-90) (91-97) Under 6 Under Age Groups

Figure 14: Overall deaths and intoxication categorized by age group [31] Those susceptible to being intoxicated included those that may have had additional responsibilities. This may then have led to a secondary impact, where those in people’s care (e.g. children) may have been placed in danger through the diminished ability of the care-giver to respond. Elderly suffered from a higher frequency of disabilities and being alone.

50

45

40

35

30 Total Deaths 25 Disabled 20

Number of Deaths 15

10 5 -1-~- -1- 0 (6-10) (11-15) (16-20) (21-30) (31-40) (41-50) (51-60) (61-70) (71-80) (81-90) (91-97) Under 6 Age Groups

Figure 15: Overall deaths and disabilities categorized by age group. [31]

42

In addition to examining factors influencing the effectiveness of response, they also examined the behavioral choices made that increased the likelihood of a fire initially occurring. There were also choices made in response to notification that reduced the chance of survival. Therefore, even when people were suitably notified they were observed to have selected an inappropriate response that then led to their demise.

They pointed to the importance of establishing and practicing an emergency plans in conjunction with the use of notification systems. They stated that,

‘Several of these fatal incidents provided clear evidence of the need for people to establish and practice escape planes.’[31]

3.4.4.2 Purser and Kuipers [95] Purser and Kuipers collected detailed incident reports that included fire service documents, interviews (fire fighters, investigators, those involved in the fire), site visits, and media reports. This enabled them to produce a database enabling detailed analysis. They examined 98 incidents that occurred in the UK between 1994-1997 involving the following occupancies: domestic incidents (77), shop facilities(2), bars(2), hotel(4), residential homes(4), sheltered housing(4), hospitals(2), industrial premises(2) , school(1). Of these incidents 91% involved a residential component. They examined 210 interviews, highlighting a possible limitation with the normal high-level statistics used to represent fire fatalities:

‘[Collected National Statistics] are usually based upon a standard form or questionnaire... They can be very useful for obtaining basic information on fire incidents and for establishing trends of various kinds, but the data collected are limited and seldom record aspects of occupant experiences.’[95]

They found that once informed of a fire, evacuees engaged in a variety of different activities. Most engaged in 4-5 activities before reaching safety; i.e. that they did not immediately move to a place of safety. It was therefore

‘Not appropriate to assume that people simply leave the building immediately on becoming aware of the fire.’ [95]

From Table 14 it is apparent that the occupants were alerted to the incident by a number of different sources. Of those in the room of origin, only 8% (2/26) were alerted by an alarm system; this was approximately the same percentage as those not originally in the room of origin (7/93). Unsurprisingly, for those originally in the room of origin, the most common cue was actually seeing the fire itself (19%, 5/23); for those not in the room of origin the most common cue was being told be others (35%, 33/93). It is apparent that irrespective of the initial location of the respondents that the manner in which they were informed of the incident was complex, deriving from a number of sources, and not necessarily relying on the alarm system in place.

43

Table 14: How respondent was alerted [95] Occupant in fire room Frequency Occupant not in fire room Frequency Saw flame 5 Told by others 33 Heard noise 4 Heard noise 11 Told by others 2 Saw smoke 10 Smelt something 2 Smelt smoke 8 Saw smoke 2 Smelt burning 8 Saw flame/smoke 2 Heard alarm 7 Heard alarm 2 Saw flames 4 Woke up coughing 1 Saw flames/ smoke 2 Smelt smoke 1 Saw sparks 2 Smelt burning 1 Woke up coughing 1 Saw sparks 1 Smelt something 1 Not known 1 Saw fire brigade 1 Heard screaming 1 Rem.chip pan 1 Felt heat 1 TV went off 1 Heard fire brigade 1 Saw flash 1 Total 26 93

From their analysis they found that smoke detectors were present in 62% (61/98) of the incidents examined. Where they were present, alarm systems were functional in 80% (49/61) of the cases. Where present, they were activated in 51% (25/49) of the cases. Most importantly, where they were activated, they were effective at notifying a person 40% (10/25) of the time. Again this supports the previous assertion that, in the cases examined, the notification system was not necessarily responsible for initiating evacuation movement.

Table 15: Presence, activity and effectiveness of smoke detector. [95] Efficacy of smoke detectors in dwelling fires Frequency Detector Functional Activated Effective 37 12 24 15 10 Total=98

Given the data collected and following their analysis they concluded that,

44

' Occupants engage in a range of behaviour sequences during fire incidents, so that design needs to take account of the time required for these behaviours to be carried out.'[95]

and finally that

‘Domestic smoke detector/alarms were remarkably ineffective in providing occupants with warning of fires, so that measures need to be taken to improve design and use of smoke detectors.’ [95]

3.4.5 Summary The previous sections represent a brief summary of the literature review conducted. A secondary review has also been conducted regarding particular vulnerabilities and notification technologies. The findings are discussed later in this report. The nature and subject areas of this secondary review were, to a large extent, determined by the findings of the work in the previous sections.

Obviously, much of the emphasis of the review performed in the previous sections was on the pre- evacuation phase and the factors that might influence it. However, it was also important to place this into context with the other phases of the evacuation. This enabled the research team to establish where factors and vulnerabilities could propagate throughout the evacuation and therefore extend their influence.

The factors highlighted in the previous sections can be combined to produce a simplified model reflecting the factors that can influence the outcome of an evacuation (see Figure 16). Although this is a simplification, and is undoubtedly incomplete, it demonstrates the complexity of such an event and indicates some of the issues that will need to be addressed in understanding and improving the effectiveness of notification systems.

, , , , , , P1-ocedural En\lll-onmental/Scenario lndiridual Active Fire P1-otection Presence ofFire Eflluent Cognitive abilities Emergency S~nage Bacl

45

From the existing data-sets and from the examination of the general evacuation material, a simple model can also be developed regarding the information available to an occupant, what this might contain and the factors that might influence its receipt (see Figure 17). Obviously, this is a simplification of the real situation; however, for this project, it was critical to get a basic understanding of the context in which information arrives and the factors that might contribute to its impact...... ,,,. - - ,; , ------I I I Subiect area ' 1 Source ofinfo ' I Existence of incident 1 I Presence of visual / audible/ olfactory cues Proximity of incident 1 Nature of incident Physical interaction with incident Egress routes I ~ Actions of other evacuees available ,...... , Information provided by Route that should be 1 staff adopted Information provided by Time available notification system Training I Familiarity ,; ' .... ______,;

✓ ... I Factors that influence response to information \ I Whether itis received I Whether it is recognized Whether it represents a clear threat Whether it is supported by other cues/info Whether it is deemed relevant Whether the source is trustworthy Whether is indicates the response required , ______Whether it can be acted upon _ , ' Figure 17: Simple model of the source, content and influence of surrounding information.

The literature review conducted enabled an identification of key factors in the evacuation process and particularly in the notification process (see Figure 16 and Figure 17); conversely, it also enabled an identification of weak points in the process (i.e. vulnerabilities). This analysis, along with a compilation of those vulnerabilities highlighted in the reviewed material, allowed the development of a comprehensive list of vulnerabilities and how they may manifest themselves during the evacuation.

46

4 Stage I: Identification of Vulnerabilities

Vulnerabilities were derived through examining the research literature and from the analysis conducted and described in the previous sections. A variety of different groups were identified including, but not limited to, those shown below. Those not described were excluded at an early stage in the research project development. The groups included were either explicitly identified in the reviewed literature or were derived. One of the key objectives of the literature review was to identify the vulnerabilities present. This was important not only to provide a set of vulnerabilities from which research objectives could be derived, but also to provide a broader understanding of how, when and where vulnerabilities might arise.

For each of the vulnerabilities identified, a brief comment is provided regarding the nature of the vulnerability (along with some example references. This is further addressed in subsequent sections, especially those vulnerabilities identified as being of particular interest.

Individual Differences/Attributes [15,20,31,39,122,124,140,176,232] • There is variation in the physical and psychological capabilities of any population. This may relate directly to the individual’s ability to perceive information, process it, and eventually respond to it. This response may be cognitive (e.g. make a decision) or physical (e.g. commence movement). The differences may fall within the range considered to enable unimpaired activities, although still having significant differences between them. They may also fall outside of this range but be unrecognized, undiagnosed, untreated and/or not addressed. Finally, the differences may fall outside of the range and be officially recognized: labeled as an impairment or a disability. Children Younger than 5 Years Old [151,152,195,205, 232, 261,262,269-271,279] • This population may have physical and psychological limitations that prevent a reliable response to the notification system in place. The population may receive the signal, but not be able to understand it or respond accordingly. Given this, the population will likely have adults caring for them. This may make demands of these care-givers during an incident; i.e. that they need to search, communicate and/or rescue those in their care. This may place the care-givers in a higher level of danger than might have been the case if they had moved directly to a position of safety. These limitations may reduce the benefit of the notification systems, given that the population may not have the capacity to comprehend the situation or physically respond to it. As well, it has the secondary affect of modifying the response characteristics of the care-givers. Children Older than 5 Years Old [31, 140, 151,152,176,187, 189,191,192,194,195,205,232, 261,262, 268-271,279] • As with younger children, this group may have physical and psychological limitations that reduce the benefit of the notification systems. Again this population will likely have adults caring for them. This may make demands of these care-givers during an incident; i.e. that they need to search, communicate and/or rescue those in their care.

47

This may place the care-givers in a higher level of danger than might have been the case if they had moved directly to a position of safety. Those who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing (identified /unidentified) [174,175,178,203,204,206,209,210, 221,230,232,243,245,246,248,260,265-267,277,278, 280, 281,283, 285-287,289] • These sensory impairments may prevent an audible notification signal/message being received. This may affect their awareness of an event and also their understanding of the required response. However, given that the individual is aware of the incident and the required response, the hearing impairment itself should not directly inhibit their ability to respond. Depending on the severity of the impairment, it may not be corrected, recognized or even understood. If it is uncorrected or undeclared then the surrounding population may not take it into account when evacuating or when an evacuation procedure is put into effect. This is particularly an issue where the deaf or the hard of hearing are in an unfamiliar environment, where other cues denoting an incident may go unrecognized and unheeded. Visual impairment (identified / unidentified) [63, 66,198,209,220, 238] • This impairment may directly influence their ability to perceive the existence of an event and also establish their response. This would be particularly problematic in unfamiliar surroundings. Depending on the severity of the impairment, it may not be corrected or recognized. Even if detailed instructions are provided as to the response required of them, they still may require considerable assistance in navigating around an unfamiliar space. Unless specifically informed, the visually disabled would have ambiguous information relating to the nature, location and extent of an incident. Untrained individuals [144,169,207,208,211,214,217,223,240,241,249,252,263,272] • The population may not be familiar with the notification signal (extending the time to respond and possibly leading to investigative activities) and may not be aware of the response required of them. This may lead them to misinterpret the information provided to them and/or respond in an unpredictable (and sub-optimal) manner. During an event, they may seek to compensate for this lack of understanding by seeking further information, confirming the existence of cues and then determining the response required of them. Unprimed individuals [144,169,207,208,211,214,217,223,240,241,249,252,263,272] • Given that the population is completely unaware of the incident prior to the call to evacuate, they may have commenced an activity from which they are reluctant to disengage or may have moved further away from safety within the building. Both of these situations would increase the time that it takes them to reach a position of safety. In relation to the performance of contrived trials it is critical that the participants are unprimed, especially if their behavioral response is being monitored. Elderly [31,52,140,146,160,173,174,178,185,193,209,212,213,215,219,222,229,232,233,244,251,253, 256,258,273,284,295,346] • This population may be subject to a variety of different health issues and impairments. As with the rest of the population there is a degree of variation in their abilities, such that some may be healthy and fit, while others have a number of cognitive and physical issues. However, in general, this group will have a higher proportion of ailments and other limiting conditions than a younger population. This situation will generally deteriorate as the person ages. Depending on the individual involved, they may have physical, sensory and/or cognitive impairments that influence their

48

perception, comprehension and response to an incident. As such their vulnerability represents a combination of several of the other vulnerabilities highlighted here; e.g. physical impairment, hearing impairment, cognitive impairment, etc. In some situations, the nature of the problems associated with the elderly are addressed and well understood (e.g. hospitals, care homes, etc.), while in public spaces they will be considered as part of the general population (e.g. shopping malls). Those asleep [176,177,179,180,182-184,187,188,190-192,196,197,199,200,215,216,226,231,235- 237,239,247,250,255,259,291-295,296] • Reduced alertness makes this population particularly susceptible to a delayed response. They will be less likely to notice the development of the fire, receive cues (visual, aural, or olfactory) from the fire and less likely to be informed by the notification system in place. The population will also be less alert when awakened; i.e. it will take them longer on awakening to recognize the situation and determine a response. Other vulnerabilities combine to make sleepers particular susceptible; e.g. the sleep deprived, medicated, intoxicated, young, etc. These groups tend to have altered sleep patterns. Sleep deprived[254,255] • Sleep deprivation may influence sleep patterns, reduce motor functions leading to clumsiness (possibly leading to behavior that makes a fire more likely) and decision- making, potentially leading to a less appropriate response. It may also lead to the individual taking medication in order to combat the condition. Drug impairment (medication/illegal narcotics) [47,155,264,290] • This can produce a variety of different effects, according to the nature of the drug itself. It may influence sleep patterns, cognitive abilities, motor skills, alertness and/or the physical response. Depending on the nature of the drug (e.g. medication, narcotics, etc.) it can therefore influence the receipt of a signal, perception and understanding of the signal and the ability to respond. Alcohol impairment [31,136,152,143,155,161,202,225,227,242,288,290,294] • This may influence sleep patterns, cognitive abilities, motor skills and alertness. It can influence the receipt of a signal, perception and understanding of the signal and the ability to respond. It can also be paired with other behaviors (e.g. smoking) that can influence the probability of the fire starting and the individual’s proximity to the fire. It can also influence the susceptibility of the individual succumbing to the fire effluent; i.e. succumbing to the effects of and toxic gases. The impact of alcohol can occur at relatively low blood alcohol levels. This is important as those people that have had only a modest amount of alcohol (e.g. two units) who may not feel impaired, but may still be more difficult to notify, especially when they are asleep. This can have further implications, if the individual has someone in their care. Chronic Health Problems [52,221] • The impact will largely depend on the nature of the ailment and its severity. In the most severe cases, the individual may not be able to respond or may have their movement severely hindered. Fire safety not the responsibility of the group [58,224] • In some situations a group may feel that the response to an incident is the responsibility of a previously defined group other than themselves. A population that does not feel responsible for their safety may delay their response and wait for the actions of others that have been deemed responsible. This places the onus upon the

49

responsible parties to act appropriately, reach the population in time, and to be sufficiently well trained to know what to do when they arrive. Depending on the nature of the incident, it may take responders a significant amount of time to reach the people waiting for them. Fire not considered likely or a threat [61,232,240,241,273,276] • In some situations fire may not be considered to be a threat, to be addressed by technological or procedural measures, or to be highly improbable. This may influence the interpretation of cues received. In such a case, the presence of a cue (e.g. smoke) or the provision of a notification signal may be ignored. Given this, the behavioral response of the population may be delayed. Fire alarms assumed to be false [115,186,218] • A detection system may not be maintained, installed appropriately, or properly configured and results in false positives. An environment may continually present conditions such that the detectors are activated by non-threatening conditions (e.g., burnt toast in dorms, high temperatures in mechanical rooms). An environment may also be subject to malicious false alarms, where occupants intentionally set off the alarm system. All of these situations may lead to the population not trusting the information provided and delay their response given that they do not necessarily associate the notification signal/message with a real incident. This factor can be reduced through technological advances (i.e. improving the accuracy of the detection systems) and procedural developments (e.g. reducing the number of inaccurate reports or malicious actions). Physically impaired [58,88,145,228,265,282,286] • The presence of a physical impairment may hinder the ability of an individual to respond to a signal/message from the notification system. Therefore, even if the message to respond is received and recognized, the physically impaired population may not be able to respond effectively. This population may have care-givers present that are responsible for their safe evacuation. Mentally impaired [58,88,265,286] • This may influence the ability to interpret the notification signal/message, assuming that it is received, and then establish an appropriate response. Again, this population may have designated care-givers. Presence of Background Pollution (e.g. noise, lights, distractions, etc.) [250,251] • The presence of background pollution may interfere with the ability to receive the emergency signal/message. This may be due to the background pollution masking the notification signal/message or causing the signal/message to be less distinctive. This may lead to difficulties in receiving the notification signal/message. Being Alone/ Being Isolated [31,61,232,257] • An individual being isolated or alone reduces the possibility of being notified or rescued by another member of the population. This is particularly problematic in domestic situations where less sophisticated notification systems are in place, and staff/wardens may not be present. Engaged in an activity (i.e. attention focused elsewhere, level of commitment) [115] • This may extend the pre-evacuation time in two distinct ways. Firstly, the attention of the population may be focused on a particular event reducing the probability of them noticing cues that indicate an incident. Secondly, a population committed to a location

50

may be less likely to initiate evacuation. For instance, a population that has invested resources (e.g. time, money, etc.) into being at a location may be reluctant to leave the space, especially where the nature of the incident is not obvious. Unfamiliar with signal/message[133] • The population may not recognize the signal/message provided and/or may not associate it with an emergency. They may therefore require additional cues in order to convince them of the nature of the incident. Unfamiliar with surroundings [13,133] • The population may not notice the change in the situation (i.e. the development of an incident). They may not then recognize the cues perceived as indicating a fire or recognize the significance of a cue. For instance, the smell of smoke in a restaurant. This may extend the time it takes them to respond to an incident. Fearful of responding (e.g. institutions) [58] • In structures that have a strict, formal hierarchy (e.g. care homes, schools, institutions, etc.), occupants may be reluctant to act in a manner that contravenes rules governing behavior in a non-emergency; e.g. opening an exit that sets off a security alarm. This may make them reluctant to respond to an emergency incident. People in large groups [13,336] • In large groups, especially where the group is familiar with each other, there may be some reluctance to be the first to respond or appearing to be the first people to respond. People in public spaces [13,133,336] • People may be unfamiliar with the notification system, the structure, the hazards present and the population around them. People in public spaces may then be subject to a number of the vulnerabilities highlighted above. All of these factors may lead the population to misinterpret cues, misunderstand or mistrust instructions and delay their response. They may also be unfamiliar with the response required of them leading to an inefficient response. Speakers of Non-Native Languages [133] • Where information is provided in a native language (e.g., English, French, Japanese, etc.), a sub-population that does not speak that language may not be aware of the incident and/or may not understand the required response described to them. This may lead the group to not understanding the nature or existence of an incident, and not knowing what to do in response. Those affected by security issues [58,133] • The prospect of security breaches may lead to restrictive security measures being adopted; e.g. where a ticket is required to enter a space. Given the nature of these measures (managed access, barred windows, etc.) they may directly restrict the egress routes available to evacuees during an incident [132]. Increasing security at transportation hubs, event arenas and educational facilities is emphasizing these issues.

To enable these groups to be further examined in a systematic manner a vulnerability model has been developed. This approach involved the notification process being broken down into its constituent components. This is discussed in the following section. The nature of vulnerabilities was then assessed and the notification technologies that could address the vulnerabilities were

51

identified. This allowed a clear definition of the vulnerability and, most importantly, whether this vulnerability could be ameliorated through improvements in notification technologies. This examination was confined to the delivery of the signal/message and therefore excluded issues of implementation, maintenance, or connectivity that may be related to the use of a particular notification system; although important issues, they were beyond the scope of the project (see Figure 18).

Connectivity

Alarm lmplementati Familiarity Maintenance selection on and Operation Preparation

, ;------,------._ \ I ------I : Method of Energy Means of : detection Supply Notification

\ y I

Figure 18: Focus of this analysis.

52

5 Stage I: Categorization of the Notification Process The vulnerability model adopted during this project required breaking down the notification process into discrete components. This allows the vulnerabilities to be better understood and establishes the effectiveness of the existing notification technologies at addressing these vulnerabilities. Four constituent components were identified. These four constituent components were derived from the literature review associated with the notification process and were particularly influenced by the seminal work of Mileti [64,65]. These four components encapsulate the key phases in the notification process once the signal/message has been provided:

a) Receipt of signal/message by the target population - the ability of the notification system to attract the attention of the target population; how effective is the notification system in alerting the target population? b) Recognition of the signal/message - Given that the target population receives the signal are they able to determine that the signal denotes an actual fire? Is the target population able to differentiate between the signal or message and others that they may received, such as car alarms, security alarms, etc.? c) Identification of Response - Given that the target population is able to receive the message/signal they are then able to identify the required response. Is the target population able to determine the appropriate response to the alarm signal? d) Evacuee Response – Given that the target population knows what response to perform, are they are actively able to do so?

This approach was important in order to establish which of the vulnerabilities identified were due to notification (a-c in the previous list) or due to some other vulnerability that could not be addressed through the improvement of notification technology. For instance, if someone was not able to respond to the notification system, the impact of improving it might only be minimal. This process both clarified our understanding of the vulnerabilities and later assisted in the prioritization of the vulnerabilities required as part of this project.

The vulnerabilities were re-examined to categorize them according to the phase of the notification process to which they were vulnerable. A metric was produced to clarify this categorization process. This enabled the nature and the severity (i.e. whether there was no vulnerability, a slight vulnerability , or a severe vulnerability) of the vulnerabilities to be better understood (see Table 16). This process required a simplification of the vulnerabilities in question; however, this was essential in order to gain a general appreciation of the vulnerabilities and then make comparisons between them. These reflect the potential extent of the vulnerabilities in each group. In reality, the extent of the vulnerability would be dependent upon the individual involved and the manner in which the vulnerability manifested itself in them. This simplification allowed the vulnerability to be characterized.

This type of categorization describes the behavioral response that is affected in each group; i.e. how the vulnerability expresses itself in a particular group. For instance, children younger than 5 years old are deemed to be vulnerable in all phases of the notification process (indicated by a “strong” rating): receiving and understanding the message, knowing how to respond to it, and then being able to perform this action. Therefore, a component of their vulnerability (i.e. their

53

ability to respond to the incident) is beyond the direct reach of notification technology. In contrast, it was established that the hearing impaired were deemed only to have vulnerabilities related to receiving and possibly recognizing the notification message, and do not necessarily have innate vulnerabilities associated with their response. As such, technological improvements to the notification system may benefit the hearing impaired.

Table 16: Categorization of the different vulnerability groups. (1) Receipt of (2) Recognition of (3) Identification (4) Respond signal/message signal/ message of Response Individual Differences1 slight slight slight slight Chronic Health Issues STRONG STRONG STRONG STRONG Committed to not leaving / slight slight STRONG Engaged in an Activity Fearful of responding STRONG People in large groups slight slight Children younger than 5 STRONG STRONG STRONG STRONG Children older than 5 STRONG STRONG STRONG STRONG Hearing impaired STRONG slight Visual impairment STRONG STRONG STRONG STRONG Untrained/Unprimed individuals slight STRONG STRONG Slight Elderly 1 STRONG STRONG STRONG STRONG Those asleep slight slight slight Sleep deprived STRONG STRONG slight Drug impairment 2 STRONG STRONG STRONG STRONG Alcohol impairment 3 STRONG STRONG STRONG STRONG Non native speaker slight STRONG STRONG Mentally impaired STRONG STRONG STRONG Background Pollution STRONG STRONG Being Alone slight slight slight slight Those subject to false alarms STRONG Physically impaired STRONG Fire Safety Not Responsibility slight STRONG Fire Not Considered slight STRONG Likely/Threat Unfamiliar with signal STRONG STRONG Unfamiliar with surroundings slight STRONG slight People in Public Spaces slight slight STRONG slight Those affected by security issues STRONG 1 Highly dependent upon the individual involved. 2 Dependent on the drug used. 3 Dependent on the amount of alcohol involved.

This vulnerability model was developed to understand the nature of the vulnerabilities and also to allow us focus on those groups that were deemed most vulnerable, but which could benefit most from the development of notification technologies; i.e. this was part of the prioritization process.

The detailed review and the separation of these vulnerabilities into their four constituent parts highlighted how vulnerabilities were not necessarily static, nor independent of each other or the scenario; instead they were dynamic and could evolve and interact in complex ways. Importantly, the analysis also highlighted how the vulnerabilities could propagate; i.e. influence an individual and others involved in the incident. Lee examined this possibility in his detailed review of factors

54

that influence the notification process in residential occupancies (see Figure 19(a)) [297] Here it is apparent that the vulnerabilities of a particular group had an impact upon the success of their response; this is termed the primary effect. However, this ‘vulnerability’ may also have an impact on another group of people (see Figure 19(b)); this is termed the secondary effect. In this manner the vulnerability in question may propagate beyond the immediate group of interest. This type of effect was considered during the prioritization of the set of vulnerabilities; this was key in establishing the research priorities for this project.

Fire/Smoke.--. Detected .._. Smoke Alarm Sounds

Fire/Smok,._____ Smoke Alarm Detected -. Sounds, or Alternative Signal

Additional escape time may Adults Children be needed Wake Wake

Children Adults Exit Exit

Safety Safety

Figure 19: (a) Primary effect of the vulnerability; (b) Secondary effect of the vulnerability [297]

For each of the vulnerabilities, a set of factors was established highlighting the primary impact of this vulnerability upon their response (see examples in Table 17). In addition to this, the secondary impact of the vulnerability was established; i.e. the potential impact on others. In this manner, the capacity for the vulnerability to propagate to other members of a population was examined. Not only is it difficult to get a consistent assessment of the vulnerabilities from examining high-level statistics, but the secondary impact of these vulnerabilities may also be overlooked; for instance, the presence of alcohol in the blood of one individual might have contributed to their demise, but may also influence the outcome of someone in their care. This consideration was therefore important both in terms of understanding the impact of the vulnerabilities and their prioritization, but also in identifying vulnerabilities that might be overlooked but have an important secondary impact upon the outcome.

55

Table 17: Primary and Secondary impact of vulnerabilities. Primary Effect Secondary Effect Children younger than 5 Depth of sleep / sleep patterns. Inability to Likely to be the responsibility of another. May comprehend seriousness of situation. require rescue potentially placing the other person Inability to form appropriate response. at risk. May respond therefore making a search for them more difficult. Children older than 5 Depth of sleep / sleep patterns. Likely to be the responsibility of another. May Inability to comprehend the serious of the require rescue potentially placing the other person situation. Inability to form appropriate response. at risk. May respond therefore making a search for without training. them more difficult. Deaf/Hearing impaired Difficulty in receiving aural signal. May be May require rescue/assistance from another person. undiagnosed or unaddressed; therefore no May prevent person in a position of responsibility technological aid. Prevalent in the elderly. rescuing someone else. Unfamiliar/ Inability to recognize notification message and Delay may prevent them from rescuing someone Untrained/Unprimed then respond appropriately. else in their care. individuals Elderly More likely to have hearing, visual, cognitive, If living in shared accommodation, may require physical impairment. More likely to live alone. notification and/or rescue These factors (alone or in combination) may pose difficulties in reacting or responding to notification. Being asleep Difficult to receive message and may also delay May delay/prevent them from rescuing someone establishing response. else in their care. Sleep deprived Subject to different sleep patterns. Difficult to Likely to delay/prevent them from rescuing receive and process message. someone else in their care. May otherwise be seen as able to perform their responsibilities; i.e. the condition may be short-term and not associated with the individual who may then be deemed able to adopt a position of care. Drug impairment Difficult to arouse. Likely to be cognitive Likely to delay/prevent them from rescuing impaired. Potentially mobility impaired. May someone else in their care. May lead to making bad lead to behaviors that cause fire. decisions even if reaching person in their care. Alcohol impairment Difficult to arouse. Likely to be cognitive Likely to delay/prevent them from rescuing impaired. Potentially mobility impaired. May someone else in their care. May lead to making bad lead to behaviors that cause fire. decisions even if reaching person in their care. Background Pollution Reduce effectiveness of notification system. May delay/prevent them from rescuing someone May place groups that are not normally else in their care. considered vulnerable (and who do not consider themselves as being vulnerable) into a vulnerable position. Being Alone Will reduce the number of chances of receiving notification. Especially dangerous where the individual is vulnerable. Unimpaired Should enable a typical response. May be expected to be in a position of However, may be less effective if subject to responsibility and therefore assist groups with an situational/ experiential vulnerabilities innate vulnerability. Non-native Speaking Does not understand the message provided to If caring for someone else, particular if they also do them. not speak the native language, it may prolong their response as well.

56

In addition to their ability to propagate to other members of a population, vulnerabilities may arise in a number of ways. The nature of the vulnerability will influence the likelihood that remedial measures had been taken to address it, and also the possibility of the vulnerability being recorded in high level statistics; i.e. that it was being looked for, that it could be detected, that the recording mechanism then allowed this information to be stored.

Innate Factors } Fire Notification Situational Factors ... developing Technology Experiential Factors ' . ~ r Development Outcome ... of scenario (largely unknown)

Recorded in general statistics Figure 20: The factors leading' to vulnerabilities.

It was possible to categorize the groups according to whether the vulnerabilities were an attribute of the individual, the situation or the knowledge/experience of the individual (i.e. that could be addressed through procedural activities). This is useful in determining situations that place otherwise capable populations at risk and also the likelihood of these being addressed and/or recorded (see Figure 20). These factors allowed us to further understand the nature of the vulnerabilities and highlight those that may produce vulnerabilities.

Innate Vulnerabilities – Those that the individual carries and are currently fixed. In some instances corrective measures may be taken to compensate for these vulnerabilities e.g. Age; Impairment level, language skills, etc.

Experiential vulnerabilities (e.g. experience and training) – those that the individual carries but that may be changed by education and training; e.g. exposure to false alarms, level of training, etc.

Situational Vulnerabilities –Factors that increase the vulnerability of all groups given the incident scenario; e.g. being in a group/alone, presence of background pollution, intoxication, being asleep, etc. Technological Vulnerabilities –Factors that increase the vulnerability of (sections of) the population given the effectiveness of the notification technology in place; e.g. the location of the notification devices, the coverage level, whether the signal/message is discernable, whether it clearly indicates a response, etc.

This analysis established that it was possible for those members of the population that would not normally be considered not to be vulnerable (e.g. unimpaired adults) to be placed in a vulnerable situation. This has significant implications to the individuals themselves and those that that may be in their care. For instance, in many situations normally unimpaired adults may be responsible

57

for the well-being of some with innate vulnerabilities (e.g. a small child). If the unimpaired adult is temporarily impaired, especially through the impact of a situational vulnerability (e.g. intoxicated), then not only does it increase the risk posed to them, but also to those in their care. These factors may also interact to increase the risk to an innately impaired individual (e.g. an intoxicated elderly individual).

5.1 Recommendations Derived from Previous Empirical Research In addition to deriving recommendations from the existing empirical, statistical, theoretical and analytical material, it was felt that it would be of value to take into consideration those recommendations made in the existing empirical literature where the material related to the effectiveness of notification technologies (see Table 19-Table 21). It is acknowledged that the recommendations are made in different ways and with different degrees of emphasis. In addition, these recommendations are provided out of context and in many of the cases the recommendations directly relate to the work conducted at the time. This has been simplified in order to reduce the number of categories to a manageable level. Therefore the mention of an area of further research is recorded, rather than the full context of the recommendation. The digest of recommendations presented here does at least provide some indication of the topics considered worthy of further work by those involved in collecting the data.

These recommendations are categorized as follows: those that relate to the vulnerabilities; those that relate to the data collection procedure; those that relate to the notification technology being examined; and those that fall beyond the notification issue itself, but were mentioned in the notification literature. The research referenced here represents an indication of the recommendations made; they do not represent an exhaustive list of recommendations, and there are a number of implicit recommendations made in the references cited that are not mentioned here. However, this description does provide an insight into the areas previously seen as requiring additional work. Table 18: Recommended topics of further research related to vulnerabilities Vulnerability Examples of research where recommendations were made Children [129, 297, 298,279] Hearing impaired [193, 73] Training [297] Elderly [177,31] Sleep deprived [193] Mentally Ill [193] Drug impairment [259] Alcohol impairment [298] Being Alone [Technical Panel] Awake [129,193] Background Noise [193] Gender Differences [129] Public Spaces/ [Technical Panel] Groups

58

Table 19: Recommended topics of further research related to experimental design. Experimental Design / Measure Examples of research where recommendations were made Approach to Waking [Technical Panel] Measure time to evacuate [247] /Terminology Lab. vs. Natural [259]

Table 20: Recommended topics of further research related to addressing the vulnerability. Means of addressing Vulnerability Examples of research where recommendations were made Vibrating device [183] Alternative Cues [297] Understanding Message [81,82] Multiple Cues/Voice [251] White Noise [Technical Panel] T-3 Mix [Technical Panel]

Table 21: Recommended topics of further research related to issues beyond notification. Beyond Notification Examples of research where recommendations were made Interconnected [193] Detection Capability [298] Education (e.g. fire development) [31] Escape Plan [31]

It is apparent that a wide range of factors are suggested for future work. This is particularly the case in reference to the vulnerabilities themselves. This information in and of itself will not guide the recommendations made here. However, it provides support when cross-referenced with the analysis already performed and the other sources examined; e.g. the omissions in current empirical data presented in the next section.

5.2 Mapping Previous Empirical Research In an effort to further inform the data collection activities performed in Stage II of this project, an analysis of a selection of the existing empirical data-sets available according to the particular vulnerability and the notification technology employed. These are limited to work reported in English and identified during the project. This includes work conducted by Ball, Bruck, Johnson, Fiddell, Petzoldt, Nober, DeVoss, Ashley, Busby, Lynch, DuBois, Bowman, Pivik, Ross, Basch , Bonnet, Gwynne, Shields, Boyce and Purser amongst many others [14,42,47,73-96,99- 102,112,129,130,174-177,180-184,187-197,203,204,210,239,246-251,259,336,346,347]. This work specifically

59

relates to the effectiveness of notification technologies in effecting a response in the target population. A schematic of this analysis is shown in Table 22. Importantly, it should be recognized that the results presented in this research occasionally contradict each other; for instance the work of Ashley [174,175] and Nober [246-248]on the effectiveness of strobe signaling. Table 22: Empirical data on the effectiveness of notification technologies according to the vulnerabilities examined. Tone T-3 T-3 Voice Vibr. Strobe Olf. Air (Var) move ment Unimpaired/ Adult Children

Hearing impaired

Visual impairment

Training

Elderly

Sleep deprived

Drug impairment (illegal and medication) 5 Alcohol impairment 5

People in large groups

Mentally impaired

Presence of Background Noise Awake

In the schematic shown a red cell indicates research performed on whether an individual responded at all; a yellow cell indicates research performed on whether an individual responded and the time that it took them to respond; and a green cell indicates research that was performed on whether an individual responded, the time that it took them to respond and their ability to perform afterwards. The references in the table indicate examples of the research examining particular issues.

It is immediately apparent that this represents a sparse array of data, with significant omissions in our empirical understanding. It should not be assumed that simply because there is a gap in this understanding that we should necessarily conduct research to fill it. Indeed, part of this analysis

60

required us to prioritize the omissions. In addition, the fact that data exists does not mean that it satisfactorily supports our understanding of the phenomena involved. This matrix will be used to support the analysis performed in order to understand vulnerabilities and the technological response and the subsequent recommendations made for future work.

5.3 Identifying Key Vulnerabilities

The various modes of analysis described above (the results of the application of the vulnerability model, the extent of vulnerability propagation, the nature of the vulnerability itself, recommendations of previous research, and the availability of existing data, etc.) enabled the selection of vulnerabilities that warranted further analysis in Stage II of this project. In addition, technical, practical and ethical considerations were examined during the selection of vulnerabilities for future work. Several vulnerabilities were therefore discarded because it would not have been feasible to include the population in the data collection process (e.g. drug impairment); their impact was deemed to be approximated by another of the vulnerabilities which was included they were either seen as being the result of a combination of other vulnerabilities, or deemed to be equivalent in terms of the impact that they may have.

As with other aspects of this work, the effect of the vulnerabilities (and how they influence performance) is the focus of this assessment, rather than the underlying causes. This approach was adopted given the difficulties at establishing the fundamental causes and also because by adopting this approach allowed us to combine vulnerabilities that affected people in similar ways.

Outlined below are the vulnerabilities excluded from the further analysis, with a brief indication of the reason: • Individual differences: will be accounted for in the data collection process rather than require additional studies; i.e. the natural variation produced can account for this issue, without necessarily conducting separate trials. • Chronic Health Issues: would be difficult to test in a trial in a manner that was representative; may present ethical problems; given the nature of the ailment, notification systems may only have a limited impact upon the outcome, especially where the conditions reduces mobility. • Committed to not leaving or engaged: population may choose to ignore the notification system. In addition, different levels of commitment/engagement could be present in any trials conducting involving a cross-section of the public evacuating from public spaces. These can be rolled into the examination of other groups (e.g. those in public spaces). • Fear of responding: primarily a procedural issue rather than a notification issue. As such, the population may be fully aware of the nature of the incident, but reluctant to respond without actions of the staff. This issue may also be addressed when examining trials involving public spaces. • Children: may have physical and psychological limitations that reduce the benefit of the notification systems. May also be deemed to be the responsibility of a third party; e.g. a parent, etc. Given the propensity for carers searching/retrieving children may not

61

necessarily be an advantage having the children responding at all. This would need to be coordinated with the evacuation plan. • Visual Impairment: A significant component of their difficulty is beyond the notification system; i.e. in their ability to respond. Possible solution could involve directional systems and other devices to locate the exits [132], which might also benefit the non-visually impaired. Issues relating to the identification of the routes available, the location of the incident and the activities of those around them. These issues are also influenced by the level of familiarity that the visually impaired individual has with the space; i.e. the problem becomes much more severe when the surroundings are not familiar. • Untrained/Unprimed/Unfamiliar/Large Groups/Subject to False Alarms/Public Spaces: these issues can be examined in conjunction with each other, given that they would likely co-exist. For instance, in public spaces involving transient populations, it would be likely that a range of people would be present who would be untrained and would be therefore be unfamiliar with the notification system in place and the response desired of them. • Mentally Impaired: ethical issues preclude the performance of these trials; they may be unduly disruptive and traumatic for those involved. Also, a significant vulnerability lies beyond the scope of notification systems; i.e. that the individual may not be able to recognize the response required of them. • Being Alone: can be covered in trials examining other factors, such as public spaces. Also, to some degree, it falls beyond the scope of this project; i.e. the likelihood of an individual being alone is not remedied by the notification system; it may be remedied by procedural and/or social issues. • Physically Impaired: the nature of the vulnerability lies beyond the effectiveness of the notification system. • Fire Safety Not Responsibility/Fire Not Considered a Threat: these factors could be examined when investigating the effectiveness of notification systems in public spaces. • Security Issues: may be too disruptive to investigate in a natural setting. May be included in a more general examination of the effectiveness of notification systems in public spaces.

By excluding these vulnerabilities, it is not suggesting that they are not important, nor that they are not worthy of further examination. However, as part of this project it was necessary to identify vulnerabilities amenable to notification technologies, and highlight those worthy of further research. As part of the prioritization process, it was therefore necessary to narrow the focus of this examination, excluding the majority of the vulnerabilities identified.

62

6 Stage I: Notification Technology

One of the goals of this project was to not only identify and characterize vulnerabilities within the population, but also to identify potential technical solutions. This requires a broad assessment of the current state of notification technologies available. A literature review has therefore been conducted in order to examine the current notification technologies and their potential applications (see Technology section in Reference List). It also enables us to suggest the technologies that will most likely assist particular vulnerabilities and determine the feasibility of the recommendations made.

6.1 State of the Art In order to combat the problems of notification, a number of technological solutions have been suggested. These adopt a variety of different approaches in order to notify the target populations. Some of these technologies are specifically designed to combat particular vulnerabilities (e.g. hearing impairment). These alternative/assistive technologies aim to improve the effectiveness of the smoke alarm signal and overall fire safety. Some background information is also provided on the limitations of the systems described. It should be noted that examples will be provided for the techniques described. These examples will by no means represent an exhaustive list; instead they demonstrate that the technology exists and provides a representative example of the capabilities present. Previous reviews of fire alarm technologies can be found elsewhere [37,47,60,64,65,72-93,99,131,178-211,218-221,226,232-241,246-251,256,259,260,269,278-280,298-336,346-348]; the reader is directed to these articles for a more detailed review of the systems available. A number of internet references are provided to existing notification technologies as part of the reference material at the end of this document.

The potential technical solutions identified have been broadly categorized as those that provide audible alarm signals, visual signals, tactile signals, and olfactory signals, along with the scope of the information provided by each system. These findings are presented in the next section.

The technologies can be broadly separated into systems composed of detection and sounder technology that are stand-alone devices (e.g. those employed in a domestic environment and referenced in the applicable codes that require them as Single/Multiple Station Detectors or Smoke Alarms) and those that are employed as part of a larger Fire Alarm Signaling System (e.g. network of distributed devices that contribute to a large notification system). Whether a standalone device or a system is employed has a significant impact on the resources available, the power available, the number of devices that can be employed and, eventually, the flexibility and sophistication of the technology involved.

6.1.1 Audible Systems - Tone Since 1996, NFPA 72, National Fire Alarm Code, has required the use of a three-pulse temporal pattern, or temporal-three, for audible signals in new fire alarm systems. This signal is intended to indicate that immediate evacuation of the building is required. Identifying an optimal evacuation signal that will reach occupants and be heard and recognized is difficult because of variations (e.g., loudness, frequency, pattern, etc.) and background noise among occupancies as

63

well as various human factors (e.g. status, presence of impairments, activities, etc.). The temporal-three standard only specifies the on/off pattern of the signal; it does not specify the frequencies to be employed.

Although not mandated, residential smoke alarms typically employ an alarm frequency of 3,000 – 4,000 Hz. The alarm signal in a smoke alarm is typically generated with a piezoelectric horn. These devices are used due to their ability to produce significant sound levels with relatively little power, which is essential when relying on batteries as a power source. This is necessary given the nature of the device used in residential/domestic settings: a stand alone detector/emitter.

The UL standard for single-station smoke alarms, UL 217 [337], also provides requirements for the smoke alarm signal. These requirements include the use of the temporal-three pattern and also require that a minimum sound level of 85 dBA be produced at 10 feet from a smoke alarm operating in a room of a specific configuration.

The high-pitched T-3 signal currently used in smoke alarms is consistent with the belief that increased signal speed, pitch and repetition are the best parameters for warnings when people are awake. However, empirical studies have indicated that a lower frequency or mixed frequency signals might be more effective [129,187-197]. There has been interest in technologies that provide alternative audible alarm signals to improve the effectiveness of smoke alarms in recent years, however only a small number of commercial smoke alarms were found that address this issue.

For instance, the Darrow Company produces a smoke alarm called Loudenlow that provides a low-frequency (250-300 Hz) alarm signal2. The low-frequency sound is delivered via a six inch dynamic speaker that is attached to an ionization smoke alarm. However, there are several important points regarding the practicality and ease-of-use of this type of technology. Producing low frequency sounds at a high volume requires significantly different hardware than is currently used for smoke alarms; the equipment required in this case is significantly bulkier than that normally used. The large size required by the current technology available may affect the location/placement of the smoke alarm, as well raise concerns with homeowners regarding aesthetics. Another possible concern with regards to the practicality of this type of technology is power consumption. For instance, the Loudenlow alarm is battery operated and draws 1 A (9 watts) of power when in alarm mode, substantially more than a typical alarm system. Therefore, six AA batteries are required to power the Loudenlow, with a battery life of one year. 3

No current smoke alarms were found that provide a mixed frequency alarm signal. Many combination carbon monoxide and smoke alarms now include a voice recording that identifies which alarm criteria has been detected; however, the primary purpose of the voice recording here is to provide additional contextual information to occupants.

One possible contributing factor to the lack of alternative audible alarm signals (e.g. low frequency and mixed frequency signals) relates to the fact that the piezoelectric horns used in

2 http://www.loudenlow.com 3 The Loudenlow is not listed to UL 217, Standard for Single- and Multiple-Station Alarms.

64

current residential smoke alarms cannot produce a low frequency sound or output a voice message at the required dB level and still satisfy the battery requirements in UL 217, Single and Multiple Station Smoke Alarms” [337]. Therefore, a significant technology change in terms of the design and hardware commonly used in smoke alarms is required to make this technology more prevalent in the marketplace.

Notification systems that are integral to, or support fire alarm systems designed for public spaces are able to generate a range of different tones and signals (including voice messages, see below). For instance, the LifeAlarm system offered by Simplex 4 is able to generate the following tones: slow whoop (rising from 200Hz ro 830 Hz); High/Low (with a high of 750 Hz lasting for 0.1 second and then a 500Hz tone lasting for 0.4 second); a horn (either at 120 or 20 beats per minute); a chime (600 Hz with a 1.5 second delay time); continuous bell (fundamental frequency of 350Hz with harmonics at 700Hz and 210Hz); bell (60 beats per minute); and the T-3 (with half second intervals, using the horn, bell, whoop or chime).

In contrast to traditional tonal systems, a system has been developed that provides broadband, multi-frequency frequencies in an attempt to guide evacuees to an available exit. This is based on the technology developed at the University of Leeds, UK [131]. This is able to present a set of pulses, covering a range of frequencies between 20Hz – 20,000Hz. This is accompanied (preceded) by an additional tone to alert the population of the alarm. In effect, it implicitly represents an attempt to provide additional information to the evacuating population. Given the broadband nature of the signal provided, it is also claimed that it may aid sections of the hearing 5 impaired population. This device is UL (464) listed.

6.1.2 Audible Systems - Voice The capabilities of voice notification systems depend on the type of occupancy involved. In large-scale public spaces the notification system will generally require a network of components connected together and powered off the mains. This system would then provide for a range of capabilities that are not dictated either by power concerns or miniaturization. A number of notification systems are available (e.g. including those provided by SimplexGrinnell, Siemens, EST, and Notifier6) that are able to provide voice notification. Many of these systems are capable of annunciation of different messages/signals over multiple channels, allowing different areas of a structure to be addressed separately, with different messages simultaneously. This allows managed procedures to be employed, where sub-sections of the population can be evacuated/relocated or kept in place accordingly. The messages can be provided in a number of forms. Firstly, a specific message can be automatically selected from a library of messages according to the nature of the incident. Secondly, a live announcement can be made overriding the pre-recorded message. This has a number of advantages: the perception of the message as being current and authentic; the ability to provide scenario-specific information; the ability to adapt the procedure according to the nature of the event and the response of the population. In addition, many of these notification systems have the capacity to splice messages such that fragments of digitally recorded material can be recombined for a particular scenario. This

4 http://www.simplexgrinnell.com 5 Although it should be noted that this listing does not relate directly to its effectiveness as a notification device. 6 http://www.est.net/fire.cfm , http://www.sbt.siemens.com/FIS/ , http://www.notifier.com/main.htm

65

increases the efficiency of the system in general, but also allows a greater range of scenarios to be addressed if need be.

These messages can be accompanied by a range of tones and sounds, in order to alert the population of the upcoming voice message. For instance, in the Simplex LifeAlarm system, mentioned above, the message can be preceded by one of a range of tonal signals including the T-3 signal; this can be selected from a range of sounds. Therefore, the systems currently available would be able to address a range of recommendations without needing significant changes to the hardware.

In contrast to these large-scale voice notification systems, residential/domestic systems are subject to greater limitations. The use of the piezoelectric horn generally employed precludes the low frequencies necessary for the voice to be projected. However, there are examples of small- scale voice notification systems. For instance, SignalONE Safety, Inc. produces the KidSmart Vocal Smoke Alarm (012504). This is a smoke alarm that incorporates a voice recording that alternates with the typical T-3 smoke alarm signal. The technology used in this alarm allows for personalized escape instructions to be provided in a familiar voice. In terms of practicality and ease of use, this technology is similar to a typical smoke alarm. However, in order to use the voice feature of the Kidsmart alarm7, the voice message needs to be recorded by a user prior to installation. Recording the vocal warning is a relatively straightforward process. Unlike most smoke alarms that require a single nine volt battery, four AA batteries are required to power the KidSmart vocal smoke alarm.8 This model of the KidSmart smoke alarms has recently been UL- listed. 9

6.1.3 Tactile Systems The sense of touch may also serve as a useful alarm signal, especially for those with a hearing disability or a situational constraint preventing them from perceiving other signals (e.g. background noise). Vibratory devices such as bed/pillow shakers are the primary means of tactile stimulation commonly used; however the use of air movement as a means of alerting has also been discussed. In addition, systems are being designed that can take advantage of non- emergency technologies (pagers, cell phones, etc.). This may be of particular value when an individual is in a public space; i.e. not in a pre-determined location where a larger vibrating device can be implemented

UL 1971 provides requirements for tactile signaling devices [280]. Vibratory devices (that are not worn) must produce a displacement of one-eighth of an inch at a frequency of 60 – 120 Hz and have a cross-sectional area of at least 6 in2. Air movement systems are required to produce a minimum peak air velocity of 270 feet/min at a distance of 5 ft. The air velocity must vary from zero to the peak velocity 15 to 20 cycles per minute.

Although not yet prevalent as part of smoke alarm systems, vibratory alarms in the form of bed shakers and portable pagers are readily available to address the needs of the hearing impaired.

7 http://www.kidsmartcorp.com/product_signalone.aspx 8 While battery only smoke alarms are widely available, the ICC requires that they be wired to the building power (110vac) with battery backup 9 Although no information could be found on what standard it had been listed.

66

These products are used to alert the hearing impaired to common occurrences such as the telephone or door bell ringing, a baby crying, to wake up in the morning (an alarm clock). Generally, the vibratory device is just a component that attaches to the alarm clock or alerting system. Since the smoke alarm needs to be placed on or near the ceiling and the vibratory alarm needs to be in contact with a person being alerted, some means of communication is necessary to allow the vibratory device to know when a smoke alarm is activated. With the recent introduction of technologies that allow wireless interconnection of battery-operated smoke alarms, vibratory stimuli for emergency alerting will likely become more prevalent.

Several manufacturers such as Clarity, ClearSounds, Sonic Alert, and Silent Call10, produce vibrating pads (bed shakers) that incorporate with their other products such as telephones, alarm clocks, or alerting systems. The vibrating pads generally are powered by either 12 volts DC or 120 volts AC.

Silent Call provides wireless alerting systems which have a variety of signaling devices. Until recently, this was the only company that provided a comprehensive alerting system for people with hearing loss that specifically included transmitters for smoke alarms (as well as for CO alarms). The strobe lights for the Silent Call receivers are often placed on a table rather than on a wall. The Silent Call Sidekick Receiver has a built-in strobe with LED indicators to indicate the type of alert; it can also activate a bed vibrator. Currently these items emit the same the signal/vibration irrespective of the source of the alert.

The Krown KA300 System 11 is designed to work with smoke alarms and other audio alerts which have a compatible transmitter attached. It is supervised to a limited degree since it provides information about weak signals from transmitters, and provides information about which alert is transmitted. The Krown KA300RX Alarm Monitor Receiver has a strobe light which will not work in a power outage; there is also an audible alert but there is no information provided about the audible frequencies used in this alert. If the frequency is too high, there is essentially no effective alert provided by this system during a power outage when a deaf person is awake.

The Ameriphone AM6000 AlertMaster Wireless Notification System12 is not only used in residences but is also used in some hotel chains to provide notification for guests to a doorbell, telephone, and optionally room sounds [REF ]. The system utilizes a large multi-function receiver and comes with a bed shaker but no strobe, though it can flash an attached lamp. An optional sound monitor, the Audio Alarm Monitor or Audio Alarm Transmitter can be placed next to the smoke alarm. There appears to be no battery back-up or supervision.

The Sonic Alert13 system utilizes the AC wiring of the residence to transmit signals to plugged in receivers. It provides a bed shaker option, Super Shaker Bed Vibrator and a small strobe light,

10 http://www.clarityproducts.com ; http://www.clearsounds.com ; http://www.sonicalert.com/ ; http://www.silentcall.com/ 11 http://www.krownmfg.com 12 http://www.contactassist.com/amalam.html 13 http://www.sonicalert.com/

67

the Sonic Blink. It also has a remote horn, which can be adjusted between 200-2000 Hz and up to 78-85dB for low frequencies up to 113 decibels for high frequencies.

In addition, a number of experimental devices are being developed that provide local/mobile tactile information[348]; this might be particularly useful when the individual is in a public space. In these cases, the technology takes advantage of the existing wireless/communication systems in place. The cell phones or pager being worn by an individual is notified of the event. Additional information can also be passed on via the MSN/paging systems in place. Using this system it would not only be possible to alert an impaired person of an incident, but it would also be possible to provide information on the nature of the incident and the response required. This may have general value. Some of these take advantage of existing alarm/notification systems. For instance, the eOmega system uses the Simplex to initiate a paging program. Once the Simplex system is activated the eOmega is able to page pre-determined information to individuals distributed about a space via e-mail or onsite paging.14

6.1.4 Olfactory There is a small body of research that has investigated the use of odors to stimulate the olfactory system and wake individuals in emergency situations. Although anecdotal evidence suggests that the olfactory system is able to detect the smell of burning materials quite well when awake, the literature that is available on sleeping occupants provides consistent evidence that olfactory stimuli cannot reliably wake individuals [200,201,285]. In addition to a lack of research supporting the use of olfactory stimuli for fire alarm signaling, no technology has been developed for this purpose.

6.1.5 Visual High-intensity light sources, such as strobe lights, are used to provide emergency alarm signaling to the hearing impaired and are prevalent in public spaces. Strobes are required to be synchronized in situations where a person would otherwise be exposed to more than two flashes per second, preventing potential seizures.

The Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA) recommends the use of visual notification (strobes) for the sleeping areas of people with hearing impairments [338]. Requirements for this technology are outlined in UL 1971, Signaling Devices for the Hearing Impaired. Compared to the other technologies examined, smoke alarms capable of providing a visual alarm signal are relatively common. In fact, most major smoke alarm manufacturers offer smoke alarm models with strobe lights, including First Alert, Gentex Corporation, and Kidde. Strobe lights may either be integrated into the smoke alarm or may be an accessory that connects to the alarm. 15 The features available on smoke alarms with strobes are fairly consistent across manufacturers. These alarms generally feature a 177 candela strobe, in addition to a piezoelectric audible alarm yielding 85 – 90 dB at 10 feet. Most of the units are AC (hardwired) or AC with battery backup. Single-station units (i.e. not interconnected) are also available that are powered with an AC

14 http://www.flashpointsoftware.com/eomegamain.html 15 Smoke alarms that include strobes will have to be wired to 110AC. A 9V battery could not provide the power required of UL1971.

68

receptacle via a cord and plug. Alarms powered by battery only are not practical due to the typical power requirements of the strobes. More sophisticated visual notification systems may be available in public spaces. Ideally these would require the use of existing technologies (screens, LED displays [339], etc.) that were already present within a space and could then be used as part of the notification system. A variation of this approach is outlined [260] where existing video displays are used to notify people of an event. More sophisticated integrated systems can be employed to distribute information to a variety of handheld and desktop devices (including PC and radio) [339,340]

6.2 Effectiveness of Current Notification Technology The potential impact of these systems will be focused upon during this analysis; i.e. the impact of the technology rather that the many different technologies employed. The general approaches to notification currently available will be highlighted and, where possible, current examples of this technology cited. The technologies are grouped according to which of the senses it is aimed (e.g. hearing, vision, etc.) and the level of information provided (see Table 23). The levels of information provided are termed ‘recognition’ (i.e. the system alerts the population of an incident, denoted by ‘Rc’ in Table 24); and ‘response’ (i.e. the systems both alerts the population and has the potential for providing information in the response required, denoted by ‘Rp‘ in Table 24). The categorization therefore focuses upon the potential effects of the technology rather than the underlying technological causes. By adopting this approach we are able to reduce the number of technological categories enabling us to better understand the impact on the vulnerabilities identified. Without this, each of the many signals/messages would need to be examined; e.g. for tonal signals this would include tones, bells, chimes, whoops, T-3, modified T-3. The analysis produced would be prohibitively complex and would not provide additional insight.

Table 23: Categorization of Notification Technology. Sense Targeted Nature of Examples of approach information Visual Recognition strobes, flashing lights (Rc) Visual Response video screens, television monitors, computer screens (Rp) Audible Recognition tones, bells, chimes, whoops, T-3, modified T-3 (Rc) Audible Recognition Directional Noise (Rc/Rp)* Audible Response Voice systems (Rp) Tactile Recognition vibrating devices (pillow/bed shaker), moving air, electric shocks, etc (Rc) Olfactory Recognition Simulated smoke, pheromones (Rc) Tactile Response Pagers, cell phones (Rp) Staff Response (Rp) *Can aid in the location of an exit, but cannot provide further information.

69

When examining these technological devices it is apparent that although some of them may provide a significant benefit to one component of the notification process, they may provide no benefit with respect to other components. For instance, the T-3 signal may alert sleeping individuals more successfully than a continuous tone (component 1 of the notification process), but it does not provide any guidance on the response of the individual (component 4). An extensive examination has been conducted of the empirical data associated with each of these technologies [37,47,60,63,64,65,72-93,99,131,178-211,218-220,226,232-241,246-251,256,259,260,269,278- 280,298-336,346], allowing more informed judgments to be made. In order to better understand the potential impact of these different technologies, the notification technologies currently available are categorized according to which of the four components of the notification process that they addressed and the extent to which they do so (see Table 24). A simple scale is derived:

black - literature suggests that the technology can address this component of the notification process. grey - literature suggests that the technology has the potential to partially address this component. empty – the technology is not able to address this component.

This enabled the potential impact of the notification technologies to be better understood and some comparisons to be made between them. For example, a strobe system may be able to aid in the receipt of a signal; it may also help in the recognition that a fire incident is developing (see Table 24). It is not, however, able to help identify the required evacuation response or aid in this response. This should be compared with a member of staff who has the potential for helping in all of these four categories.

Table 24: Example of the categorization of various notification approaches. Rc indicates the ability to improve receipt and recognition; Rc(adv) Rp indicates the ability to improve Targeted Sense Audible Visual Tactile Olfactory Staff

Rc Rc/Rp Rp Rc Rp Rc Rp Rc Rp

Receipt Recog. 1

Resp. 2 Ident. Resp. Perf. 1 Several of the advanced tactile technologies were employed as part of everyday life and the individual may therefore be attuned to it activating. It could be argued that the signal would therefore not necessarily indicate an emergency but would attract the individual’s attention. 2 This is based on the assumption that several of the advanced tactile technologies encouraged the individual to look at a device that had the potential of providing more information.

As mentioned earlier in the report, the presence of highly trained, motivated, assertive, well- informed and numerous members of staff is considered to be the benchmark for the notification process. It is included in Table 24 to allow comparisons to be made. It is apparent that there is a

70

difference between those systems simply denoting that an event has occurred, and those that provide information regarding the incident itself. These latter systems have the potential for providing information relating to the response required of the population.

By examining Table 23 and Table 24 it is possible to identify those technologies that are able to influence both the recognition and the response of the population. The ability to provide information on the nature of the incident and on the desired response has general value; however, it has particular value where the situation precludes training or familiarization. For instance, where the population is transient. Where an individual is in a domestic situation, the provision of such information may be less significant given the relative simplicity of the structure, the simplicity of the desired response, and the familiarity of the individual with their surrounding.

This analysis can be cross-referenced with the vulnerabilities already identified. By doing so, the capacity of these notification technologies to address specific vulnerabilities can be ascertained. This is performed in the next section.

71

7 Stage I: Recommendations for Stage II Research Activities In Section 4 an extensive list of vulnerabilities was identified. This represented the vulnerabilities identified from our extensive literature review. However, these were not deemed to be equally significant or amenable to the application of notification technologies. This list was reduced by • the establishment of the nature of the vulnerabilities indicating whether they could be reduced through the use of notification technology; • the examination of the empirical data on the performance of the current notification technologies to establish the feasibility of their use and their potential impact on specific vulnerabilities; • the listing of the previous recommendations on further work. By understanding the nature of the vulnerabilities, the capacity of existing notification technologies and the data currently available several objectives could be met. Firstly, the vulnerabilities could be prioritized. Secondly, the technologies could be assessed and candidate technologies suggested for further investigation. Finally, research areas could be suggested based on the previous two objectives and the absence of sufficient existing data.

This has enabled the list to be reduced to a small sub-set of the vulnerabilities identified as requiring further empirical research (see Table 25). The five vulnerabilities are • the hearing impaired, • those asleep, • the alcohol impaired, • those in public spaces, large groups, untrained, unprimed and unfamiliar • those subject to background pollution.

Table 25: Summary of the key vulnerabilities identified. (1) Receipt of (2) Recognition of (3) Identification (4) Respond signal/message signal/ message of Response Hearing impaired STRONG slight Those asleep slight slight slight Alcohol impairment / Intoxication STRONG STRONG STRONG STRONG Background Pollution STRONG STRONG Public Spaces/Large Group/ slight STRONG STRONG slight Untrained/Unprimed/Unfamiliar Sleep Deprivation/Medication STRONG STRONG STRONG STRONG

The examination of the vulnerabilities and the notification systems can be integrated in order to establish which of the notification technologies addresses the vulnerabilities in question. A simple metric has been established to represent the potential of these approaches at reducing these vulnerabilities (see Table 26). The potential impact of the notification systems is represented using a three point scale: black indicating that either a significant benefit may be attained or that some benefit is likely and that information is currently lacking; grey indicating that some benefit may be gained; no color indicating that no benefit is likely. This represents a simplification, but enables some comparison to be made and highlights where the potential for addressing these vulnerabilities exist. Several technologies were excluded as part of this process:

72

Olfactory-based technologies – from the empirical evident these appeared to be unpromising and faced a number of technical difficulties, including ensuring that the ‘signal’ was received by the target population. Technologies involving moving air were excluded on similar grounds. The concept of electric shock notification was dismissed on ethical grounds. Many of the existing ‘one bit’ [117] signals are not recommended for further research based on our extensive understanding of their limitations.

Table 26: Assessment of the potential impact of notification technologies upon vulnerabilities. Audible Tac. Visual Sta. Rc Rc/Rp)Rp Rc Rc Rp Rp Sleep Deprivation/Medication Hearing impaired 1 3 1 Composite (Public Spaces etc.) Those asleep Alcohol impairment Background Pollution 2 2 2 2 2 1 Potential for this will be largely dependent upon the severity of the hearing disability 2 This will be dependent upon the nature of the pollution present. 3 Broad band nature of sound may have particular benefit to some of the hearing impaired population.

By adopting this vulnerability model, vulnerabilities have been identified in a manner that is independent of the process used to record the incident; i.e. that could take into account the body of learning in the field of human behavior in fire, rather than rely completely on high-level statistics. This has enabled vulnerabilities to be highlighted that might otherwise have been overlooked. It also allowed the nature of the vulnerabilities to be better understood and suggested a means of potentially addressing the vulnerability in question through the application and development of different notification technologies.

Some basic data collection scenarios were presented to the Technical Panel associated with this project. The vulnerabilities and technologies were compiled in order to clearly associate those technologies with the vulnerabilities that might be reduced and the experimental conditions deemed to be most appropriate. These are listed in Table 27.

As part of the Technical Panel’s analysis of the recommendations, an ad hoc method was developed to validate the findings and refine the recommendations still further according to the number of research projects that could be conducted (in this case three research projects were possible). This involved a scheme being devised where the panel members ranked the vulnerabilities identified (see Table 16). Each member of the technical panel was asked to identify those vulnerabilities that they considered should be further examined. The same process was performed regarding the notification technologies and the experimental scenarios. This was conducted independently of the analysis outlined during this report; the only common point of reference was the original set of vulnerabilities highlighted in Table 16 and the notification technologies identified in Table 23). Reassuringly, the final three vulnerabilities arrived at by the Technical Panel were a sub-set of those highlighted in Table 25 and the notification technologies approximated those identified in Table 27. The vulnerabilities identified by the Technical Panel

73

were those impaired by alcohol, those with varying degrees of hearing disability, and the general population in public spaces. It is apparent that these represent an innate vulnerability; a situational vulnerability; and a combination of vulnerabilities.

Table 27: Data collection scenarios recommended to the Technical Panel. Scenario Vulnerabilities Status Occupancy Technologies Tested Nature of Trial 1 Intoxication Asleep Domestic T-3 (variants) Contrived Vibrating Devices Voice Strobe 2 Sleep Derivation Asleep Domestic T-3 (variants) Contrived /Medication Vibrating Devices Voice Strobe 3 Hearing Impaired Asleep Domestic T-3 (variants) Contrived Vibrating Devices Voice Strobe 4 Background Awake Domestic/Public T-3 (variants) Contrived Pollution Vibrating Devices Voice Strobe

5 Composite (Public Awake Public T-3 (variants) Natural Setting Spaces, large Voice groups etc.) Strobe Directional Noise

Those subject to sleep deprivation or medication (Scenario 2) were excluded given the variation evident in these populations, and the ethical and technical difficulties in testing the impact of medication. It was suggested that the susceptibility to background pollution (Scenario 4) should not be examined in isolation given that (1) visual and audible pollution can be examined as part of Scenario 5; and (2) some aspects of it can also be examined as part of Scenario (3), with both populations potentially having limited abilities to receive audible signals.

It was determined that the first two populations identified required a contrived data collection process; i.e. that the effectiveness of the notification technologies examined needed to be isolated. It was also felt that these two populations were particularly vulnerable when asleep. It was suggested that these two groups were tested in a domestic environment. Again, this approximates a sub-set of the data collection scenarios identified in Table 27.

It was suggested that the voice notification technology should not be included when examining the domestic occupancies. This was due to the limited success of the voice technologies exhibited in previous empirical work [ref] and also represented an attempt to limit the number of factors involved. This voice notification technology was considered of interest when examining public spaces.

74

A range of notification technologies were deemed to be of interest. These were to be examined according to their effectiveness at addressing the three vulnerabilities identified.

Table 28: Revised data collection scenarios. Scenario Vulnerabilities Status Occupancy Technologies Tested Nature of Trial A Intoxication Asleep Domestic T-3 (variants) Contrived Vibrating Devices Strobe B Hearing Impaired Asleep Domestic T-3 (variants) Contrived Vibrating Devices Strobe C Composite (Public Awake Public Existing System Natural Setting Spaces, large implemented groups etc.) T-3 Voice Strobe Directional (if feasible)

The data collection projects have been completed. In the next sections, the results produced by these three Stage II data collection activities are presented, along with a risk assessment of the three vulnerabilities examined. It represents, with only minor modifications, the work of the original authors. It was left in this form in order to reduce the probability of the original authors’ findings being misrepresented.

75

8 Stage II Research Activity - Risk Assessment In an effort to quantify the possible impact of addressing the three vulnerabilities identified, a risk assessment has been performed. This assessment was conducted in a similar manner to that conducted for the analysis of the vulnerability elderly population [215]. However, the current vulnerabilities were identified through an analytical rather than a statistical process; they were not reliant upon statistical support. There is the possibility that statistical support for this work is unavailable; i.e. that given some of the discrepancies in the recording and storing of vulnerabilities, that it is not possible to clearly establishing the outcome of addressing them. This assessment has therefore been conducted given that some vulnerabilities are less likely than others to appear in the existing statistics.

This section of work was provided by John Hall of the National Fire Protection Association [341]. The text presented here represents a slightly reduced version of the original text. Otherwise, this work was entirely produced by Dr. Hall.

Fire death risk related to alcohol Fire death risk related to alcohol cannot be isolated with as much confidence as can fire death risk related to age. There are several reasons for this:

• Prior to 1999, alcohol impairment was combined with impairment by other (legal or illegal) drugs in incident reporting, and the combined condition was one of several choices on the scale of “condition before ignition,” for which only one condition could be entered. Other choices included physical or mental disability and being asleep. In 1994- 1998, 9% of home fire death victims were reported as impaired by alcohol or other drugs. In 1999-2002, when alcohol could be coded separately and multiple conditions could be recorded under “human factor contributing to injury,” impairment by alcohol was reported for 10% of home fire death victims.

• Special studies of blood alcohol levels have indicated that alcohol impairment is substantially under-reported on fire incident reports. Studies from Maryland and Minnesota show elevated blood alcohol levels for roughly half of all adult home fire death victims or roughly one-third of total home fire death victims [3]. This is more than three times the percentage of victims shown as impaired on fire incident reports.

• There is no good data available to estimate the fraction of people-hours of exposure by people impaired by alcohol. There are estimates of what fraction of the population ever exceed a certain defined level of alcohol consumption in a defined period of time, but impairment can occur at lower levels and there is no way to estimate how often or for how long the individuals are impaired.

Fire deaths for alcohol-impaired persons by activity when injured

Table 29 shows percents of fire deaths for 1980-1998 fatal victims who were impaired by alcohol or other drugs, by four major activity when injured groups – sleeping, attempting to escape, attempting rescue or fire control, and unable to act or acting irrationally. (Occupants who were

76

sleeping when injured are also identified under condition before injury, but some sleeping occupants may be hidden under other condition codes, such as impaired by drugs or alcohol, too old, too young, or handicapped.)

Other than escaping, the other activities would indefinitely prolong the person’s presence in the fire area and so would be expected to involve some significant risk of injury.

Table 29: Home Fire Deaths for Victims Impaired by Alcohol or Other Drugs, by Activity When Injured, 1980-1998

Sleeping 41% Attempting to escape 21% Attempting rescue or fire control 5% Unable to act or acting irrationally 33%

Notes: These are national estimates of fires reported to U.S. municipal fire departments and so exclude fires reported only to Federal or state agencies or industrial fire brigades. National estimates are projections. Casualty projections can be heavily influenced by the inclusion or exclusion of one unusually serious fire. Home structure fires include one- and two-family dwellings, apartments, townhouses, flats, and manufactured homes, but exclude dormitories, barracks, hotels and motels, boarding or rooming houses, and assisted living facilities.

Source: NFIRS and NFPA survey.

Effects of improved waking effectiveness on fire risk Successful waking of sleeping occupants is not enough to assure safe escape. Some of the wakened occupants would be expected to die. There will be unsuccessful escape attempts, where the early warning from smoke alarms could not compensate in all cases for either limited time for escape or insufficient training and knowledge of effective escape routes and procedures. And the several activities that involve indefinitely extended time in hazardous conditions would be expected to lead to deaths. And some of the victims listed as sleeping when injured may have been awakened but returned to sleep. Greater waking effectiveness might make no difference to these victims either.

On the other hand, changes that lead to successful waking of more occupants could also result in earlier alerting of occupants who were already awake. Earlier alerting could lead to more success in chosen activities and fewer deaths.

Also, the changes might not be sufficient to produce 100% waking effectiveness. For example, the changes might not address the reliability of operation of the home smoke alarm, currently estimated at 80% (based on a CPSC special study). Smoke alarms that do not activate cannot wake anyone.

Setting up the analysis

77

This analysis will concern itself only with estimating the reduction in fire death rate if changes are made that are effective in waking all occupants for a given population. Hence, the percentage of fatal victims who were asleep when fatally injured are the focus of this analysis.

It is assumed that sleeping occupants, if awakened, will select activities in the same proportions as did occupants who were not asleep when fire began and will experience the same risks associated with those activities as did the occupants who were not asleep. For example, a newly wakened occupant is assumed to be just as likely to decide to fight the fire as an occupant who was never asleep and just as likely to be fatally injured while doing so.

It is further assumed that a person newly awakened at one time of day will select activities and encounter risks associated with those activities in the same way as a person newly awakened at another time of day. For example, although an older adult is more likely to be asleep at 3 am than at 3 pm, once awakened, that person is as likely to choose escape verses firefighting at 3 am as at 3 pm, and the risk of fatal injury for each activity chosen will be the same at 3 am as at 3 pm.

The basic risk model Equation (1) is a probabilistic model of the probability of dying in a fire (called ptotal), distinguishing sleeping verses all other activities. All probabilities are probabilities per year. Therefore, the best data-based estimate of ptotal is the annual fire death rate (deaths per million persons, which is a dimensionless value suitable for use as a probability), for a specified population (e.g., impaired people).

Because we have no good estimate of the total impaired population, we must use the annual fire death toll instead.

(1) pasleep pdeath,asleep + pnot pdeath,not = ptotal , where:

pasleep = prob (asleep given impaired) = [prob (asleep and impaired)]/[prob (impaired)]

pnot = prob (not asleep given age range) = [prob (not asleep and impaired)]/[prob (impaired)] = 1- pasleep

pdeath,asleep = prob (death given impaired and asleep) = [prob (death, impaired, and asleep)]/[prob (impaired and asleep)]

pdeath,not = prob (death given impaired and not asleep) = [prob (death, impaired, and not asleep)]/[prob (impaired and not asleep)]

ptotal = prob (death given impaired) = [prob (death and impaired)]/[prob (impaired)]

The reader is referred elsewhere for further details of this approach [215].

78

In equation (1), as noted, it is possible to use available data directly to estimate the right-side term (ptotal is estimated as the annual fire death rate). It is not possible to directly estimate any of the other four terms from available data. Equation (2) introduces two new terms that can be estimated from available data:

(2) qasleep + qnot = 1, where:

qasleep = asleep share of impaired deaths

qnot = not-asleep share of impaired deaths

By dividing both sides of equation (1) by ptotal and comparing the result to equation (2), it may be seen that:

(2A) qasleep = pasleep pdeath,asleep / ptotal

(2B) qnot = pnot pdeath,not / ptotal

Solving the model for reduction in risk through waking all impaired persons

Dividing equation (1) through by ptotal and substituting qasleep = pasleep pdeath,asleep / ptotal yields:

(3) qasleep + (1- pasleep) (pdeath,not / ptotal) = 1

If everyone has been wakened, then every occupant will face a probability of death equal to pdeath,not. Therefore, the reduction in risk will be equal to 1 - pdeath,not / ptotal. Solve equation (3) for the ratio:

(4) pdeath,not/ptotal = [1- qasleep] / [1 - pasleep] = [1- qasleep] / [1 - qasleep (ptotal / pdeath,asleep)]

Equation (4) shows that the ratio of the new death probability (with complete waking effectiveness) to the old death probability is a function of three terms, two of which can be estimated from available data. The term that cannot be so estimated is pdeath,asleep. It is easier to work in terms of the unknown ratio, i.e., ptotal / pdeath,asleep; call that ratio 1/A. The sensitivity of the estimated risk reduction to A is shown in Table 30. Table 30: Sensitivity of ratio to A.

If qasleep (sleeping share of fire deaths) = X Then % risk reduction in And A = total fire deaths = and pdeath,asleep / pdeath,not = X(A-1)/(A-X) 1 0% 1 2 X/(2-X) (2-X)/(1-X) 3 2X/(3-X) (3-X)/(1-X) 5 4X/(5-X) (5-X)/(1-X)

79

10 9X/(10-X) (10-X)/(1-X) 50 49X/(50-X) or roughly X (50-X)/(1-X)

The reader is referred elsewhere [215] for the derivation of the relationship between pdeath,asleep / pdeath,not, A, and the total risk reduction.

Estimating A

It is possible to estimate the critical ratio of A = 1/[ptotal / pdeath,asleep].

Start with this equation: (5) pasleep,h + pnot,h = 1.

The subscript h refers to hour h, one of the 24 hour segments in the day.

Now rearrange terms in equations (2A) and (2B) to derive values for the two terms in equation (5), then rearrange terms again: (6) qasleep,h (1 / pdeath,asleep ) + qnot,h (1 / pdeath,not ) = 1 / ptotal,h

By the assumptions stated at the beginning, pdeath,asleep and pdeath,not will not change from hour to hour, but qasleep and ptotal may change, as will qnot.

The derived coefficients should be positive values. When they are estimated as negative, this means that the higher the estimated death rate is (and the closer the inverse of the death rate is to zero), the better the estimating equations fit. Fire statistics can be used to calculate, for each hour segment, values of qasleep,h, qnot,h and ptotal,h. One can use least-squares estimation (same format as linear regression) to estimate pdeath,asleep and pdeath,not.

In the case of impaired victims, the estimate of 1/pdeath,asleep is negative, which means the estimate of the probability of death when an impaired victim is asleep is effectively certain, and that means that waking all sleeping impaired victims is estimated to save all those people. The sleeping fraction of impaired fatal home fire victims was previously estimated as 41% of the 10% of home fire victims who are reported to be impaired.

This estimate is highly uncertain. As noted earlier, there is considerable evidence that the impaired fraction of fire victims is severely under-reported. The baseline of risk might be one- third of victims rather than the 10% reported. Also, our statistics are for the combination of impairment by alcohol or other drugs. However, the post-1999 statistics suggest that alcohol strongly dominates this combination and may even be present in many cases where other drugs are present.

Fire death risk related to hearing impairment

The U.S. fire incident data base does not distinguish specific types of disabilities, and so there are no available fire statistics specifically on the hearing-impaired. The most reasonable approach is to work with the available data on the size and characteristics of the hearing-impaired population.

80

Gallaudet Research Institute’s website offers a number of useful estimates (see http://gri.gallaudet.edu/Demographics/deaf-US.php, accessed on February 14, 2007), all based on self-reporting (see Table 31).

Table 31: Levels of deafness and hearing impairment according to age. Combined deaf and Combined deaf and hard of hearing any hearing trouble Age group Deaf 2002 Deaf 1997-2003 2002 1997-2003

Under 5 N.A. 0.1% N.A. 1.8% 5 to 17 0.1% 0.1% 0.6% 3.8% 18 to 44 0.2% 0.1% 1.2% 7.9% 45 to 64 0.3% 0.2% 4.2% 19.0% 65 or older 1.5% 1.0% 16.9% 38.9%

These statistics can be combined with statistics on the age distribution of the population or on the age distribution of fatal victims of home fires (see Table 32).

Table 32: Fatalities according to levels of deafness/hearing impairment and age. Population 2000 Home fire deaths Population (midpoint of Home fire deaths 2000 (midpoint Age group 2002 1997-2003) 2002 of 1997-2003)

Under 5 6.8% 6.8% 11% 13% 5 to 17 18.5% 18.9% 11% 14% 18 to 44 39.2% 39.9% 27% 28% 45 to 64 23.1% 22.0% 25% 20% 65 or older 12.4% 12.4% 26% 26%

By combining these statistics, one can first calculate, as GRI does in the source document that self-reported deaf individuals constitute an estimated 0.2% to 0.4% of the population (with 0.2% being the calculation based on 2000 population and 0.4% being the calculation based on 2002 population). Individuals who self-report being hard of hearing constitute an estimated 3.7% of the 2002 population, and individuals who self-report having hearing troubles (from a little to a lot to full deafness) constitute an estimated 13.0% of the 2000 population.

It may also be calculated that people aged 65 or older constitute a majority of the 2002 deaf population (52%), the 2000 deaf population (53%), and the 2002 hard-of-hearing population (57%), as well as more than a third of the 2000 population with hearing troubles (37%).

If hearing-impaired individuals have no higher risk of fire death than their non-hearing-impaired same-age counterparts, then the hearing-impaired share of home fire deaths would still be larger than the hearing-impaired share of the population, because so much of the hearing-impaired population are also older adults, who have a much higher risk of fire death, presumably because of the whole range of reduced capabilities associated with higher age. This assumption of equal

81

age-adjusted risk of fire death for hearing-impaired individuals would yield estimates that the deaf population account for 0.4% of the 2000 home fire deaths and 0.5% of the 2002 home fire deaths, that the hard-of-hearing population account for 5.9% of 2002 home fire deaths (compared to 3.7% of the 2002 population), and that the population with hearing troubles account for 16.9% of 2000 home fire deaths (compared to 13.0% of the 2000 population).

If, as one might expect, the deaf population actually has much higher fire death risk than their same-age counterparts, the deaf share of home fire deaths would still be well below 5% of total fire deaths.

The older-adult share of hearing-impaired home fire deaths is the same whether hearing-impaired home fire death risk is the same as their same-age counterparts or is higher, provided that the magnitude of the difference in risk between hearing-impaired people and non-hearing-impaired people does not itself change with age. Older adults therefore account for three-fourths of deaf home fire deaths, in 2002 (72%) and 2000 (73%), and of hard-of-hearing home fire deaths (74%), as well as 60% of home fire deaths among people with any degree of hearing troubles.

Therefore, it appears clear that most hearing-impaired home fire death victims are also older adults (age 65 or older), which means they are likely to have additional limitations beyond their hearing impairments. This should be factored into estimates of the impact of improving the waking effectiveness of home smoke alarms for the hearing-impaired population.

Fire death risk related to large commercial buildings

The term “commercial buildings” is not used in defining and grouping property uses in the fire incident coding manuals in the U.S. However, we can work toward the essence of the question by examining the various categories of buildings that could qualify.

In 1999-2002 the U.S. averaged 20 fire deaths a year in all public assembly, mercantile and office properties (excluding the unique event of the attacks of 9/11/2001). These can be divided into five groups of roughly equal size:

• Eating and drinking establishments. Surveys by the U.S. Energy Information Administration show that more than two-thirds of buildings devoted to food service are less than 5,000 square feet in size and less than 10% are more than 10,000 square feet in size. Hence, very few are what one might call large buildings. • Other public assembly properties, primarily religious facilities and clubs. The EIA survey shows that nearly two-thirds of religious facilities are less than 10,000 square feet in size and roughly two-thirds of other public assembly properties (i.e., excluding food service and religious facilities) are less than 10,000 square feet. Hence, most are not what one might call large buildings. • Gasoline service stations and other retail outlets that are inherently incapable of holding a large population. • Other retail outlets. The EIA surveys show that food sales facilities plus other mercantile facilities excluding malls have a majority of their buildings under 5,000 square feet in

82

size and more than four-fifths under 10,000 square feet in size. Mall buildings tend to be much larger, but the individual stores are not and it is not easy to estimate what fraction of fire deaths in stores occur in malls. • Offices. The EIA surveys show about three-fourths of office buildings are under 10,000 square feet in size.

Put this all together and probably one-fifth of the buildings in these categories are at least 10,000 square feet in size. Of course, these buildings account for a much larger share of total building floor space and so may account for a larger share of fires and fire deaths. But it is hard to imagine that more than half of the 20 fire deaths a year in these properties occur in large buildings, and we do know that nearly all these fire deaths are in fires where exactly one person dies. Incidents in which multiple fire deaths occur in the same fire are rare, and even fewer are those in which deaths occur during a flawed or delayed evacuation process.

Accordingly, one cannot make a persuasive case that better detection and alarm in large commercial buildings will produce significant life savings, because there are so few lives being lost. If a case is to be made for better alerting effectiveness or better detection and alarm performance generally, it has to be made on other grounds, which might include the very few but still important large incidents with large losses of life or might include the value of positive assurance of safety and/or heightened feelings of safety that might be obtained with better detection and alarm performance.

As a result of the recommendations produced earlier in this report, three separate research projects have been conducted. These examined the effectiveness of notification technologies at addressing vulnerabilities in the alcohol impaired; the hearing disabled; and people in public spaces. A summary of the results produced from these projects are presented in the next sections. The reader is referred to the full reports of these projects for further details [117,342,343].

83

9 Stage II Research Activity - People who are Alcohol Impaired This is the executive summary produced by Dr. D. Bruck, Ms. M. Ball and Dr. I. Thomas [342]. This summary represents the work performed by these researchers as part of the project funded by the Research Foundation. The reader is referred to their original report for a more detailed assessment of this work [342].

Studies of fire fatalities in the US, UK and Australia across young and middle aged adult groups (e.g. 18 to 65 year olds) have consistently shown that alcohol impairment is a key factor in over half of the fire fatalities. One recent study [176], using a small sample of young adults, found that the ability to awaken to an auditory alarm was significantly reduced by alcohol intoxication. Furthermore the data suggested that the current high pitched alarm was not as effective at waking this population under sober or alcohol impaired conditions as the alternative auditory signals tested. Other studies, using children and older adults (sober) have also found that the high pitched alarm was not as effective at waking up occupants. In all cases to date an auditory signal called the “mixed T-3” has been found to be the most effective auditory signal for arousing sleepers across a range of age groups. This mixed signal is a square wave with a fundamental frequency of 520 Hz,16 presented in a Temporal 3 (T-3) pattern.

In the light of these findings it became important to investigate responsiveness to a range of alternative auditory signals, including a 520 Hz square wave, in a larger sample of sleeping adults who were impaired by alcohol. A further question of interest was whether non-auditory signals, such as strobe lights and bed shakers, would actually be more effective at awakening sleepers under the influence of alcohol than auditory signals. This study addresses these issues.

Thirty two young adults aged 18 to 26 were each exposed to a range of signals across two nights during deep sleep (stage 4). Prior to sleep all had consumed alcohol such that their blood alcohol concentration (BAC) was measured at .05. Seven signals were tested: • 400 Hz square wave signal in T-3 pulse • 520 Hz square wave signal in T-3 pulse • 500 Hz pure tone in T-3 pulse • 3100 Hz pure tone in T-3 pulse • Bed shaker- under mattress – T-3 pulse • Pillow shaker – T-3 pulse • Strobe light - T-3 pulse (modified)

Each signal was presented for 30 seconds, followed by a short period without a signal (30 -70 seconds). After this pause the signal was presented at a higher intensity level and this continued until the maximum intensity was reached, or the participant awoke.

The main conclusions from this study are:

16 Square waves have, in addition to their fundamental frequency, additional peaks at the 3rd, 5th, 7th etc harmonics.

84

1. Some auditory signals are an effective means of waking moderately alcohol impaired (.05 BAC) young adults from deep sleep. Two signals, the 400 Hz and 520 Hz square wave T-3 sounds, were significantly more effective than the 3100 Hz pure tone T-3 sound.

2. Under the testing conditions a sound level of 75 dBA at the pillow was sufficient to awaken this population using either of the two square wave sounds. These signals awoke 93-100% of participants at 75 dBA or less. In contrast, the 3100 Hz signal awoke only 61.5% of the moderately alcohol impaired participants.

3. The bed shaker and pillow shaker devices tested were not an effective means of waking moderately alcohol impaired young adults from deep sleep, either at the intensity level as purchased, or at higher intensity levels. Only 58%-64.5% awoke at the intensity level as purchased.

4. Strobe lights were not an effective means of waking this population, with only 24% waking to the lowest strobe light intensity, which was more intense than that required by the standard [344].

5. The results in this study are likely to be overestimations of the proportion of moderately alcohol impaired young adults who may awaken to these signals in an unprimed, unscreened population, especially from deep sleep. Thus extrapolations of absolute intensities and percentages awoken in the study to the field should be made with caution.

6. It was found that, where a signal was presented at a level that caused awakening, most people awoke to the signal within the first 10 seconds of the signal being on. Thus it seems highly probable that a signal that is alternatively on and off for this period of time will be more effective than a continuously sounding signal.

Recommendations:

1. That bed shakers, pillow shakers or strobe lights, presented alone, should not be considered as an alternative emergency alarm for people with normal hearing. This recommendation is made on the basis that across the population a proportion of people sleep after having consumed alcohol, and visual and tactile signals will be less likely to awaken them than auditory signals. Furthermore, it is possible that the findings with alcohol can be generalised to other types of sedating chemicals (e.g. hypnotic medication).

2. That further research be conducted to determine the nature of the best auditory signal (both in terms of spectral characteristics and on/off timing) to replace the current high pitch alarm. A square wave sound with a fundamental frequency in the lower ranges (i.e. 520 Hz) has now been consistently documented to be more effective than the current high pitched smoke alarm signal across a range of populations (children, older adults, sober young adults, alcohol impaired young

85

adults and adults who are hard of hearing17). A signal that has an ON-OFF sound pattern of about 10-15 seconds may be more effective than a continuous signal.

17 Readers may find the companion report of interest [343].

86

10 Stage II Research Activity: People who are Hard of Hearing This is the executive summary produced by Dr. D. Bruck and Dr. I. Thomas [343]. This summary represents the work performed by these researchers as part of the project funded by the Research Foundation. The reader is referred to their original report for a more detailed assessment of this work [343].

As it has been shown that smoke alarms save lives (e.g. [2]) and that being asleep is a strong risk factor for fire fatality (e.g. [15,275]), the ability of different sections of the population to wake to their smoke alarm is an issue of importance.

Among the people at risk of not waking to an auditory smoke alarm are the more than 34.5 million people in the US who are hard of hearing. In some cases such people purchase alternative alerting devices which may send a visual signal (e.g. a strobe light) or a tactile signal which vibrates. Bed shakers and pillow shakers have become available for people with hearing impairment and may be used with an alarm clock or for emergency notification. Some specialist alerting devices allow the option of a loud low frequency sound.

Standards exist for the intensity of a strobe light for emergency notification [344], with an intensity of 177 or 110 candela (cd) specified, depending on placement. However, several studies of their efficacy in alerting sleepers suggest that only about a third of hard of hearing people or people with normal hearing will awaken to strobe lights of similar intensities [183,184,210].

For bed shakers UL 1971 (1991) provides a standard but the specifications relate to a shaker of different shape to most shakers currently sold. The bed shakers tested in sleep studies report waking rates of 70-100% when using shakers at “off the shelf” intensities [210,243,280]. The British Standard, BS 5446-3 (2005) relates to a smoke alarm “kit” for people with hearing impairment which combines the normal UK smoke alarm with a vibration pad (a bed shaker) and a flashing light. The minimum intensity specified for the flashing light is quite low (15 cd). For the bed shaker frequency ranges, pulse patterns and displacement specifications are provided, along with a standardized testing procedure. However, there are no published sleep studies which test the specified kit or report the intensity of the bed shakers as compared to the BS5446- 3. This study set out to test the waking effectiveness of several different auditory signals, a bed shaker, a pillow shaker and a strobe light in a sample of hard of hearing people. A range of different intensity levels were tested for each signal. Each device was tested separately.

Participants were 38 volunteers aged 18-77 years (16 males, 22 females) with an average hearing loss of 25-70 dB in both ears (i.e. mild to moderately severe hearing loss). No deaf individuals participated. Each participant was exposed to a range of signals across two non-consecutive nights during slow wave sleep (the deeper part of sleep, stages 3 and 4). Six signals were tested: • 400 Hz square wave signal in T-3 pulse

87

• 520 Hz square wave signal in T-3 pulse18 • 3100 Hz pure tone in T-3 pulse (the current smoke alarm) • Bed shaker (under mattress) in T-3 pulse • Pillow shaker in T-3 pulse • Strobe light in T-3 pulse (modified)

The first two signals used in the sleep study (as listed above) were chosen from a larger set of audible sounds that were used in the awake portion of the study. These first two audible signals had the lowest average hearing threshold from a set of eight different auditory signals. Each signal was presented for 30 seconds, followed by a short period without a signal (30 -70 seconds). After this pause the signal was presented at a higher intensity level and this continued until a range of intensities had been presented, or the participant awoke.

In addition, a questionnaire was administered to all volunteers for the study (n=44). This asked a variety of questions about their hearing, use of specialized alerting devices and their confidence in their ability to hear different alerting sounds in their home (e.g. telephone, fire alarm) both during the day and night.

The main conclusions from this study are:

1. Under the testing conditions a 520 Hz square wave T-3 sound was the single most effective signal, awakening 92% of hard of hearing participants when presented at or below 75 dBA for 30 seconds and awakening 100% at 95 dBA. Both the 520 Hz square wave and the 400 Hz square wave were significantly more effective than the 3100 Hz pure tone T-3 sound, which awoke 56% at or below 75 dBA. In addition the 520 Hz square wave signal yielded the lowest hearing threshold when awake for this sample of people who were hard of hearing, from a set of eight alternative sounds with a range of pitch and patterns. 19

2. Under the testing conditions the bed shaker and pillow shaker devices, presented alone, awoke 80-83% of the hard of hearing participants at the intensity level as purchased (vibrating in intermittent pulses).

3. Those hard of hearing participants who were aged 60 years or more were less likely to awaken to the bed shaker than those aged below 60 years. No age group differences were found for any other signal.

18 This signal is that same as the one referred to in previous studies as the “mixed T-3” [176,177,193,194]. Square waves have, in addition to their fundamental frequency, additional peaks at the 3rd, 5th, 7th etc harmonics. 19 The efficacy of the 520 Hz square wave signal in arousing sleepers has now been demonstrated in children, sober young adults, alcohol intoxicated young adults (two studies), older adults and hard of hearing people. Furthermore, in all these studies the high pitched alarm has been found to be the least effective of the auditory alternatives tested, for waking people up.

88

4. Strobe lights, presented alone, were not an effective means of waking this population, with only 27% waking to the lowest strobe light intensity, which was more intense than that required by the standard [344].20

5. There was tentative evidence that people may respond differently to different types of signals, suggesting that a bedroom alarm “kit” that combined two types of sensory signals (i.e. an auditory signal plus a tactile signal) may be more effective than one signal.

6. The results in this study are likely to be overestimations of the proportion of the hard of hearing population who may awaken to these signals in an unprimed, unscreened population, especially from deep sleep. Thus extrapolations of absolute intensities and percentages awoken in the study to the field should be made with caution.

7. It was found that, when a signal was presented at a level that caused awakening, most people awoke to the signal within the first 10 seconds of the signal being on. Thus it seems highly probable that a T-3 signal that is alternatively ON for about 10-15 seconds and OFF for a certain period of time (possibly of the same duration) will be more effective than a continuous sounding T-3 signal.

8. Questionnaire responses indicated a high level of misplaced complacency among people who are hard of hearing in terms of their need for specialist alerting devices. In view of this, and the fact that many people are not aware of their hearing loss, it is desirable that any standard audible smoke alarm for the general population emit a signal that maximises the chances of awakening for hard of hearing people (provided such a signal presents no increased risk to other sections of the population).

Recommendations:

1. The technical feasibility of replacing the current high frequency smoke alarm T-3 signal with a low frequency square wave T-3 signal (with a fundamental frequency of 520 Hz or thereabouts21) for the entire population should be investigated as a matter of priority.

2. A suitable ON duration of such a T-3 signal appears likely to be in the range of 10-15 seconds, with the OFF duration tentatively suggested to be of similar duration but further research is required to determine this.

3. For this population of people with mild to moderately severe hearing loss the single best emergency alerting device is a low frequency square wave auditory signal and this is superior to bed shakers, pillow shakers and strobe lights, presented alone. Ideally this square wave signal should be as loud as possible. There is tentative evidence that

20 This finding is consistent with the other two studies that have controlled for stage of sleep and tested the effectiveness of strobe lights in hard of hearing or normal hearing samples. 21 We are currently undertaking a study with unimpaired young adults which will compare arousal to a 520 Hz square wave with other square wave signals with fundamental frequencies between 520 Hz and 2000 Hz.

89

combining a low frequency square wave with a tactile device may provide additional waking effectiveness.

4. Any recommendations for the use of strobe lights, presented alone, as an emergency alarm to awaken sleepers who are hard of hearing or of normal hearing should be withdrawn as soon as possible.22

5. Further study should be undertaken with people with hearing loss ranging from moderate to profound (i.e. including deaf people) to determine the best signals, or combination of signals, that will reliably awaken this population from deep sleep. This should include bed shakers, pillow shakers, low frequency square waves (beneficial for those with residual hearing) and could include strobe lights. In such research it would also be of interest to test bed shakers (vibrating in intermittent pulses) in an under-the- pillow placement.

6. Research on the efficacy of a range of different signals and signal combinations in different populations (e.g. with and without hearing loss) should also be conducted in a large number of home environments where the participants were not primed to expect a signal during the night and unscreened for factors such as medication or prior alcohol consumption.

7. There should be further investigation of an appropriate means of standardising the measurement of the intensity of bed and pillow shakers and this should inform a new standard.

22 This recommendation should not be misinterpreted to apply to people who are awake or to deaf people. Neither of these conditions were tested in this study or the companion report on the alcohol impaired ([342].

90

11 Stage II: Research Activity - Large Groups in Public Spaces This is the executive summary extracted from [117], originally authored by Gwynne.

This study was designed to investigate the effectiveness of several notification systems at expediting an appropriate response to a fire in non-domestic occupancies; e.g. public spaces, commercial spaces, places of work, etc. It is these spaces that may require the movement of a large number of people that have different levels of familiarity, abilities, information and motivation. This project focused on large unassociated groups; i.e. groups not just based around familial or social groups. The effectiveness of these notification systems is established by observing a variety of different egress trials and a smaller number of contrived situations. A number of case studies are conducted, each involving the observation of an evacuation. These observations are made in order to assess the time taken for the population to respond to the notification system in place and their subsequent behavior. Table 33: Data Collected during this activity. Primary Trials Notification System Nature of the Drill Pre-Evac. Pre-Evac Data Points Times (ec) 1 – Office Voice / Staff Partial (specific area) 72 141.3 /Unann. [40-426] 2 – Library/Admin (UK) Tone /Staff Full/Unann. 153 102 [5-290] 3 – Office T-3/Staff Full/Unann. 348 101.4 [19-269] 4 – Library/Admin (UK) Voice Partial (specific area) 15 52.2 /Unann. [20-89] 5 – Library/Admin T-3 Partial (specific area) 10 N/A* (UK) /Unann. Secondary Trial Notification System Nature of the Drill Pre-Evac. Data Points 6-7 – University T-3 Full/Unan. 73 48.9 Residences [9-104] 8 Hospital Tone/Voice Partial (Staff only) n/a N/A* /Unann. 9 Hotel Tone Partial (Staff only, specific 17 61.3 area)/Unann. [20-141] 10-14 Distribution Mixed – Primarily Tone Full/Unann. n/a 76.4* Depots [60-107] 15 Library Facilities Staff Full/Unann. 18 38.7 (Brazil) [8-70] *Overall evacuation times for five different buildings

There are many circumstances in which the occupant population would be reluctant to evacuate without the strongest indication that the incident was real; e.g. the presence of smoke. Given the potential for disruption, it would have been difficult to arrange trials to examine this population. Given that the most engaged/committed populations have therefore not been included in this project, the results produced here exclude some of the most challenging situations for staff and notification systems to overcome. The data-sets collected during this project form a body of data that can be interrogated and from which some general conclusions can be drawn. It is not possible to draw definitive conclusions from any one trial, given the limitations highlighted; indeed it is questionable whether any of the conclusions drawn are definitive. However, by cross-referencing data from trials and making comparisons between them, it is possible to increase confidence in the findings and to generalize from specific cases.

The importance of ‘information’ was apparent throughout the trials. In this context, information refers to knowledge of the existence of an incident, the threat posed by it, and the required response. During the trials this information was provided in several ways: through training and practice drills, prior to the event; through a notification system during the event; and/or through members of staff during the event. Irrespective of the method used, a minimum level of information was required to enable the population to respond and to respond appropriately. Ideally, all of the described methods would be available. None of the trials examined employed all of these methods to the required degree.

91

Given that there are three basic methods for reaching the required information levels, it is important to understand where these methods are available; i.e. in which occupancies they can be employed. A simple approach was developed to clarify where these methods are available. Instead of the general characteristics of the occupancies (e.g. occupancy levels, use, etc.), occupancies were characterized according to the manner in which information can be provided.

Occupancies labeled ALL allow all three methods to be employed; for instance, large office environments, institutions, etc. Here the population is sufficiently static to allow long-term familiarity to be developed, while the organization is large and formal enough to allow for the development of an emergency procedure, the presence of active staff and the potential for the inclusion of a voice notification system. In occupancies labeled INC, such as commercial public spaces, transport terminals, hotels, etc., the transient population would be unable to take part in drills or other exercises to increase their familiarity with the notification system or procedure in place. However, the situation is sufficiently formal to allow for an emergency procedure to be implemented, allow for active staff, and potentially allow for the introduction of a voice notification system. In occupancies labeled PRE-INC, such as domestic residencies (e.g. based around a single social or familial unit), the situation is relatively informal and small- scale. It therefore does not warrant an emergency procedure (designed by experts), active staff, and would probably not allow for the introduction of a voice notification system, given the costs involved; i.e. there would be the potential for pre-incident preparation but not for providing information during the incident.

These three types of occupancy are shown in Table 34. The value of this categorization is that it demonstrates the potential for providing information for each occupancy type. During this project occupancies labeled ALL and INC were examined. The occupancies labeled ALL were primarily examined to learn about the limitations of the methods available. By manipulating the three key variables, we were also able to appreciate the impact of notification systems in Type A and extrapolate their impact on INC occupancies. This was made possible by the analysis conducted: breaking down the notification into its constituent parts and ensuring a detailed understanding of the background conditions. Table 34: Information provisions available to each type of occupancy. Black indicates that the method is available; grey indicates a limited version of this method is available. Type ALL INC PRE-INC Potential Provision of Prep Prep Prep Information Staff Staff Staff N.S. N.S. N.S.

This scheme also demonstrated how spaces can be defined according to the potential for providing information, rather than according to their usage and occupancy levels. This is instructive when attempting to ascertain the appropriateness of notification systems.

The data collected related both to a quantitative understanding of the performance during each trial, along with qualitative information providing some explanation of the events. This allowed recommendations to be made regarding the effectiveness of the notification systems examined when applied to non-domestic spaces, including public spaces and places of work. The key findings and recommendations are shown below:

A wide range of techniques are employed to conduct and monitor practice egress trials. The seriousness afforded to these events and their authenticity varies between structures, organizations and staff. The performance of practice fire drills is an important requirement for the assessment of emergency procedures and the familiarization of the resident population. If these drills are meant to familiarize the population with the procedures in place, then they need to be authentic and unannounced. If these drills are really meant to assess the procedure employed in a structure, then they need to be independently monitored and recorded to ensure that they are conducted appropriately. Guidance should be provided on how these should be conducted, monitored, what data should be collected and the data collection methods to be employed. The independent recording of such events would ensure the quality of the drill performed, and could also lead to a body of (anonymous) data on the effectiveness of the procedures and notification systems from a variety

92

of different occupancies. This would greatly enhance our understanding of such systems and allow for recommendations to be made with greater confidence. It is critical that information levels are adequate within the population; otherwise, the evacuation process will not be amenable to management and the response of the evacuating population will be based on incomplete or erroneous information. During an evacuation information can be provided before the event (i.e. training, procedural design, practice drills) or during the event (i.e. notification systems, staff, etc.). These options are not equivalent, but they do provide opportunities to inform the population of the response required of them. The potential for these approaches varies according to the nature of the incident (e.g. the speed at which it develops); the type of occupancy (i.e. whether is Type ALL, INC, or PRE-INC) and the population (e.g. capabilities, sensory disabilities, activities, etc.). The use of notification systems that only alert the population of an incident (e.g. a tonal signal) requires the population to be sufficiently attuned to the signal, understand its meaning and are familiar with the response required of them. In this situation, a reasonable response is possible assuming that the population is able to respond independently, or that sufficient staff is on hand to provide the necessary assistance. However, it is difficult to ensure that the entire population is sufficiently trained and familiar with the procedures in place. This difficulty increases significantly with transient populations. However, this problem is also present in occupancies where there is a high turnover of employees. Measures then need to be taken to compensate for this shortfall. It would also not be possible to disseminate current information on the incident using a tonal system. Irrespective of the preparation provided, it would not be able to address the procedural modifications required in response to the many possible scenarios. This would instead be left to the arrival of informed members of staff. This may cause delays in the response of the population awaiting the arrival of staff or lead them to behave inappropriately through a lack of information. The presence of a well-informed, assertive, authoritative, well-trained and numerous staff is considered the benchmark notification system. It is able to influence all aspects of the notification process: message delivery, interpretation, recognition of response and enacting the response. The benefits of staff have been demonstrated even for an untrained population. However, this situation places a significant onus on the preparation, presence and commitment of the sufficiently large number of staff to initiate and manage the evacuation process. It is also susceptible to scenarios that distract staff from these duties. This type of staffing effort should be supported by other means of informing the population of the incident (such as a voice notification system), in order to provide redundancy in the system. This would then benefit initiating the response, and managing its development. In many public spaces, it is not possible to prepare a population to respond appropriately to a tonal signal. In such spaces (e.g. shopping malls, transport terminals, etc.), additional measures should be taken. In such situations, even a numerous, motivated and well-trained staff may have difficulty initiating and managing the response of an untrained population. Given this, there are clear benefits to providing the population with information on the incident, both to encourage their initial response and to guide there subsequent actions. This will allow an informed population to be more convinced of the need to evacuate; to better understand the issues facing them; and to be more receptive to instructions provided to them by staff. The exact manner in which this is achieved is dependent on a number of factors (e.g. nature of the incident, occupancy type, etc.); however, the capacity to provide this information is enormously valuable. The use of a voice alarm system that provides such information will ensure a higher minimum level of information within the general population; this is pivotal to the encouragement of an appropriate response. In situations where there is not enough staff, where staff are forced elsewhere, or where staff injuries have occurred, the untended evacuees would then at least have information on the existence and nature of the incident. It is therefore recommended that voice notification systems are employed to ensure that the population clearly understands the nature of the event and the response required of them. The presence of such a system should complement and assist staff where they are available. Where preparation is possible (e.g. occupancies labelled ALL), voice notification provides redundancy in the system; supports staff activities; can provide current information. Where preparation is not possible (e.g. occupancies labelled INC), voice notification ensures a high minimum level of information.

93

It is foreseeable that these two methods of informing the population (i.e. the presence of staff and a voice notification system) may be traded off against each other. This should be guarded against and ideally taken into regulatory consideration. It is critical that the information is presented in an authoritative and consistent manner; otherwise, the notification system may hinder the progress of the population rather than provide assistance. Clear regulatory guidance is required on this: how to provide the information; the format of the information; what information is included, etc. It appears that a tone is able to alert a population of an event, assuming that the tone is sufficiently distinctive or that the population is familiar with it. From previous research it appears that some tones are more distinctive than others. However, a tone cannot advise of the nature of the event or the required response. This suggests that in public spaces, where training is not possible, that the use of a tonal alarm system alone (or one accompanied by other alerting technology such as strobes) would not be sufficient. In some situations, especially where there are issues of comprehension, intelligibility and/or sensory disabilities, the presence of a tonal signal will ensure that this sub-section of the population can be alerted. This would not provide this sub-population (e.g. those with language issues, those who are deaf or hard of hearing and those in areas where a voice message is unclear) with information on the nature of the incident or their required response, but would at least alert them. This could be accompanied by alternative information (e.g. instructions provided in several languages, video screens), although the viability of this type of approach would need further study. In public spaces, the benefits of a tonal ‘alert’ should be coupled with information provided by a voice notification system. A suitably broad frequency, distinctive tone (e.g. a swooping version of the T-3 tone) would help alert the population. This would be followed by a voice instruction. This would then alert the population and then provide some information on the incident and the required response to it. This could support the actions of the staff in employing the emergency procedure. The method of delivery would need to be assessed. There are certainly advantages to live announcements, although it can place an undue pressure on the person making the announcement. In existing regulatory systems (e.g. NFPA 72 and Australian codes), a complementary tonal and voice system can be employed. The potential for this type of announcement along with similar tone/voice systems needs investigation. However, it should be recognized that this is not a panacea; it will introduce overheads with training and planning, and place an additional onus on the provision and management of accurate and timely information [345] The provision of information should not necessarily be limited to aural information. Where the potential exists, information should also be provided via graphical displays and video screens. This would also benefit those who are deaf or hard of hearing, and would further ensure sufficient coverage of the space in question.

This project required the collection and analysis of data in order to make recommendations regarding the effectiveness of notification systems in public spaces. The results produced have indicated a set of actions, recommendations and further work all of which would increase the effectiveness of the notification systems in place and therefore improve the safety levels for the resident population.

94

12 Discussion The three key objectives of the project were to identify vulnerabilities; to facilitate research on the effectiveness of notification technologies at addressing these vulnerabilities; and to provide recommendations, given the findings produced. In order to achieve these objectives, a number of novel developments had to be made. These included the development of a model to examine vulnerabilities and notification technology; a categorization of occupancies according to the potential for providing information; and a means of assessing the empirical data currently available.

Firstly, in order to identify vulnerabilities, it was necessary to develop a vulnerability model based on identifying fundamental vulnerability components rather than on adopting the traditional high-level statistical approach. This was required given the difficulties found in reliably recording, storing and interrogating fire data. By employing this model, the vulnerabilities identified could be established from first principles. It is felt that this model will be of general value, complementing statistical approaches at identifying factors that influence the evacuation process.

The development of this vulnerability model required an exhaustive literary review. Given the nature of the model, it was important that this included a range of material enabling the placement of the vulnerabilities in context with other issues that arise during an evacuation. As part of the model a metric was developed that enabled the categorization of the vulnerabilities according to fundamental attributes. A similar approach was adopted when examining notification technologies, identifying the factors that these technologies might address. The potential impact of each technology was identified; these were then cross-referenced with the set of the vulnerabilities to establish which technologies might alleviate which of the vulnerabilities. The current empirical data relating to the effectiveness of notification technologies was examined. This provided a picture of our understanding of their capabilities. By examining the relevant recommendations made by the researchers involved, we also gained insight into issues previously identified for future work.

By examining these findings, along with detailed discussions with the Technical Panel, three vulnerabilities were identified that required immediate investigation: people with an alcohol impairment; people who are hard of hearing; and the general population in public spaces. A risk assessment was performed in order to establish the potential impact of reducing these three vulnerabilities. However, the assessment was hampered by the very nature of the selection process: this type of assessment is reliant upon the existence of detailed statistical evidence; the vulnerability model adopted was not directly reliant upon this evidence. This meant that assessment could only provide a limited evaluation of these vulnerabilities. In addition, the assessment could not take into account the secondary impact of these vulnerabilities. However, some important insight was provided:

1. Based on the assumptions made in the analysis, the deaf population account for between 0.4 - 0.5% of the home fire deaths; that the hard-of-hearing population account for 5.9% of 2002 home fire deaths (and 3.7% of the 2002 population), and that the population with hearing troubles account for 16.9% of 2000 home fire deaths (and 13.0% of the 2000 population). Older adults account for approximately 75% of the deaf/ hard of hearing population that die in fires and 60% of those with hearing troubles. Therefore although

95

addressing those with a hearing disability alone may be small, the potential for addressing this vulnerability and providing assistance to the elderly is much larger. Although some elderly people may have additional (physical and cognitive) vulnerabilities that hamper other aspects of their response, the potential for addressing their hearing impairments is still important. These figures also do not reflect the benefit to people in the care of those with a hearing impairment. By addressing this vulnerability, the potential for it to propagate to others (e.g. children in their care) may also be reduced. 2. According to Hall, there is evidence to suggest that approximately 10% of domestic fire fatalities are alcohol-impaired; however, in more focused studies Hall reported that a third of total home fire deaths involved elevated blood alcohol levels. Given that 41% of these were asleep at the time, these people may have benefited directly from earlier and more effective notification. Hall identified that 26% of the alcohol- impaired fatalities succumbed while attempting to escape, rescue others or fight the fire. Although their impaired state may have influenced their capacity to act (as well as respond), this portion of the population may also have benefited from responding earlier and having more time to act. The alcohol-impaired state increases the likelihood of being in close proximity to the fire itself ; for these people additional time would most likely not have been a significant benefit. Caution should be shown in examining these figures; there are issues with identifying and reporting alcohol-impairment, both of which would act to underestimate the numbers involved. In addition, given that situational nature of this vulnerability, people may be temporarily impaired due to alcohol, but be responsible for the well-being of others. Not only would this vulnerability be unpredicted, it would not be addressed (in contrast with long standing impairments). When coupled with sleep, a vulnerability might be apparent at relatively low levels of alcohol; much lower than the public might expect. 3. The assessment of public spaces did not make a persuasive statistical case for further assessment; however, this was not the basis for its selection. Hall identified that although relatively rare, the multiple loss of life in large incidents in public spaces has an impact on the public’s perception of safety; although improvements in this area may have a modest statistical impact, it may have an important psychological impact. In addition, with the current increase in security issues, the importance of notification and confidence in safety becomes even more important.

The three data collection activities were conducted. The work conducted by Bruck , Thomas and Ball addressed domestic occupancies where the participants were asleep and impaired, either through alcohol or through an existing hearing disability. The work conducted by Gwynne addressed several non-domestic occupancies where those involved were awake. Using the categorization developed as part of the Gwynne data collection project [117], Bruck, Thomas and Ball examined PRE-INC occupancies, where detailed information on the desired response could only be provided before the incident; Gwynne examined ALL and INC occupancies [117], where information could potentially be provided before and during the incident, or just during the incident itself.

Bruck, Thomas and Ball made a number of important observations from their experiments conducted in domestic occupancies: 1. The T-3 signal presented at low frequencies (400-520Hz) was best able to alert the alcohol-impaired and those who had mild to moderately severe hearing loss. This signal is referred to as the Low-Frequency Square Wave (LFSW). 2. Strobe technology was ineffective in both cases and was not considered a viable independent alternative. 3. Vibrating technology was less effective than the LFSW signal. For the alcohol impaired 59-63% awoke, while 80-83% of those with a hearing disability awoke. Bruck and Thomas suggested that this would not be suitable for use on its own, but that there may be potential for using this in conjunction with the LFSW signal. They suggest examining the potential for a modular notification approach for the hard of hearing. This is important as, given current regulations, the vibrating device would only operate in conjunction with an audible signal. 4. They found tentative evidence to suggest that people may respond differently to different types of signals. They suggested that for those with a hearing disability that an alarm “kit” that combined two types of sensory signals (i.e. an auditory signal plus a tactile signal) may be more effective than one signal.

96

5. Given that most awakening occurred within 10 seconds of the onset of the signal intensity that caused an awakening, further work was suggested on the periods when the T-3 tone is active and inactive.

Bruck and Thomas focused on domestic occupancies; however, their findings, especially in relation to the LFSW signal, may also be relevant wherever a tonal signal is employed.

Gwynne made the following observations in his examination of trials conducted in public spaces: 1. A level of information is required to enable an efficient evacuation. Ideally this should be provided both before the incident, through preparation, and during the incident, through staff and notification systems. However, in many public spaces, this is not possible; this places an additional onus on the notification technology and staff present. Where preparation was possible and effective, the provision of detailed instructions during the event was less critical, although still important. Where preparation was not possible, the provision of information was absolutely essential, being provided by the voice communication system in place and/or by staff. 2. Where this process completely relied on staff (i.e. no information was provided by the notification system), the time for them to reach and inform the population was important as people often waited for instruction. Therefore, even where staff was present, the ability to disseminate information reliably and instantaneously would still have been of great value. Irrespective of this, the response of the population to the tone and the staff was greatly influenced by the level of preparation. 3. Although not ideal, the provision of information during the incident via notification technology could somewhat compensate for the lack of preparation and the lack of staff; at least some information was available. 4. The T-3 signal was able to alert people to an event occurring, but was not able to instruct them of the required response, especially where people were not prepared; this was accomplished by staff or the notification system. The use of a signal did, however, allow those with language issues to at least be alerted of the event. 5. A combination of tone and information is suggested. The tone (likely a variant of the T-3 signal, e.g. LFSW) would have the potential of alerting the population; this should be followed by voice instruction indicating the nature of the incident and the required response (amongst other things). This information could also be provided by other means (e.g. visually), although this was not explicitly examined as part of these trials. Ideally the tonal signal could include swooping/broadband signals in order to increase the probability of those with a hearing impairment being alerted. 6. The information would be critical where preparation was not possible. Where it was possible, it would be very useful and act as a reminder (to both staff and occupants) and potentially provide current information on the incident. Where staff was present, it could support their actions and act as an additional cue, confirming the nature of the event; in their absence, it could better inform the population.

Gwynne noted that the provision of information via the notification system allowed for a far greater flexibility (and redundancy) in the emergency procedure adopted. In the current Post-9/11 environment, there may be situations (e.g. high-rise evacuations) where people are reluctant to follow managed procedures (e.g. being told to remain while others are evacuated). The suggested approach may be of great value where such a procedure was successfully employed or where the population acted independently, ignoring the procedure in place. Where the procedure was successfully employed, providing information will enable the management to provide specific instructions on how to respond, supports the procedure/staff in place and provides current information on how the incident is developing. This aids the population in their understanding of the situation, the actions required of them and on their receptiveness to instruction. Where the population ignores the procedure in place and evacuate independently, the provision of information can at least update the population of the evolving situation and the routes available/lost (either through smoke or congestion) allowing the evacuees to make better informed decisions. The provision of authoritative, relevant and current information may reduce the probability of the population choosing to evacuate independently in the first instance, by

97

reducing the ambiguity of the situation; and if the population does evacuate independently it can at least inform their response.

In public spaces, there are a number of scenarios that can arise; e.g. a fire, a tornado, an act of terrorism, social disorder, etc. These scenarios may require completely different responses; e.g. evacuate, defend in place, etc. The ability to provide information on the nature of the incident, especially in occupancies where preparation is not possible, allows the nature of the incident to be clearly identified, reducing confusion. This may either dictate the response of the population directly, or support the other information supplied outlining the required response.

Employing such an informed and managed approach, similar to the Australian model [345], requires access to accurate information on the current conditions and also significant staff training; however, it provides additional robustness, and allows greater influence over a wider range of situations. 23

The implementation of the recommendations provided by Bruck, Thomas, Ball and Gwynne is dependent upon the technology available; some recommendations can be implemented immediately while others require technological developments to be made.

Bruck, Thomas and Ball demonstrated that the LFSW signal provides an effective means of alerting a sleeping population (in domestic occupancies) that is impaired with alcohol or with a hearing disability. This appears consistent with previous research regarding other populations including children and older adults [189-195]. Currently, there are technologies available that can provide a low frequency signal in a domestic setting. However, this is larger in size and requires more power than the typical domestic alarm system. This has implications both for positioning and maintenance.24 These technologies are not currently listed. The development of these systems needs to be explored in order to determine their feasibility. However, notification systems currently used in public spaces are able to produce this type of signal. The introduction of this signal into some public spaces would not require significant technological developments.25

Bruck, Thomas and Ball suggested that the ON/OFF patterns employed as part of the T-3 signal are investigated. It would appear that this would require the fine tuning of existing capabilities rather than significant technological developments.

23 NFPA 72 currently allows for a ‘private mode’ approach to be adopted for certain occupancies; e.g. hospitals and institutions. In these situations, it may not be possible or desirable to evacuate a population and they may instead shelter in place or be moved to a refuge. The notification systems may initially inform the staff in settings of the incident, allowing them to prepare and confirm, prior to informing the general population. Therefore systems exist that allows relatively (information-based) responses to be implemented.

24 In section 7.4.5 of NFPA 72 reference is made to ‘narrow-band’ signaling. This presents a performance-based alternative to meeting the +15dB criteria stipulated in NFPA 72. Instead, an assessment has to be made on the performance of the signal in question in relation to the sound frequencies present as part of the ambient noise conditions. This may represent a method of reducing the power required to generate low frequencies; i.e. potentially reducing the volume levels required. 25 This may represent a better short-term opportunity for examining the effectiveness of the change of signal.

98

Bruck and Thomas suggested further research on a modular approach for the hard of hearing, allowing multiple signals to be provided; e.g. a LFSW signal and a vibrating bed shaker/pillow device. This approach may have value beyond the group examined, including those that are similarly vulnerable; e.g. those subject to background noise. Given that several vibrating systems are (remotely) triggered by the sound of the alarm system in place, the development of such a system would then require that (1) the system responded to the appropriately modified trigger (e.g. LFSW signal); and (2) the general alarm system was able to produce such a signal. Other tones could be used to trigger the vibrating devices (e.g. standard T-3); however, these have been shown to be less effective than the LFSW signal [189-195].

Gwynne identified that in public spaces, especially where preparation was not possible (e.g. shopping malls, terminals, etc.), that it was critical to alert and inform. Technology currently exists to produce a tone followed by a voice message through the use of a distributed system/network. As noted in Section 6.1, voice notification systems currently exist that can also produce a range of tones (at various frequencies), followed by live or recorded voice notification. These systems are large-scale and distributed in nature and would typically be employed in non- domestic occupancies. The ability to produce voice messages indicates that the emitter/ speaker system in place is able to produce low frequencies; frequencies that would be required for a LFSW or swooping T-3 signal, for instance.

The principle of providing information during the evacuation is supported by Gwynne’s work. In the data collection activities, the support of a voice notification system was shown to compensate (albeit imperfectly) for an absence of preparation.26 Although not examined directly in this activity (given limitations in the case studies performed), the principle can be extended to visual (and tactile) information. The provision of information should not be limited to audible systems; ideally, visual (and tactile) systems should also be employed were practical. One of the reasons for introducing strobe systems was to provide ‘equivalent facilitation’ [338,344] with the tonal signal employed. Therefore, the strobe system was deemed to provide an equivalent alerting signal (to people that were hard of hearing or deaf) to that provided by a tone signal. If further information is provided by audible systems (e.g. voice message, as suggested here), then the use of a strobe would not provide equivalent information; at best it would be able to alert but not inform, although even this assumption may be questioned in some situations. The provision of additional visual methods may therefore be appropriate. It should also be noted that the provision of visual information would not only benefit those with hearing disabilities, but could also benefit the general population, particularly those that do speak the native language (depending on whether information was text-based or not), or those subject to background noise. It would act as a means of supporting the staff/procedure; providing additional coverage and providing additional supporting cues. A discussion between the relevant communities should be initiated regarding the development of equivalence; e.g. does this concept preclude the immediate extension of notification systems to alert and inform using audible means without the equivalent coverage of visual notification systems, or can a compromise position be reached.27

26 Conversely, preparation allowed occupants to appropriately respond once alerted, although would not have been able to inform the population of changes in the response or the changing environmental conditions. 27 Issue raised by R.Schifiliti.

99

The principle of informing as well as alerting has been demonstrated in Gwynne’s work. The means examined focus on audible systems; however, it is felt that visual systems would aid in this process. In many occupancies, these systems already are in place in order to provide non- emergency information. This may represent a cost-effective means of including visual notification; it also has the added advantage of representing a familiar source of information to the occupant population, given that they already receive non-emergency information from it. 28 Similarly, distributed technology can be exploited as a means of providing information directly to handheld devices with individual members of the population (e.g. cell phones, pages, etc.). These devices would have the advantage of being familiar to the population and would, hopefully, employ a signal that the individual could perceive (tactile, audible, visual, etc.). Technology is available to accommodate this suggestion. The integration of such systems into the emergency system, and the impact upon the evacuee response, requires further investigation. In many public spaces, there are separate notification/information systems to address different non-emergency and emergency scenarios; e.g. an emergency tone, a PA system, public information screens, etc. Often these adhere to different code requirements, are disconnected and compete for the attention of the population. This raises the potential for them providing contradictory information and/or disrupting the distribution of information. The suggestion made above is that current non-emergency systems could be employed in the notification of the population in a fire. This may have implications on the standards to which these systems are held (e.g. survivability of wiring, etc.). This will be influenced by whether these systems are seen as additional, redundant systems (i.e. remain separate but can be used to provide information during an incident), or critical elements of the emergency notification system (i.e. where the system is entirely integrated). This requires further investigation. The logical extension of the approach is that these notification elements should form an integrated system. The information provided to the population could then be better managed, both in non-emergency and emergency scenarios, with the means available better exploited to ensure broader coverage and consistent information. Exactly how this is achieved, especially regarding code requirements and implementation, needs thought to ensure that the findings presented here are addressed in the short-term, and that a coherent approach can be developed in the long-term.

A general scheme is proposed. This combines the modular approach suggested by Bruck and Thomas with the notification concept produced by Gwynne. Bruck, Thomas and Ball suggested that, given that people may respond to different signals, the provision of multiple signals may improve effectiveness. Their work related specifically to the hard of hearing. Although not empirically examined as part of their data collection activity, the modular approach is incorporated into a general scheme as it is suggested (by the current author) that it may have value beyond the population originally examined.

Gwynne suggests that in public spaces it is critical to alert and inform. In Gwynne’s data collection activity [117], only audible systems were examined. Where a general population is present, with a variety of physical and cognitive capabilities, and language skills, other methods

28 Where such systems do not already exist, the same principle can potentially be applied to introduce them; i.e. employing them in a dual role. For instance, screens could be paid for by commercial vendors; these would provide commercial information in non-emergency situations, while informing the population of the incident and response during an emergency.

100

should be exploited in addition to the audible systems examined above; i.e. the principle of alerting and informing should be extended from audible systems (e.g. from tone to voice), to visual systems (e.g. from strobe to screen/LED display) and tactile systems (e.g. from vibrating to paging). Although this connection may be empirically weak in this project, it is felt to be logically sound (see Figure 21).

Modular Approach

PRE-INC ALL

LFSW+OTHER? ALERT +INFO

LFSW+V0JCE? ~ . STAFF+ : STROBE+sCREEN? : ·I ~----~ ·l - ···------. I +PAGE? : ' .. ______VIB? .,.; . ' ' ' ' LFSW + VOICE : , ...... ----.. -----~----- , STROBE+SCREEN? : Exploit r---- existing VIB?+ PAGE? : svstems \ ______., ------

Figure 21: The Modular Approach. If the inclusion of a component is dependent on other factors (such as the level of preparation or availability of technology), then a question mark is included. If the impact of the component is also dependent on these factors, the font size is reduced.

In Figure 21 the selection of the notification system is initially dependent on the potential to provide information before and during the incident. The categorization employed was derived from the Gwynne data collection activity performed in Phase II of this project (see Section 11 ) [117]. If the occupancy is PRE-INC (e.g. a domestic/small-scale residential, where information can only be provided prior to the event) then, according to Bruck, Thomas and Ball, a LFSW signal should be employed. In addition, Bruck and Thomas identified that this could be accompanied by another signal (e.g. a vibrating device) given variations in the response to certain signals in the population with hearing disabilities examined (e.g. the hard of hearing). It is acknowledged that the value of extending this principle to other populations requires further investigation The technology for this approach is available although requires integration and further development.

If the occupancy type is ALL (e.g. office blocks), where the potential exists for providing information before and during the event then it would be highly beneficial to alert and inform the population. This principle should be extended to visual (and tactile) notification systems where it

101

is possible/available. This approach would provide updated information, additional cues, and support to the staff. In some situations, the level of training and motivation provided would allow the population to respond more independently, as they recognize the tone and understand the response required of them. This reduces the potential impact of the ability to inform. However, this also places a great deal of emphasis on the preparation conducted and the performance of the staff present. Irrespective of this, the ability to inform may still be of great benefit in response to an unanticipated scenario and in updating the population of the evolving scenario, should its development influence the response required. Also, in many situations the staff may not be sufficiently trained or motivated (i.e. although the potential exists for preparation, in reality this potential may not be realized). Here, the notification process could reaffirm the response required of them and assure staff of the authenticity of the incident, as much as it would the occupant population.

If the occupancy type is INC (e.g. shopping malls, transport terminals, etc.), where there is no possibility of preparing the occupant population then it is absolutely critical to alert and inform the population. The staff should be supported at least by a tone and voice notification system. Ideally this should be accompanied by other visual/tactile systems given their availability. As noted in Section 6.1, there are currently systems available that allow a more integrated approach to providing information; therefore although the dominant means of communication is through the staff and audible systems, they could be supported by the use of existing (i.e. non-emergency) screens, pagers, PC monitors, etc. Assuming that the information provided is consistent between approaches, this may provide an additional safety net. The ideal situation would involve a completely integrated approach utilizing the available audible/visual/tactile systems in order to provide consistent, comprehensive and current information in all scenarios.

As mentioned above, the management of information to a population, especially in public spaces, occurs in a variety of different scenarios and employs a number of different human and technological resources. Currently the technological (and often the human) resources are separate, uncoordinated and potentially incompatible. This can allow inconsistent information to be provided and systems/opportunities to go unexploited. This may represent a terrible waste during an emergency. Ideally the process of providing information to a population within a structure should be seen as a single system that operates under different scenarios. Fire emergencies represent just one scenario; others include natural disasters, security breaches, and the normal operation of the structure. By seeing the provision of information in this manner, it may provide an opportunity to exploit other technologies not normally used (e.g. video screens); staffing opportunities not exploited (e.g. security staff); and fields of understanding that might otherwise not appear relevant (e.g. mass notification) in response to a fire. It may provide an opportunity to learn from adjacent areas not normally examined when designing fire notification systems and better exploit the existing (and potential) resources available. Fire is but one of the many emergencies that can arise.

In designing a notification system in response to fire, we should be cognizant of the expertise available in addressing other types of incident (and learn from it) and be aware of the methods employed. This project has addressed the needs of the public, the technology available (especially in relation to the U.S.) and the information needed to facilitate safe evacuation. Although significant short-term improvements can be made through relatively small changes, the

102

future, especially as the number of incident scenarios increases, requires a change in how we provide information. Instead of employing notification systems for each scenario, a notification system should be implemented that can address different credible scenarios, be they emergency or non-emergency. The current fire notification systems may form a critical part of this system, but should not stand in isolation from it.

103

13 Conclusions and Recommendations The original scope of the project outlined four key tasks. All of these were addressed: Monitor risk assessment exercise; Review the current literature of relevant human behavior studies and make recommendations on appropriate new studies; Review current and emerging technologies that address the performance criteria; Assess the information compiled in the above tasks to develop recommendations on notification. Each of these four tasks represented a major effort. Importantly each of the tasks was designed and completed in reference to each other; i.e. in reference to overall issue of understanding vulnerabilities and evaluating notification technologies.

The notification process does not happen in isolation. When it occurs, it is subject to the same detrimental factors (e.g. information pollution) that are apparent in non-emergency conditions. It is therefore imperative to understand the conditions under which the notification technologies operate and the nature of the target audience before implementing them.

Vulnerabilities can take many forms. These can be associated with the individual themselves or be due to the scenario in which they find themselves. The manner in which the vulnerability arises can influence the probability of it being recorded and it being addressed. These factors needed to be understood before prioritizing the vulnerabilities in the population. By developing and employing the vulnerability model, vulnerabilities were identified in a manner more independent of the data collection techniques; they were instead established from first principle. By doing this, it was possible to identify where vulnerabilities exist, understand their nature and highlight how reducing one vulnerability may have a direct and indirect benefit; i.e. to the person directly involved and to others.

By reviewing a range of material, vulnerabilities were identified and an assessment made of the current technologies available. This, along with further discussion and analysis, allowed vulnerabilities to be prioritized and trials conducted. These trials addressed domestic situations and larger public spaces. Although imperfect, they shed light on the vulnerabilities examined and identified recommendations and future work.

Bruck, Thomas and Ball established the importance of adopting a low frequency square wave T- 3 (LFSW) signal in spaces where people sleep. During this work, the effectiveness of this signal was established for alcohol impaired and those with a hearing disability. This was consistent with previous research regarding other populations including children and older adults. Other technologies examined were less effective; e.g. strobes and vibrating devices presented in isolation. Bruck, Thomas and Ball identified several key areas for future work: 1. That the technical feasibility of replacing the current high frequency smoke alarm T-3 signal with a low frequency square wave T-3 (LFSW) signal, for the entire population, be investigated. 2. That the T-3 pattern duration (ON/OFF pattern) be further investigated. 3. That the strobe should not be considered as an effective viable means of independent notification for people asleep.

104

4. That the combined use of signals be investigated (e.g. auditory T-3 plus one other), in conjunction with the development of a modular kit where combinations of these signals can be delivered. 5. That there should be further investigation of an appropriate means of measuring the intensity of bed and pillow shakers and that this inform a new standard.

In public spaces, Gwynne clearly established the importance of both alerting and informing the population. This importance increased where preparation was not possible and the effectiveness of the emergency procedure was not guaranteed. Gwynne identified several key areas of future work: 6. Guidance should be provided on the use of a tone/voice combination in public spaces. This is generally important, but critical when preparation is not possible. The tone employed (e.g. low frequency square wave T-3, swooping tone, broadband etc.) should be examined along with the content and format of the voice message provided including whether the voice announcement is live or pre-recorded. Clear guidance should be supplied on the information provided in such messages, making reference to literature associated with mass notification systems. 7. The technical feasibility of utilizing existing means of visual (and tactile) notification should be explored for public spaces. The potential for the emergency notification system to exploit existing non-emergency communication (e.g. large screens, televisions, monitors, paging, etc.) should also be examined.

These findings were derived directly from the three data collection activities. Building on these findings, and on the rest of the work conducted during this project, a simple framework was developed recommending the notification systems to be implemented. This is sensitive to the potential for providing information to a population before and during an incident, rather than just to the type of occupancy involved.

105

14 References [1] Ahrens, M. (2006), U.S. Fires in Selected Occupancies. National Fire Protection Association. [2] Ahrens, M. U.S. (2004), Experience with Smoke Alarms and Other Fire Detection/Alarm Equipment. National Fire Protection Association. [3] Hall, J., (2005), Characteristics of Home Fire Victims, National Fire Protection Association. [4] Hall, J.R., (2004), How Many People Can Be Saved From Home Fires if Given More Time to Escape, , 40, 117-126. [5] Karter, M.J., (2006), FIRE LOSS IN THE UNITED STATES DURING 2005 ABRIDGED REPORT, Fire Analysis and Research Division, National Fire Protection Association, July. [6] Office of the Deputy Prime Minister (2003), Fire Statistics, United Kingdom, 2001, National Statistics. [7] Regional distribution of deaths from residential fires— United States (1987), 1978–1984. Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 1987;36: 645–9. [8] Sekizawa A. (1991), Statistical Analyses on Fatalities Characteristics of Residential Fires, Fire Safety Science-- Proceedings of the Third International Symposium, pp.475-484. [9] Sekizawa A. (1994), International Comparison Analyses on Fire Risk Among the United States, the United Kingdom, and Japan, Fire Safety Science--Proceedings of the Fourth International Symposium, pp.961-969. [10] U.S. Census Bureau and U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development Office of Policy Development and Research (2004), American Housing Survey for the United States: 2003 Current Housing Reports, September. [11] Budd T, Mayhew P. (1997), Fires in the home in 1995: results from the British Crime Survey. London: Government Statistical Service. [12] Collier, P., and Weston, L. (1998), Fire Statistics: United Kingdom 1997, Home Office Research, Development and Statistics Directorate, Issue 25/99, ISSN 0143 6384, 3 November. [13] Best, R.,L.(1997), Reconstruction Of A Tragedy: The Beverly Hills Supper Club Fire, Southgate, Kentucky, May 28, NFPA. [14] Boyce, K.E., Shields, T.J., and Silcock, G.W.H. (1999), Toward the Characterization of Building Occupanices for Fire Safety Engineering: Capability of People with Disabilties to Read and Locate Exit Signs, Fire Technology, 35, 1. [15] Brennan, P. (1998), Victims and Survivors in Fatal Residential Building Fires, Proceedings of the First International Symposium on Human Behaviour in Fire, pp.157-166. [16] Brennan, P. (1996), Impact Of Social Interaction On Time To Begin Evacuation In Office Building Fires:Implications For Modelling Behaviour, Proceedings Of The Seventh International Fire Science And Engineering Conference, Interflam'96, Ed:Franks,C. and Grayson, S., pp701-710, ISBN 0951632094, 26- 28/3/1996. [17] Brennan, P. (1995), Smoke Gets In Your Eyes:The Effect Of Cue Perception On Behaviour In Smoke, Proceeding For The International Conference On Fire Science And Engineering, Asiaflam'95,Eds: Chow,W.K.Et Al, pp187-197,ISBN 0951632078,March. [18] Brennan, P. (1999), Modelling Cue Recognition and Pre-Evacuation Response, Proceedings – 6th International Symposium, International Association for Fire Safety Science, , MA, July 1999, pp. 1029-1040. [19] Brennan P and Thomas IR (2001) Victims of Fire? Predicting Outcomes in Accidental Fires, Second International Symposium on Human Behaviour in Fire, Boston, March, Interscience Publications, London [20] Bryan, J.L. (1977), Smoke As A Determinant Of Human Behaviour In Fire Situations, University Of Maryland. [21] Bryan, J. (2001), Psychological Variables That May Affect Fire Alarm Design, Fire Protection Engineering, Summer 2001, pp. 42-48. [22] Bryan, J.L. (1983), An Examination And Analysis Of The Dynamics Of Human Behaviour In The MGM Grand Hotel Fire,National Fire Protection Agency, Quincy. [23] Butler, G. W. (1993), The Factors Involved In Evacuation And The Extent To Which Efficient Management Can Influence These, The Proceedings Interflam '93, Interscience Comms Ltd., London, pp 628 - 635. [24] Butler, G.W. (1993), Private Communication - Observation Of A Routine At A Multi Occupied Building In Newcastle Upon Tyne., 23rd April. [25] Canter, D. (1990), Overview Of Human Behaviour, Fires And Human Behaviour(2nd Edition), Ed. D.Canter, Fulton, pp205-234.

106

[26] Canter, D., Breaux, J., Sime, J. (1990), Domestic, Multiple Occupancy, And Hospital Fires, Fires And Human Behaviour (2nd Edition), Ed. Canter,D., Fulton, pp116-136. [27] Chubb, M. (1993), Human Factors Lessons From Public Fire Educators: Lessons From Major Fires, Phoenix Education Section. NFPA . [28] Donald, I. And Canter, D.(1990), Behavioural Aspects Of The King's Cross Disaster, Fires And Human Behaviour (2nd Edition), Ed. Canter,D., Fulton, pp15-30. [29] Dunlop, K. E. and Shields, T. J., (1994), Real Fire Emergency Evacuation for Disabled People, Proceedings - Symposium: Engineering Fire Safety in the Process of Design, University of Ulster at Jordanstown, Newtownabbey, Northern Ireland 13-16 September 1993, pp. 157-164. [30] Fahy, R. (1996), Exit89 - High-Rise Evacuation Model - Recent Enhancements And Example Applications, Proceedings Of The Seventh International Fire Science And Engineering Conference, Interflam 96,pp1001- 1005, Ed:Franks,C.And Grayson,S., ISBN 0951632094, 26-28/3/1996. [31] Fahy, R., and Molis, J.,L. (2004), Fatalities in Home Fires Where Smoke Alarms Operated, Human Behavior in Fire – Proceedings of the Third International Symposium, September 1-3, 2004, Belfast, N. Ireland, Interscience Communications Ltd., London, England. [32] Fahy, R.F. & Proulx, G. (2001), Toward Creating a Database on Delay Times to Start Evacuation and Walking Speeds for Use in Evacuation Modeling, 2nd International Symposium on Human Behavior in Fire, Boston, MA, March 2001, pp. 175-183. [33] Feinberg, W.E. And Johnson, N.,R. (1998), Queuing, Exit-Sorting And Evacuation In Fire Emergencies: A Computer Simulation Investigation, Proceedings Of The 1st International Symposium On Human Behaviour In Fire, Shield, J. (Ed), Fire Engineering Research And Technology Centre, University Of Ulster. [34] Final Report on the Collapse of the World Trade Center, Federal Building and Fire Safety Investigation of the World Trade Center Disaster, NIST NCSTAR 1, National Institute of Standards and Technology, U.S. Department of Commerce, September, (2005). [35] Fruin, J., J. (1994),The Causes And Prevention Of Crowd Disasters, Engineering For Crowd Safety , Eds :Smith, R.A. and Dickie, J.F., Elsevier, ISBN 0444899200. [36] Glass, A.J. (1970), The Psychological Aspects of Emergency Situations, in Abram,.H.S. (ed), Psychological Aspects of Stress, 62-69, Illinois. [37] Glass, R And Rubin, A.J. (1977), Emergency Communication In High-Rise Buildings, from Human response to Tall Buildings, ed: Conway,D.J., Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross, ISBN 0879332689,Stroudsburg. [38] Grosshandler, W.L., Bryner,N., Madrzykowski, D., and Kuntz., K. (2005), Draft Report of the Technical Investigation of the Station Nightclub Fire, NIST NCSTAR 2, National Institute of Standards and Technology, U.S. Department of Commerce, March.. [39] Gwynne, S., Galea, E.R., Owen, M. and Lawrence, P.J. (1999), Escape As A Social Response, Published By The Society Of Fire Protection Engineers. [40] Gwynne, S., Galea, E.R., Lawrence, P. J., Owen ,M. And Filippidis L. (1999), Adaptive Decision-Making In buildingEXODUS In Response To Exit Congestion, Proceedings Of The 6th IAFSS Symposium, Poitiers, France. [41] Gwynne, S., Galea, E., R. And Owen, M. (1999), An Investigation Of The Aspects Of Occupant Behaviour Required For Evacuation Modelling, Journal Of Applied Fire Science, Vol 8(1), pp 19-59, 1998-1999. [42] Gwynne, S., Galea, E.R., Parke, J., and Hickson, J., (2003), The collection and analysis of pre-evacuation times from evacuation trials and their application to evacuation modelling, Fire Technology, 39 (2), 2003, pp. 173– 195. [43] Hasofer, A.M. and Bruck, D. (2004), Statistical Analysis of Response to Fire Cues, , 39, pp663-688. [44] Horiuchi, S.,Murozaki, Y.,Hokugo, A.,A. (1986), Case Study Of Fire And Evacuation In A Multi-Purpose Office Building, Osaka,Japan. Fire Safety Science-The Proceedings For The First International Symposium, Hemisphere Publishing Corp., Washingtion, pp523-532. [45] Jones, B.K. And Hewitt, J.,A. (1986), Leadership And Group Formation In High-Rise Building Evacuations, Fire Safety Science-The Proceedings For The First International Symposium, Hemisphere Publishing Corp., Washingtion, pp513-522. [46] Johnson, N.R. (1997), Panic At 'The Who Concert Stampede: An Empirical Assessment, Social Problems , Vol 34, No.4, October. [47] Johnson, L.C. Spinweber, C. L., Webb, S. C. & Muzet, A. G. (1987), Dose Level of Triazolam on Sleep and Response to a Smoke Detector Alarm, PsychoPharmacology, 91:397-402.

107

[48] Johnson, N.,R. And Feinberg, W.,E. (1997), The Impact Of Exit Instructions And Number Of Exits In Fire Emergencies: A Computer Simulation Investigation, Journal Of Environmental Psychology,17, pp123-133, Academic Press Limited, 1997. [49] Johnson, N.,R., Feinberg, W.,E. And Johnston, D.,M., Microstructure And Panic:The Impact Of Social Bonds On Individual Action In Collective Flight From The Beverly Hills Supper Club Fire, From Disaster, Collective Behaviour And Social Organisation, (Ed:Dynes, R., And Tierney, K.) [50] Johnston, D.,M., And Johnson, N., Role Expansion In Disaster: An Investigation Of Employee Behaviour In A Night Club Fire, Sociological Focus, 22,pp39-51 [51] Julliet, E. (1993), Evacuating People With Disabilities, Fire Engineering, 126,12, pp100-103,November. [52] Kakegawa, S., Yashiro,Y., Ebhira, M., Ohtsuki, A. (1994), Evaluation Of Fire Safety Measures In Care Facilities For The Elderly By Simulating Evacuation Behaviour, Proceedings Of Fourth International Symposium On Fire Safety Science, Ed:Kashiwagi,T.L, pp645-656, ISBN 1-886279-00-4. [53] Keating, J.P. And Loftus, E.,F. (1981),Psychology Today, 14. [54] Keating, J.P. And Loftus, E.,F. (1974), People Care in Fire Emergencies – Psychological Aspects, SFPE Technological Report 75-4, MA, USA. [55] Kilpatrick, E.P. (1957), Problems of Perception in Extreme Situations, Human Organization, 16, Summer, 20- 22. [56] Ko, S., Spearpoint, M., and Teo, A.(2007), Trial Evacuation of an Industrial Premises and Evacuation Model Comparisons, Fire Safety Journal, 42, 2007, 91-105. [57] Latane, B. And Darley, J.M. (1969), Bystander Apathy, American Scientist,57, pp224-268. [58] Lerup, L., Cronrath, D., Liu, J.K.C, Fires In Nursing Facilities, Fires And Human Behaviour (2nd Edition), Ed. D.Canter, Fulton, pp155-180,1990 [59] Levin B.M. (1987),Exitt-A Simulation Model Of Occupant Decsion And Actions In Residential Fires:User's Guide And Program Desrciption, NBSr 87-3591,National Bureau Of Standards,Gaithersburg, MD. [60] Lindell, M.K. and Perry, R.W. (1987), Warning Mechanisms in Emergency Response Systems, International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters, 5,2,137-153. [61] Loveridge, R.W. (1998), Fatalities from Fire in One and Two Family Residential Dwellings. (In Human Behaviour in Fire: Proceedings of the 1st International Symposium, Belfast. 393-399. Belfast: Textflow). [62] Macphail, C. (1991), The Myth Of The Madding Crowd, Aldine De Gruyter, New York. [63] Guth, D.A.; Hill, E.W.; Rieser, J.J. (1989),Tests of blind pedestrians’ use of traffic sounds for street-crossing alignment. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness,83(9), 461-468. [64] Mileti, D.S., Drabek, T.E., and Haas, J.E. (1975), Human Systems in Extreme Environments: A Sociological Perspective, Inst. Of Behavioral Science, CO. [65] Mileti, D.S. (1990), Communication of Emergency Public Warnings: A Social Science Perspective and State- of-the-Art Assessment, Prepared for FEMA, ORNL-6609. [66] Passini, R.,Proulx, G.(1995), Building Access And Safety For The Visually Impaired Person,Faculte De L'amenagement, University Of Montreal., Proceeding For The International Conference On Fire Science And Engineering, Asiaflam'95, Eds: Chow, W.K.Et Al, pp139-150, March, ISBN 0951632078, March. [67] Pauls, J.(1989), Evacuation and Other Behavior in Emergencies in Buildings, Meeting Special Needs of the Disabled in Evacuation and Sheltering Systems, March 22, 1989, Federal Emergency Management Agency, USA, 11 p.. [68] Pauls, J., Juillet, E. (1993), Life Safety of People with Disabilities: How Far Have We Progressed? Proceedings of the CIBW14 International Symposium and Workshops Engineering Fire Safety in the Progress of Design: Demonstrating Equivalency, Part 1, University of Ulster, pp.17-40, 1993. [69] Pauls, J., 1989, Recent Technical and Social Developments Influencing the Life Safety of People with Disabilities, prepared for The Pacific Rim Conference of Building Officials, Honolulu, HA, April 9-13, 1989 and for The National Fire Protection Association Annual Meeting, Washington, DC, May 15-18, 1989, 20 p. [70] Pauls, J. and Groner, N., 1988, Emergency Management Planning to Improve Safety for People with Disabilities: Guidelines for Facility Managers, in Egress Procedures and Technologies for People with Disabilities, United States Architectural and Transportation Barriers Compliance Board, Washington, DC,. [71] Paulsen, T.(1994), The Effect Of Escape Route Information On Mobility And Way Finding Under Smoke Logged Conditions, Proceedings Of Fourth International Symposium On Fire Safety Science, Ed:Kashiwagi, T.L, pp693 -704, ISBN 1-886279-00-4. [72] Pigott, B.B.(1989), Fire Detection And Human Behaviour, Proceedings Of The Second International Symposium On Fire Safety Science, Ed: Wakamatsu, T., Hasemi, Y., Seizawa, A., Seeger, P., Pagni, P., And Grant, C., pp573-581, ISBN 0-891168648.

108

[73] Proulx, G., and Pineau, J. (1996), ‘Review of Evacuation Strategies for Occupants with Disabilities’, IRC-IR- 712, IRC-NRC. [74] Proulx, G.(2002), Evacuation Planning for Occupants with Disabilities, Internal Report No. 843. IRC-NRC. [75] Proulx, G. and LaRoche, C. (2003), Recollection, Identification and Perceived Urgency of the Temporal-Three Evacuation Signal, Journal of fire Protection Engineering, Vol. 13, No. 1, 2003, pp. 67-82. [76] Proulx, G. and Sime, J. (1991), To Prevent Panic In An Underground Emergency: Why Not Tell People The Truth?, Fire Safety Science- 3rd Symposium, Elsevier, Appl. Sci., NY, pp843-853. [77] Proulx, G. and Fahy,R. (1997), A Study Of The New York World Trade Centre Evacuation, Proceedings Of The Third International Symposium On Fire Safety Science, Eds: Cox, G. And Langford, G., pp713-724, Elsevier, London. [78] Proulx, G. (1996), Lessons Learned On Occupants Movement Times And Behaviour During Evacuation Drills, Interflam 96, pp1007-1011. [79] Proulx, G. (1995), Evacuation Times And Movement Times In Apartment Buildings, Fire Safety Journal, 24, pp229-246, Elsevier,0379-7112. [80] Proulx, G. (1994), Time Delay To Start Evacuating Upon Hearing The Fire Alarm, Proceedings Of Human Factors And Ergonomics Society 38th Annual Meeting , pp811-815. [81] Proulx, G. (1998), The Impact Of Voice Communication Messages During A Residential High-Rise Fire, Human Behaviour In Fire: Proceedings Of The First International Symposium, 31 August-2 September 1998, ISBN 1-85923-103-9, pp265-271. [82] Proulx, G., Laroche, C., and Latour, J.C. (1995), Audibility Problems with Fire Alarms in Apartment Buildings, Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 29th Annual Meeting. [83] Proulx, G. and Pineau, J.(1996), Difference in the Evacuation Behavior of Office and Apartment Building Occupants, Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 30th Annual Meeting. [84] Proulx, G. and Pineau, J. (1996), Review of Evacuation Strategies for Occupants with Disabilities, Internal Report No. 712, NRC-CNRC, April. [85] Proulx,G. and McQueen, C.(1994), Evacuation Timing in Apartment Buildings, Internal Report No. 660, NRC- CNRC, June. [86] Proulx,G.(1993), Housing Evacuation of Mixed Ability Occupants: A Case Study, CIB Seminar/Workshop, University of Ulster,13-16th September, UK. [87] Proulx, G. and Pineau, J.(1996), The Impact of Age on Occupant’s Behaviour During A Residential Fire, Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 40th Annual Meeting, September 2-6, Pennsylvania, USA. [88] Proulx, G., Latour, J. C., MacLaurin, J. W., Pineau, J., Hoffman, L. E. and Laroche, C., 1995, Housing Evacuation of Mixed Abilities Occupants in Highrise Buildings, Internal Report 706, Institute for Research in Construction, National Research Council of Canada, Ottawa, Ontario, 92 p. [89] Proulx, G.(2007), Response to Fire Alarms, Fire Protection Engineering, Winter. [90] Proulx,G. and Reid,I.M.A.(2006), Occupant Behavior and Evacuation during the Cook County Administration Fire, Fire Protection Engineering, Vol. 16, Number 14, pp293-309. [91] Proulx,G. (2000), Strategies for Ensuring Appropriate Occupant Response to Fire Alarm Signals, Construction Technology Update No. 43, Published by the Institute for Research in Construction, NRC-CRC, ISSN 1206- 1220, Canada.. [92] Proulx, G., Laroche, C., Jaspers-Fayer, F.,Lavallée, R.(2001) , Fire Alarm and Signal Recognition, Internal Report IRC-IR-828, Institute for Research in Construction, National Research Council of Canada, Ottawa, Ontario. [93] Purser, D. (2005), Comparisons Of Evacuation Efficency And Pre-Movement Times In Response To A Sounder And Two Different Voice Alarm Messages, Hartford Environmental Research.. [94] Purser, D. (1999), People and Fire, Inaugural Lecture Series, 17th February, University Of Greenwich, ISBN 1- 86166-117-7. [95] Purser, D. and Kuipers, M.E. (2004), Interactions Between Buildings, Fires and Occupant Behavior Using a Relational Database Created From Incident Investigations and Interviews, Proceedings of the Third International Symposium, pp443-456, Interscience Communications Ltd., London, England. [96] Purser, D.A. (1998), Quantification of Behavior for Engineering Design Standards and Escape Time Calculations, Human Behaviour In Fire: Proceedings Of The First International Symposium, 31 August-2 September 1998, ISBN 1-85923-103-9, pp497-506. [97] Quarantelli, E.L. (1984), Perceptions and Reactions to Emergency Warnings of Sudden Hazards, Ekestics, 309, Nove-Dec.

109

[98] Quarantelli, E., L (1977)., Panic Behaviour: Some Empirical Observations, In Human Response To Tall Buildings (Edited By Donald J. Conway), (Community Development Series;34), ISBN 087933268,Stroudsburg, Pa, Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross. [99] Shields, T.J., and Boyce, K.E. (2000), A Study of Evacuation from Large Retail Store, Fire Safety Journal, 35, 25-49. [100] Shields T. J., Dunlop K. E., And Silcock G. W. H.(1996), Escape Of Disabled People From Fire. A Measurement And Classification Of Capability For Assessing Escape Risk, BRE Report 301, ISBN 1 86081 0675.. [101] Shields, T.J., Proulx, G. (1999), The Science of Human Behaviour: Past Research Endeavours, Current Developments and Fashioning a Research Agenda, Fire Safety Science--Proceedings of the Sixth International Symposium, pp.95-114. [102] Shields, T. J., Smyth, B., Boyce, K.E., Silcock, G.W.H.(1998), Evacuation Behaviours of Occupants with Learning Difficulties in Residential Homes, Proceedings of the First International Symposium on Human Behaviour in Fire, pp.369-377. [103] Sime, J. D.(1987), Access and Egress for the Handicapped in Public Buildings, in G. Haber and T. Blanks (Eds.) Building Design for Handicapped and Aged Persons: An International Inventory, Portsmouth, UK, 27 p.. [104] Sime,J.(1990),The Concept Of Panic, Edited D. Canter pp 83 - 95). Fires And Human Behaviour (2nd Edition), Ed. D. Canter, Fulton, pp63-82, 1-85346-105-9. [105] Sime, J.(1992), Human Behaviour In Fires Summary Report, CFBAC Report No.450,Portsmouth Polytechnic. [106] Sime, J.(1984), Escape Behaviour In Fire: 'Panic' Or Affiliation?, PhD Thesis, Department Of Psychology, University Of Surrey. [107] Sime, J. (1990), Escape Behaviour In Fires And Evacuations, Fires And Human Behaviour(2nd Edition), Ed. D.Canter,Fulton, pp56-87. [108] Sime, J.(1983), Affiliative Behaviour During Escaping To Building Exits, Journal Of Environmental Psycholgy, 3(1), 1983, pp21-42. [109] Sime, J., Breaux, J., & Canter, D.(1994), Human Behavior Patterns in Domestic and Hospital Fires, Fire Research Station, UK, BRE Occasional Paper 59, May. [110] Sime, J.,D.(1997), Visual Access Configurations: Spatial Analysis And Occupant Response Inputs To Architectural Design And Fire Engineering, IAPS Conference, 14-17 July, Eindhoven, The Netherlands, August. [111] Summerland Fire Commission Report (1974), Douglas: Isle Of Man Fire Report. [112] Tavares, R.M., Gwynne, S, and Galea E.R., (2007) Collection and Analysis of Trial Evacuation Data Collected from Library Facilities in Brazil, CMS Press Report 07/IM/132, ISBN: 978-1-904521-44-0. [113] Tong, D. And Canter, D.(1985), Informative Warnings: In Situ Evaluations Of Fire Alarms, Fire Safety Journal , 9, pp267-279. [114] Wood, P.G.(1990), Survey Of Behaviour In Fires, Edited D. Canter pp 83 - 95). Fires And Human Behaviour (2nd Edition), Ed. D. Canter, Fulton, pp205-234. [115] Woolworth, The Inquest (1979), Fire, 72, 892, pp245-248. [116] Gwynne, SM.V.(2007), A Framework for Identifying Groups Particularly Vulnerable to Fire and the Effectiveness of Notification Technologies, Suppression and Detection Research and Applications (SUPDET 2007), Orlando, Florida. [117] Gwynne,S.M.V., (2007), Notification Effectiveness of Large Groups, Prepared for the Fire Protection Research Foundation, NFPA, Hughes Associates, Inc. [118] NFPA 101 (2006), Life Safety Code, National Fire Protection Association, Quincy, MA. [119] Nelson, H.E. And Maclennan, H.A., "Emergency Movement,” The SFPE, Handbook Of Fire Protection Engineering (2nd Edition), Ed: DiNenno, P.J., Beyer, C.L., Custer, R.L.P., Walton, W.D., Watts, J.M.W., Drysdale, D., Hall, J.R., National Fire Protection Association, Quincy, MA, pp(3-286)-(3-295),1996. [120] Pauls, J. (1996), Movement Of People, The SFPE, Handbook Of Fire Protection Engineering (2nd Edition), Ed: DiNenno, P.J., Beyer, C.L., Custer, R.L.P., Walton, W.D., Watts, J.M.W., Drysdale, D., Hall, J.R., National Fire Protection Association, Quincy, MA, pp(3-263)-(3-285). [121] Jin,T.(2002), Visbility and Human Behavior in Smoke, The SFPE Handbook of Fire Protection Engineering (3rd edition), Ed: DiNenno, P.J., et al, National Fire Protection Association, Quincy, MA, pp(2-42)-(2-53). [122] Bryan, J.(1996), Behavioural Response To Fire And Smoke,The SFPE, Handbook Of Fire Protection Engineering(2nd Edition), Ed:DiNenno, P.J., Beyer, C.L.,Custer, R.L.P.,Walton, W.D.,Watts, J.M.W.,Drysdale, D., Hall, J.R., , National Fire Protection Association,Quincy,Ma, pp (1-241)-(1-262).

110

[123] Purser,D. (2002), Toxicity Assessment of Combustion Products, The SFPE Handbook of Fire Protection Engineering (3rd edition), Ed: DiNenno, P.J., et al, National Fire Protection Association, Quincy, MA, pp(2- 83)-(2-171). [124] Proulx,G.(2002), Movement Of People: The Evacuation Timing of People, The SFPE Handbook of Fire Protection Engineering (3rd edition), Ed: DiNenno, P.J., et al, National Fire Protection Association, Quincy, MA, pp(3-342)-(3-366). [125] Gwynne, S.(2006), Post-WTC Management of Emergency Movement from Tall Buildings, International Fire Protection Magazine, Issue 27, August. [126] Muir, H.C. and Cobbett, A., (1996), Influence Of Cabin Crew Behaviour In Emergency Evacuations At Floor Level Exits, Report Prepared For CAA/FAA, CAA Paper 95006:Part A, (FAA No.Dot/Faa/Ar-95/52), CAA, London, ISBN 0 86039 6495. [127] Gwynne, S.,(2006),Emergency Show and Tell: The Use of Audible and Visual Notification Systems, International Fire Protection Magazine, November. [128] Gwynne, S.M.V., and Boswell, D. (2007), Integrating Fire Safety and Security into Movement Management, Fire and Security Today, pp38-45, January/February. [129] Bruck, D., (2001) The Who, What, Where and Why of Waking to Fire Alarms: A Review, Fire Safety Journal, Vol. 36, 2001. [130] Bruck, D.(2003), Smoke Alarms: Waking Up to the Truth, National Fire and Rescue. http://www.nfrmag.com/backissues/NovDec03/feature2.asp [131] Withington, D.J., (2000), Faster evacuation from ferries with sound beacons, Fire, 30. [132] Jiang, H., Galea, E.R., Jia, F., Lawrence, P., Gwynne, S., Ingason, H.(2004), The Forensic Investigation of Fire Incidents Using Computational Fire Engineering Techniques and Experimental Data, CMS 04/IM/ 115, CMS Press. [133] Weinspach, P.,M., Gundlach,J., Klingelhofer,H.,G., Ries, R. And Schneider, U.(1997), Analysis Of The Fire On April 11th, 1996, Recommendations And Consequences For Dusseldorf Rhein-Ruhr-Airport, Staatskanzlei Nordrhein-Wstfalen, Mannesmannufer 1 A, 40190 Dusseldorf, Germany. [134] Berl, W. G., and Halpin, B. M. , (1979), Human fatalities from unwanted fires. Fire J 73: 105-115. [135] Berl, W. G., and Halpin, B. M.(1997), Fire-related fatalities: an analysis of their demography, physical origins, and medical causes. In Fire standards and safety. ASTM STP 614, edited by A. F. Robertson, American Society for Testing and Materials, Washington, DC, pp 26-54. [136] FEMA (2003), Establishing a Relationship Between Alcohol and Casualties of Fire. Federal Emergency Management Agency. Topical Fire Research Series 3:3. [137] Leth,P.(1998), Gregersen, M., and Sabroe, S., Fatal Residential Fire Accidents in the Municipality of Copenhagen, 1991–19961PREVENTIVE MEDICINE 27, 444–451. [138] Runyan, C. W., Bangidiwala, S.I., Linzer, M.A., Sacks, J.J. & Butts, J.(1992), Risk Factors for Fatal Residential Fires. New England J. Med. 327. 859-863. [139] Mallonee S, Istre GR, Rosenberg M, et al.(1996), Surveillance and prevention of residential-fire injuries. N Engl J Med 1996;335:27–31. [140] Karter M J (1986) Patterns of fire deaths among the elderly and children in the home. Fire Journal, March,19- 22. [141] Marshall, S.W., Runyan, C.W., Bangdiwala, S.I., Linzer, M.A., Sacks, J.J., and Butts, J.D.(1998), Fatal Residential Fires: Who Dies and Who Survives?, JAMA, May 27, 1998 – Vol 279, No. 20. [142] Ballard JE, Koepsell TD, Rivara F. Association of smoking and alcohol drinking with residential fire injuries. Am J Epidemiol 1992;135:26–34, 1992. [143] Howland J, Hingson R. (1987), Alcohol as a risk factor for injuries or death due to fires and burns: review of the literature. Public Health Rep 1987;102:475–83. [144] Istre, G.R., McCoy, M.A., Osborn, L., Barnard, J.J., and Bolton, A.(2001), Deaths and Injuries from House Fires, NEJM 2001;344:1911-1916. [145] Hokugo, A., Tumura, A., Murosaki, Y.(2001), An Investigation on Proportion and Capabilities of Disabled People at Shopping Centers for Fire Safety, Proceedings of the Second International Symposium on Human Behaviour in Fire, pp.167-174, Interscience communications. [146] Hirschler,M. And Christian,S.(1996),Fires And The Elderly. Fatalities During Residential Fires In The UK:1982-1984, Proceedings Of The Seventh International Fire Science And Engineering Conference, Interflam'96, Ed:Franks, C. and Grayson, S., pp777-791, ISBN 0951632094, 26-28/3/1996.

111

[147] DiGuiseppi, C., Roberts, I., Wade, A., Sculpher, M., Edwards, P., Godward, C., Pan, H., Slater, S.(2002), Incidence of fires and related injuries after giving out free smoke alarms: cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ 325: 995-995. [148] DiGuiseppi, C., Slater, S., Roberts, I., Adams, L., Sculpher, M., Wade, A., McCarthy, M.(1999),The "Let's Get Alarmed!" initiative: a smoke alarm giveaway programme. Inj Prev 5: 177-182. [149] DiGuiseppi, C., Higgins, J. P T.(2000), Systematic review: Systematic review of controlled trials of interventions to promote smoke alarms. Arch. Dis. Child. 82: 341-348 [150] DiGuiseppi, C., Roberts, I., Speirs, N. (1999). Smoke alarm installation and function in inner London council housing. Arch. Dis. Child. 81: 400-403 [151] Scholer, S.J., Hickson, G.B., Mitchel Jr., E.F.(1998), Ray, W.A., Predictors of Mortality From Fires in Young Children, Pediatrics, 1998, 101, 12-. [152] Scholer, S., Hickson, G.B., & Ray, W.A (1998). Sociodemographic Factors Identify US Infants at High Risk of Injury Mortality. Pediatrics. 103(6): 1183-1188. [153] Warda, L., Tenenbein, M., Moffatt, M.E., (1999), House fire injury prevention update. Part I. A review of risk factors for fatal and non-fatal house fire injury, Injury Prevention; 5:145-150. [154] Barillo D.J., Goode R.(1996), Fire fatality study: demographics of fire victims. Burns 1996;22:85–8. [155] Barillo D.J., Goode R.(1996), Substance abuse in victims of fire. J Care Rehabil. 1996;17:71–76 [156] Chernichko L, Saunders LD, Tough S.(1993), Unintentional house fire deaths in Alberta 1985–1990: a population study. Can J Public Health 1993;84:317–20. [157] Conway, G. A., et al.(1985),Deaths associated with fires, burns, and explosions-New Mexico, 1978-1983. MMWR 34: 623-625, Oct. 11. [158] Cruickshanks, K.J., Wiley, T.L., Tweed, T.S., Klein, B.E.K., Klein, R., Mares- Perlman, J.A., Nondahl, D.M., (1998) “Prevalence of hearing loss in older adults in Beaver Dam, Wisconsin: The Epidemiology of Hearing Loss Study,” American Journal of Epidemiology, Vol. 148, No. 9. [159] Deaths associated with fires, burns, and explosions—New Mexico, 1978–1983. Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 1985;34:623– 5, 1985. [160] Gates, G.A., Cooper, Jr., J.C., Kannel, W.B., Miller, N.J., (1990) Hearing in the elderly: The Framingham Cohort, 1983–1985, Part I. Basic audiometric test results, Ear and Hearing, Vol. 11, No. 4, 1990. [161] Gerson, L.(1979), Fire deaths and drinking: data from the Ontario fire reporting system. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 6: 125-133.. [162] Gorman R, Charney E, Holtzman N, et al.(1985), A successful city-wide smoke detector giveaway program. Pediatrics 1985; 75: 14-18. [163] Gulaid J, Sattin R, Waxweiler R.(1988), Deaths from residential fires, 1978–1984. Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 1988;37(SS-1): 39–45. [164] Haddix, A C, Mallonee, S, Waxweiler, R, Douglas, M R (2001). Cost effectiveness analysis of a smoke alarm giveaway program in Oklahoma City, Oklahoma. Inj Prev 7: 276-281. [165] Mierley, M.C., & Baker, S.P. (1983). Fatal House Fires in an Urban Population. J. American Medical Association. 249: 1466-1468. [166] Patetta M.J., Cole T.B.(1990), A population-based descriptive study of housefire deaths in North Carolina. Am J Public Health 1990;80:1116–7. [167] Ploeg, J., Black, M.E., Hutchinson, B.G., Walter, S.D., Scott, E.A.F., and Chambers, L.W.(1994), Personal, home, and community safety promotion with community-dwelling elderly persons: response to a public health nurse intervention, Canadian Journal of Public Health, 1994; 85: pp. 188-191. [168] Rhodes, A. & Reinholtd, S. (1998). Residential Fire Fatalities: A Study of Factors Contributing to Residential Fire Fatalities and their Prevention. Mount Waverley Victoria; Country Fire Authority. [169] Rowland, D., and DiGuiseppi, C., Roberts, I., Curtis, K., Roberts, H., Ginnelly, L., Sculpher, M., Wade, A., Prevalence of working smoke alarms in local authority inner city housing: randomised controlled trial, BMJ VOLUME 325 2 NOVEMBER 20 [170] Shai, D., and Lupinacci, P. (2003). Fire Fatalities Among Children: An Analysis Across Philadelphia’s Census Tracts. Public Health Reports. 118: 115-126. [171] Beller, D., and Sapochetti,J.(2000), Searching for Answers to the Cocoanut Grove Fire of 1942, NFPA Journal, May/June. [172] Drabek, T.E.(1969), Social Processes in Disaster: Family Evacuation, Social Problems, 16, Winter,: 336-349. [173] Furukawa, Y., Ohyama, Y., Hasemi, Y.(2003), Statistics and Investigations of Fire Incidents in “Silver Peer” Collective Housings for Senior Citizens in Tokyo Metropolitan District—Disaster Prevention Planning for

112

Elderly Residents in Apartment Buildings, Bulletin of the Japan Association for Fire Science and Engineering, Vol.53 No.2, pp.39-48 (in Japanese). [174] Ashley, E.(2004), Waking Effectiveness o Emergency Alerting Devices For The Aged And Hard Of Hearing. Combustion Science and Engineering. [175] Ashley, E., Du Bois, J., (2005) Waking Effectiveness of Audible, Visual and Vibratory Emergency Alarms Across All Hearing Levels,” Fire Suppression and Detection Research Application Symposium – Proceedings of the 9th Annual Symposium, Orlando, FL. [176] Ball, M.,Bruck, D.(2004), The Effect of Alcohol Upon Response to Fire Alarm Signals in Sleeping Young Adults, Human Behavior in Fire – Proceedings of the Third International Symposium, September 1-3, 2004, Belfast, N. Ireland, Interscience Communications Ltd., London, England. [177] Ball, M.,Bruck, D.(2004), The Salience of Fire Alarm Signals for Sleeping Individuals: A Novel Approach to Signal Design, Human Behavior in Fire – Proceedings of the Third International Symposium, pp 303-314, September 1-3, 2004, Belfast, N. Ireland, Interscience Communications Ltd., London, England. [178] Berkowitz, J.P. and Casali, S.P. (1990), Influence of Age on the Ability to Hear Telephone Ringers of Different Spectral Content, Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 34th Annual Meeting, Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society, 132-136. [179] Bixler, E.(1975), Emergencies: Arousal from Sleep. Proceedings of Fire Detection for Life Safety Symposium. [180] Bonnet, M., Laverne, C.(1978), The Reliability of Arousal Threshold During Sleep. Psychopharmacology. [181] Bonnet M.H .(1979), The effectiveness of smoke detectors in awakening sleep subjects . Sleep Res 1979 :8 :158. [182] Bonnett, M.H. and Horasan, M.(1995), Non-Arousal and Non-Action of Normal Sleepers in Response to a Smoke Detector Alarm, Fire Safety Journal, 25,125-139. [183] Bowman, S.(1995), Waking Effectiveness of Visual Alerting Systems. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development. [184] Bowman, S.K., Jamieson D.G. & Ogilvie, R.D. (1995) Waking effectiveness of visual alerting signals. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development, 32 (1), 43-62. [185] Brant, L.J. and Fozard, J.L.(1989), Age changes in pure-tone hearing thresholds in a longitudinal study of normal human aging”, J. Acoustic Soc. Am. 88, 2, 813 – 820. [186] Breznitz, S.(1984), Cry Wolf: The Psychology of False Alarms, Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. [187] Bruck, D.(1998), Arousal from Sleep with a Smoke Detector in Children and Adults, Fire Code Reform Research Program, Fire Code Reform Centre Limited. [188] Bruck, D.(1995), Non-Arousal and Non-Action of Normal Sleepers in Response to a Smoke Detector Alarm, Fire Safety Journal, 25, pp. 125-139. [189] Bruck, D.(1999), Non-Awakening in Children in Response to a Smoke Detector Alarm, Fire Safety Journal, 32, pp. 369-376. [190] Bruck, D., Brennan, P., (2001) Recognition of fire cues during sleep, Human Behaviour in Fire – Proceedings of the Second International Symposium, March 26-28, 2001, Boston, MA, Interscience Communications Ltd., London, England. [191] Bruck, D., Bliss, A.(2000), Sleeping Children and Smoke Alarms, 4th Asia-Oceania Symposium on Fire Science and Technology. [192] Bruck, D. The Effectiveness of Different Alarms in Waking Sleeping Children, Victoria University. [193] Bruck, D., Thomas, I., Kritikos, A., (2006) Investigation of auditory arousal with different alarm signals in sleeping older adults, Draft Report, Victoria University, Australia, April 2006. [194] Bruck, D., et al(2004), The Effectiveness of Different Alarms in Waking Sleeping Children, Human Behavior in Fire – Proceedings of the Third International Symposium, September 1-3, 2004, Belfast, N. Ireland, Interscience Communications Ltd., London, England. [195] Bruck,D., and Bliss, R.A.(2000), Sleeping Children and Smoke Alarms, Proceedings of the 4th Asio-Oceania Symposium on Fire Science and Technology, Tokyo, May. [196] Busby, K.(1994), Ontogenetic Variations in Auditory Arousal Threshold During Sleep, Psychophysiology, 31, pp. 182-188. [197] Bruck, D. and Horasan, M.(1995), Non-arousal and non-action of normal sleepers in response to a smoke detector alarm. Fire Safety Journal 25: 125-139. [198] Busboom, M. and May, M.(1999), Mobile navigation for the blind. Proceeding of the International Conference on Wearable Computing, Vienna, Austria.

113

[199] Busby, K. and Pivik, R.T.(1984), Auditory Arousal Thresholds During Sleep in Hyperkinetic Children, Sleep, 8,4,332-341. [200] Carskadon, M., and Herz, R.S., (2004) Minimal olfactory perception during sleep: Why odor alarms will not work for humans, Sleep, Vol. 27, 2004, pp. 402-405. [201]Carskadon MA, Bigler PJ, Can J, et al .(1990), Olfactory arousal thresholds during sleep. Sleep Res 1990 :19 :147. [202] Cherpitel, C.J. (1992), The Epidemiology of Alcohol Related Trauma. Alcohol, Health and Research World. 1992. 16(3):191-196. [203] De Voss, F., (1991) ,Report of Research on Emergency Signaling Devices for Use by the Hearing Impaired, Underwriters Laboratories, Northbrook, IL. [204] De Voss, F., (1989), Report of the Meeting of the Ad Hoc Advisory Group of UL for Emergency Signaling for Use by the Hearing Impaired, Underwriters Laboratories, Northbrook, IL. [205] Dean, J.C., Underwriters Laboratories Report on Smoke Alarms Waking Children Concludes – Further Research Is Necessary, A Message From State Fire Marshal. [206] Delgado JL, Israelson T, Lindstrom J, Skjetne J, Welsby K .(1992), Awareness of alarm signal for hard of hearing, deaf, and deaf-blind people. In : von Tetzchner S, Issues in telecommunication and Sleep Res 1992 :21 :143. [207] Department for Communities and Local Government, 58/1994: Reliability and Effectiveness of Domestic Smoke Alarms, 1994 http://www.communities.gov.uk/index.asp?id=1124884 [208] Carter, M.T., Kendall, S., and Clark, J.P.(1983), Household Response to Warnings, International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters, 9,1,94-104. [209] Desai, Mayur, et al.(2001), Trends in Vision and Hearing Among Older Americans, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics, Aging Trends No. 2, March. [210] DuBois, J.(2005), Waking Effectiveness of Audible, Visual, and Vibratory Emergency Alarms on People of All Hearing Abilities, Accessible Emergency Notification and Communciation: State of the Science Conference. [211] Duncan, C.(1999), The Effectiveness of the Domestic Smoke Alarm Signal, University of Canterbury. [212] Elder A.T., Squires T., Busuttil A. (1996), Fire fatalities in elderly people. Age Ageing 1996;25:214–6. [213] Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics(2004), Older Americans 2004: Key Indicators of Well- Being. November 2004. [214] Fire Safety Council(2006), Home Smoke Alarms and Other Fire Detection and Alarm Equipment, April. [215] Geiman, J.A. and Gottuk,D.T.(2006), Reducing Fire Deaths in Older Adults: Optimizing the Smoke Alarm Signal, Prepared for The Fire Protection Research Foundation, April. [216] Grace, T.(1997), Improving the Waking Effectiveness of Fire Alarms in Residential Areas, University of Canterbury. [217] Graham, R .A. (1998). A Closer Look at Domestic Fire Deaths. (In Human Behaviour in Fire: Proceedings of the 1st International Symposium, Belfast. Belfast: Textflow). [218] Guettinger H., and Scheidweiller, A.(1989),New Ways towards a Solution of the False Alarm Problem, Proceedings Of The Second International Symposium On Fire Safety Science, Ed: Wakamatsu, T., Hasemi, Y., Seizawa, A., Seeger, P., Pagni, P., And Grant, C., pp583-590, ISBN 0-891168648. [219] Gulaid,J., Sacks, J.J., and Sattin, R.W.(1984), Deaths from residential fires among older people, J. Am. Geriatrix Soc., 1983;37;331-334, U.S.. [220] Guth, D.A.; Rieser, J.J.(1997), Perception and the control of locomotion by blind and visually impaired pedestrians. Foundations of Orientation and Mobility, (Second Edition), AFB Press, pp. 9-38. [221] Hall J.R.(1990), The elderly, the sick and health care facilities. Fire J 1990;84:33–42. [222] Hall, J.(2006), Estimating the Estimating the Impact on Risk of Fire Death for Older Adults From Changes in Waking Effectiveness (for Older Adults) of Smoke Alarms, NFPA Fire Analysis & Research Division, January 18. [223] Halliwell R.E., Sultan M.A.(1989), A method to determine the optimum location for fire alarms in residential buildings. Can Acoustics 1989 :17(2) :9-18. [224] Harvey P.A., Aitken M., Ryan G.W., Demeter L.A, Givens J., Sundararaman R., Goulette S. (2004), Strategies to increase smoke alarm use in high-risk households. J Comm Health 2004; 29: 375-85. [225] Hollis, W.S.(1973), Drinking:Its part in Fire Deaths, Fire Journal, pp10-12, May.

114

[226] Kahn, M.J.(1984), Human Awakening and Subsequent Detection of Fire-Related Cues, Fire Technology, 20,1, pp20-26. [227] Kawakami, M., et al(1990), Chronic Ehanol Exposure Before Injury Produces Greater Immune Dysfunction After Thermal Injury in Rats, Journal of Trauma, 1990, Jan; 30(1) 27-31. Keay, DG and Murray, JAM (1988), “Hearing loss in the elderly: a 17-year longitudinal study”, Clinical Otolaryngology, 13, 31-35. [228] Klote, J. H., Nelson, H. E., Deal, S. and Levin, B. M., (1992), Staging Areas for Persons with Mobility Limitations, prepared for the Office of Real Property Management andSafety, NISTIR 4770, US Department of Commerce Technology Administration, Gaithersburg, MD, 179 p. [229] Kose, S.(1998), Emergence of Aged Populace: Who Is at Higher Risk in Fires?, Proceedings of the First International Symposium on Human Behaviour in Fire, pp.333-339. [230] Lahr, E.J.( 1985), Fire Safety for the Hearing Impaired, Fire Chief Magazine, Communications Channels Inc., Atlanta, GA, June, pp 37-39,. [231] Langford, G. W., Meddis, R., & Pearson, A. J. D. (1974). Awakening latency from sleep for meaningful and non-meaningful stimuli. Psychophysiology, 11, 1-5. [232] Lee, A., (2005) The Audibility of Smoke Alarms in Residential Homes, CPSC-ES-0503, U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission, Washington, DC, 2005. Available online: [233] Lee, A., (2005), Possible Technologies for Improving the Audibility of Residential Smoke Alarms for Older Adults, CPSC-ES-0505, U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission, Washington, DC, 2005. [234] Levere, T.E.(1974), Arousal from Sleep: The Differential Effect of Frequencies Equated for Loudness, Physiology and Behavior, 12, pp. 573-582. [235] LeVere T.E., Bartus R.T., Morlock G.W., Hart F.D.(1973), Arousal From Sleep: Responsiveness to Different Auditory Frequencies Related to Loudness.Physiological Behaviour. (1973), vol 10, pp 53 - 57. [236] LeVere T.E., Morlock G.W., Thomas L.P. and Hart F.D.(1974), Arousal From Sleep: The Differential Effect of Frequencies Equatedfor Loudness, Physiology and Behaviour. (1974), vol 12, pp 573 - 582. [237] LeVere T.E., Davis N., Mills J. and Berger E.(1976), Arousal From Sleep: The Effects of the Cognitive Value of Auditory Stimuli. Physiological Psychology. (1976), vol 4, no 3, pp 376 - 382. [238] Loomis, J. M., Golledge, R. G., & Klatzky, R. L. (1998). Navigation system for the blind: Auditory display modes and guidance. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7, 193-203. [239] Lynch, J.L. (1998),Nocturnal Olfactory Response to Smoke Odor, Human Behavior in Fire – Proceedings of the First International Symposium, August 31-September 2, 1998, Fire SERT Centre, University of Ulster, 1998, pp. 231 – 242. [240] McConnell, C.F., Dwyer, W.O., and Leeming, F.C.(1996),A behavioral approach to reducing fires in public housing,” Journal of Community Psychology, 1996; 24: pp. 201-212. [241] Marriot, M. D. (1995) Reliability and Effectiveness of Domestic Smoke Alarms, Fire Engineers Journal, Volume 55 Part 177, 10-12. [242] Mitchell, D. S., Packham, S. C., and Fitzgerald,W. E. (1978), Effects of ethanol and carbon monoxide on two measures of behavioral incapacitation of rats. Proc West Pharmacol Soc 21: 427-431. [243] Murphy TI, Alloway CED, LaMarche CH, Bernstein DM, Ogilvie RD, MacLean AW and Jamieson DG (1995) How reliably does a vibro-tactile smoke alarm awaken individuals with hearing loss? Sleep Research, 24A [244] National Institute on Aging and National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, (2002), Alcohol Use and Abuse,, May. [245] NFPA, (2006) Smoke Alarms For People Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing, Center for High Risk Outreach, National Fire Protection Association, Quincy, MA, 23 Oct 2006. Available online: http://www.nfpa.org/assets/files//PDF/Public%20Education/Alarmguidedeaf.pdf> [246] Nober, H.E., Well,A.D. and Moss, S. (1995), Does Light Work as Well as Sound? Smoke Alarms for the Hearing Impaired, Fire Journal, Vol. 84, No.1, National Fire Protection Association, Boston, MA, pp 26-30. [247] Nober H.E.(1983), Waking Effectiveness of Household Smoke and Fire Detection Devices, Final Report, Bureau of Fire Research, National Bureau of Standards. [248] Nober EH, Well A, Moss S .(1990), Fire safety protection program for the severely hearing impaired . In : Proceedings of the 5th Annual Conference on Technology and Persons with Disabilities, 1990 :473-83. [249] Nober, E.H., Peirce, H., and Well, A. (1981),Acoustic Spectral Characteristics of Household Smoke Detector Alarms, FIRE JOURNAL, Voi. 75, No. 3 (May 1981), p. 94.

115

[250] Nober, E.H., Peirce, H., and Well, A.,Johnson, C. and Clifton, C.,(1983) Waking Effectiveness of Household Smoke and Fire Detector Devices, National Bureau of Standards, Center for Fire Research, September 1980,75 pp. Revised final report, August 1983, 78 pp. [251] Fidell, S.(1979), Evaluation of Effectiveness of Residential Fire Detection System Audible Warning Signals, 83rd National Fire Protection Association Annual Meeting. [252] Notake, H., Sekizawa, A., Kobayashi, M., Mammoto, A., Ebihara, M.(2004), Study on Measures for Mitigating the Risk of Residential Fires and Fire Fatalities, Proceedings of the Sixth Asia Oceania Symposium on Fire Safety and Technology, pp.500-511, Daegu, Republic of Korea. [253] ODPM Report. (2002). Establishing Fire Safety Issues Among Older People. Office of the Deputy Prime Minister: London. [254] Ogilvie RD, Hunt HT, Sawicki C, Samahalskyi J . Psychological old and pattern in young adult good and poor sleepers . Sleep Res 1977 :6 :128. [255] Oswald, I., Taylor, A. M. & Treisman, M.(1960)., Discriminitive responses to stimulation during human sleep. Brain, 83 (1960) 440-52. [256] Pederson, K.E.; Rosenhall, U.; and Moller, M.B. (1989), Changes in Pure-Tone Thresholds in Individuals Aged 70-81: Results from a Longitudinal Study, Audiology, 28, 194 – 204. [257] Peek-Asa, C, Allareddy, V, Yang, J, Taylor, C, Lundell, J, Zwerling, C (2005). When one is not enough: prevalence and characteristics of homes not adequately protected by smoke alarms. Inj Prev 11: 364-368 [258] Petraglia, J.S. (1991). Older Adults Stand Out as One of the Largest Groups in the United States at Risk of Dying in a Fire. National Fire Protection Agency Journal. Quincy, MA. [259] Pezoldt, V.J.(1978), Arousal from Sleep by Emergency Alarms: Implications from the Scientific Literature, National Institute of Standards and Technology, NISTIR 78-1484, 1978. [260] Poeppelmeyer, D.(2004), Emergency Notification for the Texas School for the Deaf, Captioned Media Program, U.S. Department of Education, August. [261] Roberts, I. (1995). Deaths of children in house fires. BMJ 311: 1381-1382 [262] Roberts I, Power C.(1996), Does the decline in child injury death rates vary by social class? BMJ 1996;313:784–6. [263] Rowland, D., Afolabi, E., & Roberts, I. (2002). Prevention of Deaths and Injuries Caused by House Fires. J. Pub. Health Medicine. 24 (3):217-218. [264] Royal Society of Medicine (2000),Side Effects of ‘Everyday’ Drugs in Elderly Can Be Mistaken for Dementia, September . [265] Rudabiri, L., and Roberts, J.P.(1994), Evacuating Workers with Disabilities, The Safety and Health Practitioner, Institution of Occupational Safety and Health, pp21-24, UK. [266] Sekizawa, A.(2004), Care of Vulnerable Populations: Who are vulnerable to fires and what care is needed?, Human Behavior in Fire – Proceedings of the Third International Symposium, pp 267-278, September 1-3, 2004, Belfast, N. Ireland, Interscience Communications Ltd., London, England. [267] Self Help for Hard of Hearing People (SHHH), (2006), Facts on Hearing Loss, 6 Feb. 2006, [268] Shapiro, J. Recorded Parental Voice as a Method of Awakening and Evacuating Children, Preliminary Report of Pilot Test Data. [269] Shapiro, J. The Potential of Recorded Caregiver Voice as an Auditory Signal in Smoke Alarms. [270] Squires, T., & Busuttil, A. (1996). Can Child Fatalities in House Fires be Prevented? Injury Prevention. 2(2): 109-113. [271] Squires T, Busuttil A.(1996), Can child fatalities in house fires be prevented? Inj Prev 1996;2:109–13. [272] Stocker, W.(2006), SO What Do You Expect? Fire Protection Engineering, SFPE, Winter. [273] Taylor, B., Manifold, T., & Lodge, J. (2001). Towards Developing a Picture of Those Most at Risk of Death by Fire. (In Human Behaviour in Fire: Proceedings of the 2nd International Symposium, Boston. London: Interscience). [274] Thomas, I., & Brennan, P.(2004), Occupants, Ignition and Fire Outcomes, Human Behaviour in Fire, Proceedings of the 3rd International Symposium, Belfast. Interscience, London. [275] Thomas, I., Brennan, P.(2002), Injuries and Fatalities in Apartment Building Fires, Fire Safety Science-- Proceedings of the Seventh International Symposium, pp.1085-1096. [276] Thompson, N.J., Waterman, M.B., and Sleet, D.A.(2004), Using behavioral science to improve fire escape behaviors in response to a smoke alarm, Journal of Burn Care and Rehabilitation, 2004; 25(2): pp. 179-188. [277] Tridata Corporation(1999), Fire Risks for the Deaf or Hard of Hearing, United States Fire Administration.

116

[278] Underwriters Laboratories(1991), Report of research on emergency signaling devices for use by the hearing impaired (Subject 1971). [279] Underwriters Laboratories. Report from the UL Smoke Alarm STP Research Group on Childhood Awakening in Response to Smoke Alarms. [280] Underwriters Laboratories, Inc.(2002), UL 1971, Signaling Devices for the Hearing Impaired, NorthBrook, IL. [281] United States Fire Administration (1999), Fire Risks for the Blind or Visually Impaired, FA-205, United States Fire Administration/National Fire Data Center, Emmitsburg, MD, December. Available online: [282] United States Fire Administration (1999), Fire Risks for the Mobility Impaired, FA-204, United States Fire Administration/National Fire Data Center, Emmitsburg, MD, October 1999. Available online: [283] United States Fire Administration (1999),Risks for the Deaf or Hard of Hearing, FA-202, United States Fire Administration/National Fire Data Center, Emmitsburg, MD, December 1999. Available online: [284] United States Fire Administration, (2006) Fire and the Older Adult, FA-300, United States Fire Administration/National Fire Data Center, Emmitsburg, MD, 2006. Available online: [285] Vondrasek, R.J., (1989) Fire Alarm and Detection Systems for the Hearing Impaired, United States Fire Administration, Emmitsburg, MD. [286] Waldrop, J. (2003), Disability Status: 2000, United States Census Bureau. [287] Wallace, M.S.; Ashman, M.D., M.S., M.N.; and Matjasko, M.D., M.J. (1994), Hearing Acuity of Anesthesiologists and Alarm Detection, Anesthesiology 81, 13-28. [288] Waller, P.F., Stewart, J.R.,. Hansen, A.R , Stutts, J.C., Popkin, C.L., and Rodgman, E.A.(1986), The Potentiating Effects of Alcohol on Driver Injury. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1986. Sep 19;256(11):1461-6 [289] Webb PA. (1992), Smoke alarms and signalling systems for deaf and hard of hearing people . Care Saf Q 1992:3 :2 [290] White, A.C. (1983), Pre-accident medication and alcohol in burn and other accident victims. J Clin Hosp Pharm 8: 301-303. [291] Wiegand L, Latz B, Hartel M, Wiegand D .(1992), Arousal frequency varies with sleep stage. Sleep Res 1992 :21 :124. [292] Wilkinson, R. and Stretton, M.(1971), Performance After Awakening at Different Times of Night, Psychon. Sci., Vol 23, 283-285. [293] Wilson WP, Zung WK.(1966), Attention, discrimination and arousal during sleep. Archives General Psychiatry, 1966; 15: 523-8. [294] Williams, H.L., and Salamy, A.(1972), Alcohol and sleep, (in Kissin, B., and Begleiter, H.,eds) The Biology of Alcoholism, Volume 2, Physiology and Behavior, New York: Plenum. [295] Zepelin, H.(1984), Effects of Age on Auditory Awakening Thresholds, Journal of Gerontology, 39,294-300. [296] Zung, W.W.K. and Wilson, W.P.(1961), Response to Auditory Stimulation During Sleep, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry, 4, 548-552. [297] Lee,A., Midgett, J., and White,S.(2004), Sound Effectiveness of Residential Smoke Alarms,CPSC-ES0502, U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission, Washington, DC. [298] U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission(2004), A Review Of The Sound Effectiveness Of Residential Smoke Alarms, Cpsc-Es-0502, Washington, D.C. 20207-0001 December 2004 (Revised) [299] Bellamy, L.J.(1990), Experimental Programme To Investigate Information Warning Characteristics For Motivating Fast Evacuations, Building Research Establishment. [300] Bellamy, L.J, Geyer, T.A.W., Max-Lino, R., Harrison, P.I., Bahrami, Z. And Modha, B.(1988),An Evaluation Of The Effectiveness Of The Components Of Informative Fire Warning Systems, Safety In The Built Environment, Ed:Sime,J., pp36-47,Spon, ISBN 0-419-14480-3. [301] Benthorn,L. and Frantzich, H.,Fire Alarm in a Public Building: How Do People Evaluate Information and Choose an Evacuation Exit, BRANDFORSK, LUND, June 1996 [302] Berry, C. H. (1978) Will Your Smoke Detector Wake You?, Fire Journal, July,105-108. [303] Blasch, B. , (1991) Environmental information needs for wayfinding. VA RR&D Final Reports (Atlanta VA Rehab R&D Center).

117

[304] Crandall, W.; Bentzen, B.L.; Myers, L.; Mitchel, P. (1995),Transit accessibility improvement through talking signs remote infrared signage: A demonstration and evaluation. Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute RERC, San Francisco, CA. [305] Douglas MR, Mallonee S, Istre GR.(1998), Comparison of community based smoke detector distribution methods in an urban community. Inj Prev 1998; 4: 28-32. [306] Fire Research Station (1990), Experimental Programme To Investigate Informative Fire Warning Characteristics For Motivating Fast Evacuation, Br172, Building Research Establishment, UK,. [307] Geoffroy, N.A.(2005), Measuring Speech Intelligibility in Voice Alarm Communication Systems, Master of Science Thesis, WPI. [308] Geyer, , Bellamy, L., Max-Lino, R.,Harrison, Bahrami, Z. And Modha, B.(1988), An Evaluation Of The Effectiveness Of The Components Of Information Fire Warning Systems, In Sime J.,(Ed) Safety In The Built Environment, E And Fn Spon, pp36-47. [309] Grace, T., Woodger, N., & Olsson, P. (2001). On the use of voice alarms. Human Behaviour in Fire- Proceedings of the Second International Symposium, Shields J (Ed.), Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 185-196. [310] Gruntfest, E.C., Warning Dissemination and Response with Short Lead Times, in Handmer, J. (ed), Flood Hazard Management: British and International Perspectives, pp191-202, Geo Books,UK. [311] Hansen AS, Hovden J.(1998), Do smoke detectors and portable extinguishers in residents affect fire safety? In: Shields J, editor. Human Behaviour in FireFProceedings of the First International Symposium, Fire SERT Centre, University of Ulster, 1998. p. 135–43. [312] Heimdall Consulting Ltd.(2005), Human Behavior Contributing to Unintentional Fire Deaths 1997-2003, February. [313] Hellier E, Edworthy J, Weedon B, Walters K, Adams A.(2002), The perceived urgency of speech warnings: Semantics versus acoustics. Human Factors, 2002; 44(1), 1-6. [314] Holburn, S. C. and Dougher, M. J., (1985), The Fire-Alarm Game: Exit Training Using Negative and Positive Reinforcement Under Varied Stimulus Conditions, Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, Vol. 79, Nov., The American Foundation for the Blind, New York, NY, pp. 401-403. [315] Huey, R.W.; Buckly, D.S. and Lerner, N.D. (1994), Audible Performance of Smoke Alarm Sounds, Designing for an Aging Population, 6-10. [316] Jacob, K.(2001), Understanding Speech Intelligibility and the Fire Alarm Code, NFPA. [317] Kozeki, D., Satoh, K., Tamura, H., Hosokawa, M.(1996), Study Of Evacuation Assisting System Incorporated With Intelligent Fire Detection System, Fire Research Institute, Japan, University Of Science And Technology Of China., Proceedings Of The Seventh International Fire Science And Engineering Conference, Interflam'96, Ed:Franks, C. and Grayson, S., pp731-740, ISBN 0951632094, 26-28/3/1996. [318] Mande, I.(1975), A Standard Fire Alarm Signal Temporal or a Slow Whoop, Fire Journal, Vol. 69, No. 6, 1975, pp. 25-28. [319] Morales, F., (2001) Network Audio-Link Fire Alarm Monitoring System and Method, U.S. Patent No. 6,215,404, April 10, 2001. [320] Myles, M.(1979), Analysis if Acoustic Signals Produced by Residential Fire Alarms, 83rd Annual Meeting, St. Louis, MO. [321] Myles, M. and Fidell, S.(1978), Evaluation of the Detectability of Residential Fire Alarms, Bolt Beranek and Newman Report No. 3833. [322] National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion (2002), Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System, Online Prevalence Data. [323] NEMA(2004), Fire Alarm Systems and Interior Finish, Fire Protection Engineering, Fall. [324] NEMA(2006), Visual Signaling in Large Spaces Part I, Fire Protection Engineering, Summer. [325] NEMA(2006), Visual Signaling in Large Spaces Part II, Fire Protection Engineering, Fall. [326] Nigg, J.M.(1987), Communication and Behavior: Organizational and Individual Response to Warnings, in Dynes, R.R. et al, Sociology of Disasters, pp103-117, Italy. [327] Perry, R.W., Greene, M., and Lindell, M.K.(1980), Enhancing Evacuation Warning Compliance: Suggestions for Emergency Planning, Disasters, 4, 4, 443-449. [328] Robinson, D. A. (1988) Sound Attenuation in Buildings: Implications for Fire Alarm System Design, Fire Safety Journal, 14,512. [329] Schifiliti, R.P.(2006), Direct Visual Signaling as a Means for Occupant Notification in Large Spaces, Technical Report, Fire Protection Research Foundation, January. [330] Schifiliti, R.P.(2006), Understanding and Applying 15/75 cd eff. Strobes, RPSA Fire Protection Engineering.

118

[331] Street, T.T., Williams, F.W., (2003),The Implementation and Demonstration of Wireless Communications Capabilities in Off-The-Shelf, Battery Powered Smoke Alarms, Phase II Report, Naval Research Laboratory, Navy Technology Center for Safety and Survivability Chemistry Division, December. [332] Tamura, H., Kozeki, D., Takanashi, K.(2004), Development of Fire Alarm Device for People with Hearing Difficulties”, Poster Presentation, the Third International Symposium on Human Behaviour in Fire. [333] Thompson CJ, Jones AR, Davis MK, Caplan LS. (2004),Do smoke alarms still function a year after installation? A follow-up of the Get-Alarmed Campaign. J Comm Health 2004; 29: 171-181. [334] U.S. Consumer Product Safety Commission(2005), The Audibility Of Smoke Alarms In Residential Homes, , Cpsc-Es-0503, Washington, D.C. 20207-0001 January. [335] U.S. Food and Drug Administration(2003), FDA Consumer Magazine, September–October 1997,revised January 1999, August 2000, November 2002, and September 2003. [336] Woolley, W.D. ,(1985) A Scientist's View Of The Fire At Bradford City Football Stadium, Handout Notes Given At The Research Colloquium, Fire Research Station, 6 November [337] UL 217(2004), Standard for Safety Single and Multiple Station Smoke Alarms, Fifth Edition, Dated February 21, 1997, Revisions through January 15, 2004, Underwriters Laboratories, Northbrook, IL. [338] ADA Standards for Accessible Design(1994). [339] Simplex TrueAlert Text MEssagin Appliances, S4907-0001-0.5( 2007). [340] RoamSecure - http://www.roamsecure.net/content.php?contentID=65 [341] Personal communication John Hall (2007). [342] Bruck,D., Thomas,I., and Ball, M. (2007), Waking effectiveness of alarms (auditory, visual and tactile) for the alcohol impaired , Prepared for the Fire Protection Research Foundation, NFPA, Victoria University, Australia [343] Bruck,D., and Thomas,I. (2007), Waking effectiveness of alarms (auditory, visual and tactile) for adults who are hard of hearing , Prepared for the Fire Protection Research Foundation, NFPA, Victoria University, Australia [344] National Fire Alarm Code, NFPA 72, National Fire Protection Association, 2007 edition. [345] Personal communication Ian Thomas (2007). [346] Notake, H.,Sekizawa, A.(2004), How to Save Lives of Vulnerable People from Residential Fires?, Human Behavior in Fire – Proceedings of the Third International Symposium, pp 315-326, September 1-3, 2004, Belfast, N. Ireland, Interscience Communications Ltd., London, England. [347]Prinzo, O.V.(1998), An Analysis of Voice Communication in a Simulated Approach Control Environment, DOT/FAA/am-98/17, Office of Aviation Medicine, Federal Aviation Authority, United States Department of Transportation, May. [348] Ross, D.A., and Blasch, B.B.(2000), Wearable Interfaces for Orientation and Wayfinding, ACM SIGACCESS Conference on Assistive Technologies, Proceedings of the fourth international ACM conference on Assistive technologies, Arlington, Virginia, United States, 193 - 200,ISBN:1-58113-314-8.

119

15 Notification Technology Links http://www.activfire.gov.au/ http://www.adipanel.com/ http://www.advancedsafety.com/ http://aes-intellinet.com/ http://www.americanfiretech.com/ http://www.boschsecurity.us/ http://www.brkelectronics.com http://www.clarityproducts.com http://www.clarityproducts.com/products/categories/category326.asp http://www.clearsounds.com http://www2.dupont.com/Fire_Safety/en_US/ http://systemsensor.com/html/avn.html?UniqueID=157 http://exitpoint.systemsensor.com/ http://www.energyrating.gov.au/library/pubs/sb200405-smokealarms.pdf http://www.evax.com/ http://www.faradayfirealarms.com/ http://www.fenwalprotection.com/utcfs/Templates/Pages/Template-46/1,8060,pageId=892&siteId=391,00.html http://www.firecom.com/ http://www.firelite.com/main.html http://www.firstalert.com/FireFacts.aspx http://www.firstalert.com/ http://www.flashpointsoftware.com/faq.html http://www.gamewell.com/main.htm http://www.gemcom.com/ http://www.gentex.com/fire_plo.html http://www.harringtonsignal.com/ http://www.hrkirkland.com/ http://www.innovalarm.com/ http://www.jeromi.com/ http://www.kellerfire.com/ http://www.kiddeus.com http://www.kiddeus.com/utcfs/Templates/Pages/Template-50/0,8061,pageId%3D3909%26siteId%3D384,00.html http://www.kidsmartcorp.com/product_signalone.aspx http://www.krownmfg.com http://www.ledinc.com/ http://www.loudenlow.com http://www.nadidistribution.com http://www.notifier.com/main.htm http://www.shenghua-fire.com/Index-e.htm http://www.onelinkalarms.com http://www.roamsecure.net/content.php?contentID=65 http://www.security.honeywell.com/hsce/products/smoke/index.html http://www.security.honeywell.com/hsce/products/sensor/so/vo/66569.html http://www.sbt.siemens.com/FIS/ http://www.signalonesafety.com http://www.silentcall.com/ http://www.silentknight.com/ http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/SimplexGrinnell http://www.simplexgrinnell.com/ http://www.sonicalert.com/ http://www.systemsensor.com/

120

http://www.universalsecurity.com http://www.vikingservnet.com/ http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Wheelock_Inc.#Products http://www.wheelockinc.com/ http://www.wheelockinc.com/products/product_catalog.htm http://www.wasol.co.uk/

121

16 Appendix A: Request for Proposals – Large Groups

Optimizing Fire Alarm Notification for High Risk Groups Request for Proposals for Human Behavior Studies Request 3 – Notification Effectiveness for Large Groups

BACKGROUND: The Fire Protection Research Foundation has been awarded a Fire Prevention and Safety Grant by the US Fire Administration for a new project on the effectiveness of alarms for emergency notification of high risk groups. The project Panel has identified three groups for study: the hearing impaired, alcohol impaired, and the general population in large groups in commercial and assembly occupancies. The study aims to determine the optimum notification system (most effective signal(s)) for these groups in different stages of the notification process. SCOPE OF WORK: This is a request for proposals for the conduct of studies to determine the most effective signal to provide emergency notification to the general population in large groups in a) assembly and b) commercial occupancies. In this context, “signal” may include an audible, visible or tactile signal or a combination (e.g. mixed T-3/voice). The studies should address the effectiveness of the signal in two aspects of the notification process: alerting and informing. Other aspects to be considered in the studies are the influence of signal pollution and the influence of group interaction. The sample size for the studies should be sufficient to represent the general U.S. population. Subjects should be unprimed if at all possible. The selected contractor is responsible for any required ethical reviews associated with the study. Proposals should include a proposed scope of the study including number of subjects and range of signals to be evaluated, as well as the technique proposed to evaluate each aspect of notification effectiveness. IMPLEMENTATION: The research program will be conducted under the auspices of the Fire Protection Research Foundation and will be guided by a Project Technical Panel consisting of technical experts in the field. The Panel will select the contractor, review periodic reports of progress and research results, and review the final project reports.

REPORTING and DELIVERABLES: The project consultant will provide periodic reporting and communication on all project tasks to the Project Technical Panel and will prepare the following deliverables: a detailed work plan, and draft and final project reports.

INTELLECTUAL PROPERTY: The Research Foundation will retain rights to the project report.

PROJECT MILESTONES and Approximate SCHEDULE Proposals Due August 31 Selection of Contractor September 8 Detailed Work Plan September 22 Draft Report* March 1, 2007 Final Report April 1, 2007

*Contractors should indicate what, if any, enhancements to the project scope could be accomplished with a six month additional work period.

HOW TO RESPOND Letter proposals (not to exceed six pages) shall be submitted electronically to Kathleen Almand, Executive Director of the Foundation, at [email protected] no later than 5:00 p.m. EDT August 31, 2006. Each proposal shall include a description of the following which will be weighted equally in evaluation of proposals: • Prior relevant experience and personnel expertise • Scope and approach within a fixed fee not to exceed $80,000.

122

17 Appendix B: Request for Proposals – Alcohol-Impaired Optimizing Fire Alarm Notification for High Risk Groups Request for Proposals for Human Behavior Studies Request 1 – Waking Effectiveness of Alarms for the Alcohol-Impaired

BACKGROUND: The Fire Protection Research Foundation has been awarded a Fire Prevention and Safety Grant by the US Fire Administration for a new project on the effectiveness of alarms for emergency notification of high risk groups. The project Panel has identified three groups for study: the hearing impaired, alcohol impaired, and the general population in large groups in commercial and assembly occupancies. The study aims to determine the optimum notification system (most effective signal(s)) for these groups in different stages of the notification process.

SCOPE OF WORK: This is a request for proposals for the conduct of a sleep study to determine the most effective signal to alert (awaken) sleeping adults who are alcohol impaired. In this context, “signal” may include an audible, visible or tactile signal or a combination (e.g. mixed T-3/strobe). Note that previous studies have indicated that among audible alerting signals, a mixed T-3 signal has produced the best results in the populations tested. This study is limited to the alerting aspect of notification and does not extend to the effectiveness of the signal in providing information to the awakened subject. Subjects with relatively low (i.e in the range of 0.05 BAC) alcohol impairment should be studied. If possible, subjects should be unprimed; control or measurement of the stage of sleep is preferred but not a requirement. The selected contractor is responsible for any required ethical reviews associated with the study. Proposals should include a proposed scope of the study including number of subjects and range of signals to be evaluated, as well as the technique proposed to evaluate waking effectiveness. IMPLEMENTATION: The research program will be conducted under the auspices of the Fire Protection Research Foundation and will be guided by a Project Technical Panel consisting of technical experts in the field. The Panel will select the contractor, review periodic reports of progress and research results, and review the final project reports.

REPORTING and DELIVERABLES: The project consultant will provide periodic reporting and communication on all project tasks to the Project Technical Panel and will prepare the following deliverables: a detailed work plan, and draft and final project reports.

INTELLECTUAL PROPERTY: The Research Foundation will retain rights to the project report.

PROJECT MILESTONES and Approximate SCHEDULE

Proposals Due August 31 Selection of Contractor September 8 Detailed Work Plan September 22 Draft Report* March 1, 2007 Final Report April 1, 2007

*Contractors should indicate what, if any, enhancements to the project scope could be accomplished with a six month additional work period.

HOW TO RESPOND Letter proposals (not to exceed six pages) shall be submitted electronically to Kathleen Almand, Executive Director of the Foundation, at [email protected] no later than 5:00 p.m. EDT August 31, 2006. Each proposal shall include a description of the following which will be weighted equally in evaluation of proposals: • Prior relevant experience and personnel expertise • Scope and approach within a fixed fee not to exceed $100,000.

123

18 Appendix C: Request for Proposals – Hearing-Impaired Optimizing Fire Alarm Notification for High Risk Groups Request for Proposals for Human Behavior Studies Request 2 – Effectiveness of Alarms for the Hearing Impaired

BACKGROUND: The Fire Protection Research Foundation has been awarded a Fire Prevention and Safety Grant by the US Fire Administration for a new project on the effectiveness of alarms for emergency notification of high risk groups. The project Panel has identified three groups for study: the hearing impaired, alcohol impaired, and the general population in large groups in commercial and assembly occupancies. The study aims to determine the optimum notification system (most effective signal(s)) for these groups in different stages of the notification process. SCOPE OF WORK: This is a request for proposals for the conduct of studies to determine: the most effective signal to provide emergency notification to hearing impaired adults a) in the asleep and b) in the awake condition. In this context, “signal” may include an audible, visible or tactile signal or a combination (e.g. mixed T-3/strobe). Note that previous sleep studies have indicated that among audible alerting signals, a mixed T-3 signal has produced the optimum waking effectiveness in the populations tested. This study includes both the alerting aspect of notification (in the case of sleeping adults, the awakening aspect) and the effectiveness of the signal in providing information to the subject. Subjects with hearing impairment ranging from mild to severe (specific levels to be defined in consultation with the project Panel) should be evaluated, and should be unprimed if at all possible. For the sleep study, control or measurement of the stage of sleep is desirable but is not a requirement. The selected contractor is responsible for any required ethical reviews associated with the studies. Proposals should include a proposed scope of the study including number of subjects and range of signals to be evaluated, as well as the technique proposed to evaluate waking effectiveness. IMPLEMENTATION: The research program will be conducted under the auspices of the Fire Protection Research Foundation and will be guided by a Project Technical Panel consisting of technical experts in the field. The Panel will select the contractor, review periodic reports of progress and research results, and review the final project reports.

REPORTING and DELIVERABLES: The project consultant will provide periodic reporting and communication on all project tasks to the Project Technical Panel and will prepare the following deliverables: a detailed work plan, and draft and final project reports.

INTELLECTUAL PROPERTY: The Research Foundation will retain rights to the project report.

PROJECT MILESTONES and Approximate SCHEDULE

Proposals Due August 31 Selection of Contractor September 8 Detailed Work Plan September 22 Draft Report* March 1, 2007 Final Report April 1, 2007

*Contractors should indicate what, if any, enhancements to the project scope could be accomplished with a six month additional work period.

HOW TO RESPOND Letter proposals (not to exceed six pages) shall be submitted electronically to Kathleen Almand, Executive Director of the Foundation, at [email protected] no later than 5:00 p.m. EDT August 31, 2006. Each proposal shall include a description of the following which will be weighted equally in evaluation of proposals: • Prior relevant experience and personnel expertise • Scope and approach within a fixed fee not to exceed $175,000.

124