Implementing the Syntax of Japanese Numeral Classifiers

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Implementing the Syntax of Japanese Numeral Classifiers Implementing the Syntax of Japanese Numeral Classifiers Emily M. Bender Melanie Siegel Department of Linguistics Saarland University University of Washington Computational Linguistics Box 354340 PF 15 11 50 Seattle WA 98195-4340 D-66041 Saarbruck¨ en [email protected] [email protected] Abstract originally developed as part of the Verbmobil project (Siegel, 2000) to handle spoken Japanese, and then While the sortal constraints associated extended to handle informal written Japanese (email with Japanese numeral classifiers are well- text; (Siegel and Bender, 2002)) and newspaper text. studied, less attention has been paid to Recently, it has been adapted to be consistent with the details of their syntax. We describe the LinGO Grammar Matrix (Bender et al., 2002). an analysis implemented within a broad- coverage HPSG that handles an intricate 2 Types of numeral classifiers set of numeral classifier construction types and compositionally relates each to an ap- Paik and Bond (2002) divide Japanese numeral clas- propriate semantic representation, using sifiers into five major classes: sortal, event, men- Minimal Recursion Semantics. sural, group and taxanomic, and several subclasses. The classes and subclasses can be differentiated ac- 1 Introduction cording to the semantic relationship between the classifiers and the nouns they modify, on two lev- Much attention has been paid to the semantic aspects els: First, what properties of the modified noun mo- of Japanese numeral classifiers, in particular, the se- tivate the choice of the classifier, and second what mantic constraints governing which classifiers co- properties the classifiers predicate of the nouns. As occur with which nouns (Matsumoto, 1993; Bond we are concerned here with the syntax and com- and Paik, 2000). Here, we focus on a more neglected positional semantics of numeral classifiers, we will aspect of this linguistic phenomenon, namely the focus only on the latter. Sortal classifiers, (kind, syntax of numeral classifiers: How they combine shape, and complement classifiers) serve to indi- with number names to create numeral classifier viduate the nouns they modify. Event classifiers phrases, how they modify head nouns, and how they quantify events, characteristically modifying verbs can occur as stand-alone NPs. We find that there is rather than nouns. Mensural classifiers measure both broad similarity and differences in detail across some property of the entity denoted by the noun they different types of numeral classifiers in their syntac- modify (e.g., its length). NPs containing group clas- tic and semantic behavior. We present semantic rep- sifiers denote a group or set of individuals belonging resentations for two types of numeral classifiers, and to the type denoted by the noun. Finally, taxonomic describe how they can be constructed composition- classifiers force a kind or species reading on an NP. ally in an implemented broad-coverage HPSG (Pol- In this paper, we will treat the syntax and compo- lard and Sag, 1994) for Japanese. sitional semantics of sortal and mensural classifiers. The grammar of Japanese in question is JACY,1 However, we believe that our general analysis can 1http://www.dfki.uni-sb.de/ siegel/grammar- be extended to treat the full range of classifiers in download/JACY-grammar.html Japanese and similar languages. 3 Data: Constructions NumClPs can be modified by elements such as yaku ‘approximately’ (before the number name) or mo Internally, Japanese numeral classifier expressions ‘even’ (after the floated numeral classifiers). consist of a number name followed by a numeral The above examples illustrate the contexts with a classifier (1a,b,c). In this, they resemble date ex- sortal numeral classifier, but mensural numeral clas- pressions (1d).2 sifiers can also appear both as modifiers (3a) and as (1) a. juu mai b. juu en NPs in their own right (3b): 10 NumCl 10 yen (3) a. ni kiro no ringo wo katta c. juu kagetsu d. juu gatsu 2 NumCl (kg) GEN apple ACC bought 10 month 10 month ‘(I) bought two kilograms of apples.’ ‘10 months’ ‘October’ b. ni kiro wo katta 2 NumCl (kg) ACC bought In fact, both numeral classifiers and date expressions ‘(I) bought two kilograms.’ are tagged as numeral classifiers by the morpho- logical analyzer ChaSen (Asahara and Matsumoto, NumClPs serving as NPs can also appear as mod- 2000). However, date expressions do not have the ifiers of other nouns: same combinatoric potential (syntactic or semantic) as numeral classifiers. We thus give date expressions (4) a. san nin no deai wa 80 nen haru a distinct analysis, which we will not describe here. 3 NumCl GEN meeting TOP 80 year spring Externally, numeral classifier phrases (NumClPs) ‘The three’s meeting was in the spring of appear in at least four different contexts: alone, as ’80.’ anaphoric NPs (2a); preceding a head noun, linked b. ichi kiro no nedan ha hyaku en desu by the particle no (2b); immediately following a 1 kg GEN price TOP 100 yen COPULA head noun (2c); and ‘floated’, right after the asso- ‘The price of/for 1 kg is 100 yen.’ ciated noun’s case particle or right before the verb (2d). These constructions are distinguished prag- As a result, tokens following the syntactic pattern matically (Downing, 1996).3 of (2b) and (3a) are systematically ambiguous, al- though the non-anaphoric reading tends to be pre- (2) a. ni hiki wo kau ferred. 2 NumCl ACC raise Certain mensural classifiers can be followed by ‘(I) am raising two (small animals).’ the word han ‘half’: b. ni hiki no neko wo kau 2 NumCl GEN cat ACC raise (5) ni kiro han ‘(I) am raising two cats.’ two kg half c. neko ni hiki wo kau ‘two and a half kilograms’ cat 2 NumCl ACC raise In order to build their semantic representations com- ‘(I) am raising two cats.’ positionally, we make the numeral classifier (here, d. neko wo (ni hiki) ie de kiro) the head of the whole expression, and ni and cat ACC (2 NumCl) house LOC han its dependents. Kiro can then orchestrate the se- (ni hiki) kau mantic composition of the two dependents as well (2 NumCl) raise as the composition of the whole expression with the ‘(I) am raising two cats in my house.’ noun it modifies (see 6 below). 2Note that many of the time units are ambiguous with date Although they aren’t tagged as numeral classi- expressions, although some, like the one for months shown in fiers by ChaSen, we extended our analysis of mensu- (1), are distinguished. ral classifiers to certain elements that appear before 3Downing also notes NumClPs following the head noun with an intervening no. As this rare construction did not appear numbers, namely currency symbols (such as $), and in our data, we have not incorporated it into our account. prefixes like No. ‘number’ in (6). (6) kouza No. 1234 gou al., 2001). Abstracting away from handle con- account number 1234 number straints,4 illocutionary force, tense/aspect, and the ‘account number 1234’ unexpressed subject, the representation we build for (2b,c) is as in (8).5 Finally, we found that number names can some- times occur without numeral classifiers, either as (8) cat n rel(x), udef rel(x), card rel(x,“2”), modifiers of nouns or as anaphora: raise v rel(z,x) (7) (kouza) 1234 wo tojitai This can be read as follows: A relation of raising ¡ ¡ (account) 1234 ACC close.volitional holds between (the unexpressed subject), and . ‘(I) want to close (account) 1234.’ denotes a cat entity, and is bound by an underspeci- fied quantifier (as there is no explicit determiner). ¡ Due to space considerations, we won’t describe our is also an argument of a card rel (short for ‘cardi- analysis of such bare number names here. nal relation’), whose other argument is the constant 4 Data: Distribution value 2, meaning that there are in fact two cats being referred to.6 We used ChaSen to segment and tag 10,000 para- For anaphoric numeral classifiers, the representa- graphs of the Mainichi Shinbun 2002 corpus. Of tion contains an underspecified noun relation, to be the resulting 490,202 words, 11,515 (2.35%) were resolved in further processing to a specific relation: tagged as numeral classifiers. 4,543 of those were (9) noun relation(x), udef rel(x), card rel(x,“2”), potentially time/date expressions, leaving 6,972 nu- raise v rel(z,x) meral classifiers, or 1.42% of the words. 203 ortho- graphically distinct numeral classifiers occur in the Mensural classifiers have somewhat more elab- corpus. The most frequent is nin (the numeral clas- orated semantic representations, which we treat as sifier for people) which occurs 1,675 times. similar to English measure NPs (Flickinger and We sampled 100 sentences tagged as containing Bond, 2003). On this analysis, the NumClP de- numeral classifiers to examine the distribution of the notes the extent of some dimension or property constructions outlined in 3. These sentences con- of the modified N. This dimension or property is tained a total of 159 numeral classifier phrases and represented with an underspecified relation (un- the vast majority (128) were stand-alone NPs. This spec adj rel), and a degree rel relates the mea- contrasts with Downing’s (1996) study of 500 exam- sured amount to the underspecified adjective rela- ples from modern works of fiction and spoken texts, tion.7 The underspecified adjective relation modi- where most of the occurrences are not anaphoric. fies the N in the usual way. This is illustrated in Furthermore, while our sample contains no exam- (10), which is the semantic representation assigned ples of the floated variety, Downing’s contains 96. to (3a).8 The discrepancy probably arises because Downing 4The potentially underspecified MRS representation of only included sortal numeral classifiers, and not any scope.
Recommended publications
  • A Syntactic Analysis of Copular Sentences*
    A SYNTACTIC ANALYSIS OF COPULAR SENTENCES* Masato Niimura Nanzan University 1. Introduction Copular is a verb whose main function is to link subjects with predicate complements. In a limited sense, the copular refers to a verb that does not have any semantic content, but links subjects and predicate complements. In a broad sense, the copular contains a verb that has its own meaning and bears the syntactic function of “the copular.” Higgins (1979) analyses English copular sentences and classifies them into three types. (1) a. predicational sentence b. specificational sentence c. identificational sentence Examples are as follows: (2) a. John is a philosopher. b. The bank robber is John Smith. c. That man is Mary’s brother. (2a) is an example of predicational sentence. This type takes a reference as subject, and states its property in predicate. In (2a), the reference is John, and its property is a philosopher. (2b) is an example of specificational sentence, which expresses what meets a kind of condition. In (2b), what meets the condition of the bank robber is John Smith. (2c) is an example of identificational sentence, which identifies two references. This sentence identifies that man and Mary’s brother. * This paper is part of my MA thesis submitted to Nanzan University in March, 2007. This paper was also presented at the Connecticut-Nanzan-Siena Workshop on Linguistic Theory and Language Acquisition, held at Nanzan University on February 21, 2007. I would like to thank the participants for the discussions and comments on this paper. My thanks go to Keiko Murasugi, Mamoru Saito, Tomohiro Fujii, Masatake Arimoto, Jonah Lin, Yasuaki Abe, William McClure, Keiko Yano and Nobuko Mizushima for the invaluable comments and discussions on the research presented in this paper.
    [Show full text]
  • Copula-Less Nominal Sentences and Matrix-C Clauses: a Planar View Of
    Copula-less Nominal Sentences and Matrix-C0 Clauses: A Planar View of Clause Structure * Tanmoy Bhattacharya Abstract This paper is an attempt to unify the apparently unrelated sentence types in Bangla, namely, copula-less nominal sentences and matrix-C clauses. In particular, the paper claims that a “Planar” view of clause structure affords us, besides the above unification, a better account of the Kaynean Algorithm (KA, as in Kayne (1998a, b; 1999)) in terms of an interface driven motivation to break symmetry. In order to understand this unification, the invocation of the KA, that proclaims most significantly the ‘non-constituency’ of the C and its complement, is relevant if we agree with the basic assumption of this paper that views both Cs and Classifiers as disjoint from the main body (plane) of the clause. Such a disjunction is exploited further to introduce a new perspective of the structure of clauses, namely, the ‘planar’ view. The unmistakable non-linearity of the KA is seen here as a multi-planar structure creation; the introduction of the C/ CLA thus implies a new plane introduction. KA very strongly implies a planar view of clause structure. The identification of a plane is considered to be as either required by the C-I or the SM interface and it is shown that this view matches up with the duality of semantics, as in Chomsky (2005). In particular, EM (External Merge) is required to introduce or identify a new plane, whereas IM (Internal Merge) is inter- planar. Thus it is shown that inter-planar movements are discourse related, whereas intra-planar movements are not discourse related.
    [Show full text]
  • Identification of Zero Copulas in Hungarian Using
    Proceedings of the 12th Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2020), pages 4802–4810 Marseille, 11–16 May 2020 c European Language Resources Association (ELRA), licensed under CC-BY-NC Much Ado About Nothing Identification of Zero Copulas in Hungarian Using an NMT Model Andrea Dömötör1;2, Zijian Gyoz˝ o˝ Yang1, Attila Novák1 1MTA-PPKE Hungarian Language Technology Research Group, Pázmány Péter Catholic University, Faculty of Information Technology and Bionics Práter u. 50/a, 1083 Budapest, Hungary 2Pázmány Péter Catholic University, Faculty of Humanities and Social Sciences Egyetem u. 1, 2087 Piliscsaba, Hungary {surname.firstname}@itk.ppke.hu Abstract The research presented in this paper concerns zero copulas in Hungarian, i.e. the phenomenon that nominal predicates lack an explicit verbal copula in the default present tense 3rd person indicative case. We created a tool based on the state-of-the-art transformer architecture implemented in Marian NMT framework that can identify and mark the location of zero copulas, i.e. the position where an overt copula would appear in the non-default cases. Our primary aim was to support quantitative corpus-based linguistic research by creating a tool that can be used to compile a corpus of significant size containing examples of nominal predicates including the location of the zero copulas. We created the training corpus for our system transforming sentences containing overt copulas into ones containing zero copula labels. However, we first needed to disambiguate occurrences of the massively ambiguous verb van ‘exist/be/have’. We performed this using a rule-base classifier relying on English translations in the English-Hungarian parallel subcorpus of the OpenSubtitles corpus.
    [Show full text]
  • A Parallel Analysis of Have-Type Copular Constructions in Two Have-Less Indo-European Languages
    A PARALLEL ANALYSIS OF HAVE-TYPE COPULAR CONSTRUCTIONS IN TWO HAVE-LESS INDO-EUROPEAN LANGUAGES Sebastian Sulger Universitat¨ Konstanz Proceedings of the LFG11 Conference Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King (Editors) 2011 CSLI Publications http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/ Abstract This paper presents data from two Indo-European languages, Irish and Hindi/ Urdu, which do not use verbs for expressing possession (i.e., they do not have a verb comparable to the English verb have). Both of the languages use cop- ula constructions. Hindi/Urdu combines the copula with either a genitive case marker or a postposition on the possessor noun phrase to construct pos- session. Irish achieves the same effect by combining one of two copula ele- ments with a prepositional phrase. I argue that both languages differentiate between temporary and permanent instances, or stage-level and individual- level predication, of possession. The syntactic means for doing so do not overlap between the two: while Hindi/Urdu employs two distinct markers to differentiate between stage-level and individual-level predication, Irish uses two different copulas. A single parallel LFG analysis for both languages is presented based on the PREDLINK analysis. It is shown how the analysis is capable of serving as input to the semantics, which is modeled using Glue Semantics and which differentiates between stage-level and individual-level predication by means of a situation argument. In particular, it is shown that the inalienable/alienable distinction previ- ously applied to the Hindi/Urdu data is insufficient. The reanalysis presented here in terms of the stage-level vs. individual-level distinction can account for the data from Hindi/Urdu in a more complete way.
    [Show full text]
  • Hungarian Copula Constructions in Dependency Syntax and Parsing
    Hungarian copula constructions in dependency syntax and parsing Katalin Ilona Simko´ Veronika Vincze University of Szeged University of Szeged Institute of Informatics Institute of Informatics Department of General Linguistics MTA-SZTE Hungary Research Group on Artificial Intelligence [email protected] Hungary [email protected] Abstract constructions? And if so, how can we deal with cases where the copula is not present in the sur- Copula constructions are problematic in face structure? the syntax of most languages. The paper In this paper, three different answers to these describes three different dependency syn- questions are discussed: the function head analy- tactic methods for handling copula con- sis, where function words, such as the copula, re- structions: function head, content head main the heads of the structures; the content head and complex label analysis. Furthermore, analysis, where the content words, in this case, the we also propose a POS-based approach to nominal part of the predicate, are the heads; and copula detection. We evaluate the impact the complex label analysis, where the copula re- of these approaches in computational pars- mains the head also, but the approach offers a dif- ing, in two parsing experiments for Hun- ferent solution to zero copulas. garian. First, we give a short description of Hungarian copula constructions. Second, the three depen- 1 Introduction dency syntactic frameworks are discussed in more Copula constructions show some special be- detail. Then, we describe two experiments aim- haviour in most human languages. In sentences ing to evaluate these frameworks in computational with copula constructions, the sentence’s predi- linguistics, specifically in dependency parsing for cate is not simply the main verb of the clause, Hungarian, similar to Nivre et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Resulting Copulas and Their Complements in British and American English: a Corpus Based Study
    UNIVERZITA PALACKÉHO V OLOMOUCI FILOZOFICKÁ FAKULTA Katedra anglistiky a amerikanistiky Martin Dokoupil anglická filologie & francouzská filologie Resulting Copulas and their Complements in British and American English: A Corpus Based Study. Bakalářská práce Vedoucí diplomové práce: Mgr. Michaela Martinková, PhD. OLOMOUC 2011 Prohlašuji, že jsem tuto bakalářskou práci vypracoval samostatně na základě uvedených pramenů a literatury. V Olomouci, dne 10. srpna 2011 podpis 2 I hereby declare that this bachelor thesis is completely my own work and that I used only the cited sources. Olomouc, 10th August 2011 signature 3 Děkuji vedoucí mé bakalářské práce Mgr. Michaele Martinkové, PhD. za ochotu, trpělivost a cenné rady při psaní této práce. 4 Table of Contents: 1 Introduction ..........................................................................................................................6 2 Theoretical Preliminaries ....................................................................................................7 2.1 Literature .....................................................................................................................7 2.2 Copular verb in general ..............................................................................................8 2.2.1 Copular verb .......................................................................................................8 2.2.2 Prototypical copular usage ...............................................................................8 2.2.3 Copular verb complementation
    [Show full text]
  • Copula Omission: a Deeper Look Into One Explanation
    Copula omission: A deeper look into one explanation Copula omission: A deeper look into one explanation Strohman, A. Introduction When a child acquires English as a first language, there is a long period of grammatical development that may stretch into the fourth year of life (de Villiers & de Villiers, 1973). During this period, there is a phenomenon in which children omit specific grammatical morphemes, one of which is copula be. This period of grammatical development has been characterized as the Optional Infinitive (OI) Stage (Wexler, 2011). This stage is a cross-linguistic phenomenon, but some languages tend to have more drop qualities than others. One of these languages is English. According to Rice, Wexler and Hershberger (1998), copula omission is part of the extended OI phase in children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI), and children omit due to a processing difficulty. Additionally, problems in the Optional Infinitive Stage have shown to be a clear clinical marker of language delay (Hadley & Short, 2005). According to this study, analyzing the onset of tense marking, specifically copula be, can be used to evaluate grammatical development in children. Currently, there are numerous approaches as to why children omit copula. These approaches highlight the various theories of child language development. For example, Function morphemes in young children's speech perception and production by Gerken and Landau (1990) discusses the metrical template explanation as to why children omit. In this study, three experiments were performed to analyze how children distinguish between different function morphemes. The purpose was to observe the idea that children 1 Copula omission: A deeper look into one explanation may be able to detect these specific functors even though they omit them during speech.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 29 Animacy Is a Presupposition in Swahili Jonathan Pesetsky ILLC, University of Amsterdam
    Chapter 29 Animacy is a presupposition in Swahili Jonathan Pesetsky ILLC, University of Amsterdam In this paper, I argue that the phenomenon of animacy override in Swahili arises from the interaction between a syntactic structure with multiple nominal heads and general principles of distributed morphology. This syntactic analysis narrows the possibilities for a semantic analysis of animacy, strongly suggesting an ap- proach previously proposed for gender in Romance languages. Specifically, I argue that Swahili has an interpretable +animate feature which denotes a partial func- tion which is defined only on animate predicates of type 푒푡 and which denotes the identity function where it is defined. 1 Introduction In this paper, I argue that a puzzle in the distribution of animate morphology in Swahili arises from the interaction between a syntactic structure with multiple nominal heads and general principles of Distributed Morphology. This syntactic analysis narrows the possibilities for a semantic analysis of animacy, strongly suggesting an approach previously proposed for gender in Romance languages. The central puzzle of this paper is presented in(1), which shows that Swahili animate-denoting nouns obligatorily trigger animate agreement whether or not they themselves bear animate prefixes. (1) a. Ki-ongozi w-etu m-refu a-li-anguka. 7-leader 1-our 1-tall 1-pst-fall ‘Our tall leader fell down.’ b. * Ki-ongozi ch-etu ki-refu ki-li-anguka. 7-leader 7-our 7-tall 7-pst-fall Jonathan Pesetsky. 2019. Animacy is a presupposition in Swahili. In Emily Clem, Peter Jenks & Hannah Sande (eds.), Theory and description in African Linguistics: Se- lected papers from the 47th Annual Conference on African Linguistics, 555–570.
    [Show full text]
  • Copular Verbs and Copula Drop in Tundra Nenets
    Copular verbs and copula drop in Tundra Nenets Veronika Hegedűs1, Nikolett Mus1 and Balázs Surányi∗1,2 1Research Institute for Linguistics Hungarian Academy of Sciences 2Pázmány Péter Catholic University SWL8, Paris, 3–5 September 2018 1 Introduction 1.1 Background: general • Copular sentences exhibit rich and multifaceted – and in many respects still ill- understood – morphosyntactic variation across languages. Parameters of both cross- linguistic and intra-linguistic variation in copular clauses: – verbal copulas vs non-verbal copulas: e.g. English has a verbal copula (be), while Russian has a verbal copula (byt’) as well as a pronominal one (eto). – the number of different copulas within a language: e.g. English is a language with one verbal element be, while Tundra Nenets has three distinct morphological forms corresponding to English be, all of them verbal (Na-, me-, tańa-) – the distribution of copulas across copular clause types: e.g. Hungarian uses van ‘be’ in copular sentences, in existential constructions and with predicative possessives, while English uses be in copular and existential sentences (and uses have for predicative possessives). – the licensing conditions of a ‘zero copula’, i.e., the possibility of absence of an overt copula: e.g. English always has overt be in full clauses, Hungarian allows for zero copula (has “nominal sentences”) with nominal predicates in present tense indicatives with a third person subject. ∗[email protected], [email protected], [email protected] 1 1.2 Background: Tundra Nenets • Tundra Nenets is an endangered indigenous language that belongs to the Samoyedic branch of the Uralic language family.
    [Show full text]
  • What Does the Copula Do? Kunio Nishiyama Cornell University, [email protected]
    University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics Volume 4 | Issue 2 Article 15 1-1-1997 What Does the Copula Do? Kunio Nishiyama Cornell University, [email protected] This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. http://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol4/iss2/15 For more information, please contact [email protected]. What Does the Copula Do? This working paper is available in University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: http://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol4/ iss2/15 What Does the Copula Do?* Kunio Nishiyama 1. Introduction It is widely assumed in the literature on the copula that there are at least two kinds of copula (cf. Higgins 1973): (1) a. John is a boy. (predicative) b. Dr. Jekyll is Mr. Hyde. (equative) It is controversial whether these different usages should be treated differently or can be given a unified account, and this is not my concern here. The purpose of this paper is to analyze the syntactic structure of predicative copular sentences like (1a) from a crosslinguistic perspective. Since Bach (1967), it has often been claimed that the copula is a tense-supporter.1 For example, Rapoport (1987: 152ff) notes the following contrast: (2) a. I consider [Xeli a nut]. b. Xeli *(is) a nut. In a small clause structure like (2a), no copula is necessary, even though there seems to be a predication relation between Xeli and a nut. In the matrix sentence, however, the copula is necessary (2b). Since (2a) shows that the copula is not necessary for predication, Rapoport (1987: 157) claims that “[b]e is inserted to support the feature of INFL, in the cases above [2b] the features of tense ([- past]) and agreement.” According to this hypothesis, (3) is analyzed as (4): (3) Sal was strong.
    [Show full text]
  • Hypomnemata Glossopoetica
    Hypomnemata Glossopoetica Wm S. Annis June 8, 2021 1. Phonology Normally illegal clusters may occur in particular grammatical contexts, and thus look common (cf. Latin -st in 3sg copula est). Hierarchy of codas:1 n < m, ɳ, ŋ < ɳ « l, ɹ < r, ʎ, ʁ < ɭ, ɽ « t < k, p < s, z, c, q, ʃ < b, d, g, x h « w, j. There is a slight place hierarchy: alveolar < velar < retroflex or tap. Classes percolate, such that in complex codas, if Nasals, Resonants and Stops are permitted you usually expect n, r, s, nr, ns and rs as coda sequences. Other orders are possible, but the above rule is common-ish. Hierarchy of clusters (s = sonorant, o = obstruent), word initial: os < oo < ss < so; word final: so < oo < ss < os. Onset clusters tend to avoid identical places of articulation, which leads to avoidance of things like *tl, dl, bw, etc., in a good number of languages. /j/ is lightly disfavored as c2 after dentals, alveolars and palatals; /j/ and palatals are in general disfavored before front vowels. Languages with sC- clusters often have codas. s+stop < s+fric / s+nasal < s+lat < s+rhot (the fricative and nasal are trickier to order). Even if a particular c is a permitted coda, its allowed environment may be quite restricted. Potential con- straints: forbidden before homorganic stop; or homorganic nasal; geminates forbidden. Solutions: delete with compensatory vowel lengthening; debuccalize (become fricative, glottal stop, delete without compensation); nasal deletion with nasalized vowel remaining; tone wackiness. Lower vowels are preferred as syllabic nuclei; high vowels are more prone to syncope (either midword or fi- nally).
    [Show full text]
  • Copula Omission: a Deeper Look Into One Explanation
    CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by Illinois Digital Environment for Access to Learning and Scholarship Repository Copula omission: A deeper look into one explanation Copula omission: A deeper look into one explanation Strohman, A. Introduction When a child acquires English as a first language, there is a long period of grammatical development that may stretch into the fourth year of life (de Villiers & de Villiers, 1973). During this period, there is a phenomenon in which children omit specific grammatical morphemes, one of which is copula be. This period of grammatical development has been characterized as the Optional Infinitive (OI) Stage (Wexler, 2011). This stage is a cross-linguistic phenomenon, but some languages tend to have more drop qualities than others. One of these languages is English. According to Rice, Wexler and Hershberger (1998), copula omission is part of the extended OI phase in children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI), and children omit due to a processing difficulty. Additionally, problems in the Optional Infinitive Stage have shown to be a clear clinical marker of language delay (Hadley & Short, 2005). According to this study, analyzing the onset of tense marking, specifically copula be, can be used to evaluate grammatical development in children. Currently, there are numerous approaches as to why children omit copula. These approaches highlight the various theories of child language development. For example, Function morphemes in young children's speech perception and production by Gerken and Landau (1990) discusses the metrical template explanation as to why children omit. In this study, three experiments were performed to analyze how children distinguish between different function morphemes.
    [Show full text]