1 a History of ASAO Sessions

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

1 a History of ASAO Sessions A History of ASAO Sessions: Formats and Topics Alexander Mawyer Program Coordinator 2014–2017; ASAO Board 2017–2019 Alan Howard ASAO Executive Committee 1971–1973; Program Chair 1973; ASAO Board 1995–1997; Website Manager 2000–2015 Much can be learned about an academic organization by examining its activities in historical perspective. In this paper we focus on the development of the Association for Social Anthropology in Oceania (ASAO)’s unique structure of annual conferences. Among the topics we address are: What was the original justification for holding meetings? How did the format for meetings evolve? Who were the main actors in shaping the structure and functioning of the organization’s activities? And what does an analysis of sessions held during the annual meetings over the past fifty years have to tell us about changes and continuities in the direction of ASAO? Our sources include ASAO Newsletters dating back to 1967, the recollections of some of the surviving founders of the organization and long-term members who were involved in the shaping of ASAO’s policies over the years, and a database we compiled of some 700 sessions from the nascent origin of the association in 1967 through 2017, which includes the organizers of sessions, the names given to the sessions, and the authors and titles of papers contributed to sessions where such information was available. ASAO Beginnings In order to understand the way in which the organization of meetings and the topics dealt with evolved, one has to begin with the initial vision that prompted the organization of ASAO, or more appropriately its forerunner, ASAEO (the Association for Social Anthropology in Eastern Oceania). The idea for an organization that would focus on anthropological issues in the Pacific 1 Islands had one of its defining moments during a luncheon meeting between Vern Carroll and Alan Howard at Lynn’s Delicatessen at Ala Moana Shopping Center in Honolulu in 1966. Carroll had recently returned from three years on Nukuoro Atoll (1963–1966) and Howard had spent two years studying Rotumans on Rotuma and in Fiji (1959–1961). In the course of their discussion, the topic of comparative research came up, especially the work in Africa and the publication of African Political Systems (Fortes and Evans-Pritchard 1940) and African Systems of Kinship and Marriage (Radcliffe-Brown and Forde 1950). A. R. Radcliffe-Brown had made a strong case for regionally based comparative studies. For example, in the preface to African Political Systems, he argued that the “comparative study of political institutions, with special reference to the simpler societies, is an important branch of social anthropology which has not yet received the attention it deserves” and that doing so will allow scholars to “discover the universal, essential, characters which belong to all human societies, past, present and future” (Radcliffe-Brown 1940: xi). And in a 1951 article entitled “The Comparative Method in Social Anthropology,” he wrote: For social anthropology the task is to formulate and validate statements about the conditions of existence of social systems (laws of social statics) and the regularities that are observable in social change (laws of social dynamics). This can only be done by the systematic use of the comparative method, and the only justification of that method is the expectation that it will provide us with results of this kind, or, as Boas stated it, will provide us with knowledge of the laws of social development. It will be only in an integrated and organised study in which historical studies and sociological studies are combined that we shall be able to reach a real understanding of the development of human society. (Radcliffe- Brown 1951: 22) Carroll and Howard both subscribed to the idea that above all, social anthropology must be a comparative endeavor if any real progress were to take place in our understanding of social dynamics and social history. 2 At the time, the idea that Polynesia in particular was a natural laboratory for comparative research was in the air, stimulated in large measure by Marshall Sahlins’s publication of Social Stratification in Polynesia (1958). Carroll felt that Micronesia, too, should be included in the “laboratory,” insofar as, like Polynesia, it also manifested enough of a shared cultural template to warrant comparative investigations. Credit for translation of these ideas into an organizational mode—the decision to form an organization to conduct comparative investigations in the Eastern Pacific—was entirely Carroll’s, and accordingly, he, along with Roger Keesing, organized the first “symposium” in March 1967 at Keesing’s home institution, the University of California–Santa Cruz. The sole topic of the first meeting was adoption in Eastern Pacific societies (initially including Island Melanesia). Papers on adoption were first solicited by Carroll in the summer of 1964 during a break from his fieldwork, and a symposium on the topic was held at the annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association in November of that year. As noted in a prospectus for the volume Adoption in Eastern Oceania (Carroll 1970), the papers from that AAA symposium, along with some additional papers, were circulated among the invitees to the UC–Santa Cruz meeting, and the authors commented on one another’s papers through correspondence. Why the focus on adoption? This was a period in sociocultural anthropology when kinship issues were of great prominence. In 1949, George Peter Murdock had published Social Structure, a book in which kinship terminology played a major role; Claude Lévi-Strauss published Les Structures Élémentaires de la Parenté (The Elementary Structures of Kinship) in the same year; and componential analysis of kinship terms was still in vogue (eg, Goodenough 1956; Lounsbury 1956; Wallace and Atkins 1960). All of these approaches proved highly 3 controversial and stimulated spirited debates (eg, Sahlins’s 1963 review of Murdock’s 1960 edited volume Social Structure in Southeast Asia; Homans and Schneider’s 1955 critique of Lévi-Strauss; Leach’s 1971 criticism of componential analysis). Within kinship studies, a number of anthropologists were interested in adoption in relation to the transmission of rights in land and other forms of property. While in Africa unilinear (primarily patrilineal) kin groups readily lent themselves to the formation of corporate entities, most Oceanic societies were characterized by cognatic kin groups, which resulted in blurred boundaries and the possibility of multiple and overlapping memberships. The driving question became how such ill-defined groups could function effectively as property-holding corporations, a topic that stimulated considerable discussion and debate in the 1960s and 1970s. Perhaps the strongest critic of prevailing kinship theories at the time was David Schneider, Carroll’s mentor at the University of Chicago, where Carroll had done his graduate work. Schneider was a strong advocate for an empirical approach and disdained theoretical and formalistic approaches to kinship based on European concepts of consanguinity. He asserted that the notion of kinship exists in the minds of anthropologists rather than in the minds of the people they study: “Kinship has been defined by European social scientists, and European social scientists use their own folk culture as the source of many, if not all, of their ways of formulating and understanding the world about them” (Schneider 1984: 193). Schneider’s involvement in the adoption project from the beginning is apparent in a July 7, 1965, memo from Carroll, circulated to colleagues, that stated, “If there are any who would like to do a paper towards a later seminar (and eventual publication) on this topic [adoption], please declare yourself now to David Schneider, University of Chicago.” 4 In his introduction to Adoption in Eastern Oceania, Carroll reflected Schneider’s approach, and the relevance of adoption to understanding kinship: The answer to questions about the nature of kinship can only be determined on the basis of investigations into the precise extent to which adoptive relations are construed as tantamount to “biological” relationships. To what degree does adoption really rearrange ties of consanguinity? Is it possible that, appearances to the contrary, adoption is not perceived as not changing the “natural” relationship of the child to his “biological” parents (as I have argued in chapter 6)? Or is it perhaps the case, as [Robert] Levy has suggested (chapter 4), that biological parenthood may, in some societies, be usefully construed as “contingent” in the same way that an adoptive relationship is contingent? (Carroll 1970: 14) As Michael Lieber recalled, Schneider’s role in the adoption symposium was significant in several respects. One outcome of his role was the nearly excruciating tension that characterized that session. Harold Scheffler, Paul Kay, and Ward Goodenough represented approaches to interpretation of data to which Schneider was unalterably opposed. Is genealogy a scientific representation of biological reality or a cultural construct? Everyone in the room was wary of starting a row. “We never resolved this tension, in great part because we had no experience in how to talk to one another” (Lieber, personal communication, March 2015). The significance of kinship for the nascent organization, at least in Carroll’s mind, was clearly manifest in the February 1968 ASAEO Newsletter (#2: 11) in which he proposed possible topics for the symposium to follow the adoption session (see table 1). Table 1: Carroll’s Proposed Topics for Next Symposium 1. Incest 2. Primary kin roles (mo, fa, child) 3. “close” kin vs. “distant kin” 4. Kinship terminology 5. Affinity as a principle of social organization 6. Marital stability and divorce 7. Wives as quasi-kin 8. Friends as quasi-kin 9. Domestic economy 10. Male/Female 5 11. Age as a principle of social organization 12. Economic implications of extended kin ties 13. Sex practices and their implications 14.
Recommended publications
  • A Cognitive Approach to Medieval Mode
    Empirical Musicology Review Vol. 3, No. 2, 2008 Commentary on “The Perception and Cognition of Time in Balinese Music” by Andrew Clay McGraw IAN CROSS, SATINDER GILL, SARAH KNIGHT, CHRIS NASH, TAL-CHEN RABINOWITCH, LYDIA SLOBODIAN, NETA SPIRO, GHOFUR WOODRUFF, and MATTHEW WOOLHOUSE[1] University of Cambridge ABSTRACT: We review the paper by Andrew Clay McGraw, noting that it represents an interesting and valuable contribution to the study of music in cognition in its informed exploration of non-western musical perceptions. We raise a number of concerns about the methods used, and make suggestions as to how the issues that were empirically addressed in the paper might have been tackled in ways that would have enhanced the interpretability of its findings. Submitted 2008 April 5; accepted 2008 April 14. KEYWORDS: experimental method, non-western music, collaborative research THIS fascinating paper takes as its starting point the idea that time in music and time in general, may be bound by culture in our conceptions, in particular, by processes of cross-domain mapping of the types that have been postulated as operational by ethnomusicologists, music theorists, and music psychologists. It explores by means of three experiments Becker's hypothesis that Balinese gamelan music reflects and perhaps embodies emic conceptions of time. The first uses an experimental paradigm known as the Implicit Association Test to investigate, by means of response times, the strengths of associations between musical and/or dance terms, music and/or instrument terms, and time and/or position terms, among Balinese musicians. It found evidence for a slight tendency for time-in music to be 'implicitly iconic' of time-in-general, though noted that the results showed high variability.
    [Show full text]
  • Seeger's Unitary Field Theory Reconsidered 142 Lawrence M
    131 Seeger 's Unitary Field Theory Reconsidered notion of a properly musical concept in greater detail in the latter half of this essay; in my own work it has been fundamental to addressing the question of how it is we structure our understanding of music and for developing an account of the relationship between language and music. Charles Seeger wrestled with the relationship between language and music throughout his long life. In 1913, only a year after he took charge 6 of starting a music program at Berkeley, he wrote, "The term 'Musicol- ogy' comprises, in its widest sense, the whole linguistic treatment of Seeger's Unitay Field Theoy Reconsidered music-the manual instruction, the historical study, the music-research \ of the psycho-physical laboratory, the piece of mugc criticism. Mu- Lawrence M. Zbikowski sic is not founded upon language or upon language studies. But its con- duct in our day depends customarily on an extensive use of language" (Seeger 1913: n.p.). In later years Seeger situated the paradox of music's dependence on language at the musicological juncture. Speaking to the Society for Ethno- musicology in 1976, Seeger described the musicological juncture as "the situation we place ourselves in when we talk about music and particu- larly when, as now, we talk about talking about music" (lgna:180). The essays of Seeger's final two decades repeatedly explored this situation, As musicians, we are aware of a certain tension between making music- examining the way speech was used generally, how it was applied to through composition, performance, or ritual-and talking about music.
    [Show full text]
  • Giovanni Bennardo, Ph.D. Curriculum Vitae
    Giovanni Bennardo, Ph.D. Presidential Research Professor Curriculum Vitae Northern Illinois University Department of Anthropology Cognitive Studies Initiative and Environment, Sustainability and Energy Institute DeKalb, Illinois USA ph: (815) 753-8574 e-mail: [email protected] webpage: www.niu.edu/bennardo Place of Birth: Cosenza, Italy Country of Residence: US, since 1988 Country of Citizenship: Italy and US EDUCATION • Ph.D. Anthropology (Linguistic and Cognitive), University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; May 1996. • M.A. in Linguistics (General and Applied), Ohio University, Athens; June 1987. • Laurea in Lingue e Letterature Straniere: English. Universitá di Pisa, Italy. Academic Interests Topical Interests: Linguistic and cognitive anthropology, knowledge representations, spatial cognition, cultural models, language and culture, language and space, cultural models of nature, conceptual semantics, Pacific navigation, Polynesian kinship, and social networks. Geographical Focus: Oceania, Polynesia, The Kingdom of Tonga. Dissertation Title: A Computational Approach to Spatial Cognition: Representing Spatial Relationships in Tongan Language and Culture. Sabbatical Leaves Professor (Linguistic and Cognitive Anthropology) Department of Anthropology and Cognitive Studies Initiative, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb; Spring semester, 2019 Presidential Research Professor (PRP) (Linguistic and Cognitive Anthropology) Department of Anthropology and Cognitive Studies Initiative, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb; Spring semester, 2015
    [Show full text]
  • Bradd Shore. Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture, and the Problem of Meaning
    Comparative Civilizations Review Volume 39 Article 9 Number 39 Fall 1998 10-1-1998 Bradd Shore. Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture, and the Problem of Meaning. Palmer Talbutt Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/ccr Recommended Citation Talbutt, Palmer (1998) "Bradd Shore. Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture, and the Problem of Meaning.," Comparative Civilizations Review: Vol. 39 : No. 39 , Article 9. Available at: https://scholarsarchive.byu.edu/ccr/vol39/iss39/9 This Book Review is brought to you for free and open access by the All Journals at BYU ScholarsArchive. It has been accepted for inclusion in Comparative Civilizations Review by an authorized editor of BYU ScholarsArchive. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. Talbutt: Bradd Shore. <em>Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture, and the Pro 97 BOOK REVIEWS Bradd Shore. Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture, and the Problem of Meaning. New York: Oxford University Press, 1996. This is a remarkable book that will repay close study; it is by no means light bedtime reading. There are two poles to it, or more exactly, the second pole conflates two themes: "cognitive science" and late modernity. This interesting conflation comes off better, to my mind, than do Shore's heroic bridging efforts between cultural anthropology and recent cognitive science. He seems to attempt a tight-rope spanning of the Grand Canyon between Way Back When and Right Now. He was a Peace Corps member of astute abilities in Samoa, and followed up diligently into other anthropological researches. Then he was a student at Berkeley when the most current and extravagant ideas of psy- chology and symbolisms were bruited about.
    [Show full text]
  • Bradd Shore Curriculum Vitae September, 2012 PERMANENT ADDRESS: Department Anthropology Emory University Atlanta, Georgia 30322 (404) 727-4200
    Bradd Shore Curriculum Vitae September, 2012 PERMANENT ADDRESS: Department Anthropology Emory University Atlanta, Georgia 30322 (404) 727-4200 EDUCATION 1967 B.A. (English Literature, Summa Cum Laud). University of California, Berkeley. 1971 M.A. (Anthropology). Department of Anthropology, The University of Chicago. 1977 Ph.D. (Anthropology). Department of Anthropology, The University of Chicago. THESES, DISSERTATIONS, ADVISORS B.A. Honors Thesis (Berkeley). The Winters Tale: Shakespeare's Pastoral Care. John Anson, Advisor. 1967. M.A. Thesis (Chicago). Adoption, Alliance and Political Mobility in Samoa. David M. Schneider, Advisor. 1971. Ph.D. Dissertation (Chicago). A Samoan Theory of Action: Social Control and Social Order in a Polynesian Paradox. Raymond Fogelson, Marshall D. Sahlins, David M. Schneider, Advisors. 1977. MAJOR AREAS OF INTEREST IN ANTHROPOLOGY Cultural Anthropology; Cognitive Anthropology; Culture Theory; Myth, Ritual, Religion; Language and Culture; Kinship and Social Organization; Primitive Religion; Symbolic Processes; Samoa; Polynesia; Australian Aborigines, Anthropology and Literature; Psychological Anthropology; Culture and Technology; American Popular Culture. Ritual and Myth in American Family Life AWARDS, HONORS, FELLOWSHIPS, GRANTS 1963-4 Full Scholarship, Union College 1965-6 Alfred J. Sloan Scholarship, University of California, Berkeley 1966 Election to Senior Honor Society, University of California, Berkeley 1967 Election to Phi Beta Kappa, University of California, Berkeley 1970-1 Awarded Unendowed University
    [Show full text]
  • Margaret Mead and the Culture of Forgetting in Anthropology: a Response to Paul Roscoe
    MICAELA Dl LEONARDO Margaret Mead and the Culture of Forgetting in Anthropology: A Response to Paul Roscoe Only connect. research, and write and speak. And this is what is curious about Paul Roscoe's piece: its anachronism, its radical lack —E. M. Forster of a sense of the historical shifts in anthropology and in the world, since Margaret Mead and Reo Fortune wrote EARLY A QUARTER CENTURY AGO, the late Eric about the Mountain Arapesh in the 1930s. Thus, while I NWolf published a ringing editorial in the New York appreciate Roscoe's long familiarity with Papua New Times titled "They Divide and Subdivide, and Call It An- Guinea (PNG) populations and his archival work, this lack thropology" (1980). Wolf's concern was the proliferation of "history and history of theory" connection means that of mutually uncommunicative subfields in our discipline both his framing of the question at hand and his empirical to the detriment of any overarching set of understandings claims leave much to be desired. Let me elaborate. of the human condition. He laid out his larger history-of- First, I certainly agree with Roscoe on the importance thought vision in Europe and the People without History of revisiting Margaret Mead's oeuvre. Mead is still, as I (1982), in which he argued for our discipline's release wrote in Exotics at Home, "the most well-known anthro- from the "bounds of its own definitions" in an historical pologist across this century in the United States, and prob- political-economic vision uniting the social sciences and ably the world.
    [Show full text]
  • Gleanings from Academic Gatherings
    History of Anthropology Newsletter Volume 28 Issue 2 December 2001 Article 9 January 2001 Gleanings From Academic Gatherings Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/han Part of the Anthropology Commons, and the History of Science, Technology, and Medicine Commons Recommended Citation (2001) "Gleanings From Academic Gatherings," History of Anthropology Newsletter: Vol. 28 : Iss. 2 , Article 9. Available at: https://repository.upenn.edu/han/vol28/iss2/9 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/han/vol28/iss2/9 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Whitehead. N.L. 2000. A history of research on warfare in anthropology-reply to Keith Otterbein. American Anthropologist I 02 :834-3 7 Yamauchi, E.M., ed. 2001 . Afiica and Afiicans in antiquity. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press. Zamito, J.H. 2002. Kant Herder and the birth of anthropology. University ofChicago Press. Zimmerman, Andrew. 2001. Anthropology and antihmnanism in imperial Germany. University of Chicago Press. ABK= Alice Kehoe PF= Paul Friedrich CR = Chris Roth RDF= Raymond Fogelson EK= Esteban Krotz RH=Richard Handler WCS= William Sturtevant GLEANINGS FROM ACADEMIC GATHERINGS American Anthropological Association. The theme ofthe annual meeting in Washington, D.C. November 28-December 2, was "One Hundred Years of Anthropology," and the program was replete with sessions and papers relating in one way or another to the history of the discipline-too many, in fact, to attempt to list them all here in full detail. Sessions listed in the program index under the heading "History of Anthropology" were five in number: "Anthropology in the Margins: Hurston, Landes, Literaure, Folklore, Race, Renaissance," organized by Bradley Evans (Rutgers U.
    [Show full text]
  • Pacific Studies
    PACIFIC STUDIES A multidisciplinary journal devoted to the study of the peoples of the Pacific Islands JUNE 1996 Anthropology Archaeology Art History Economics Ethnomusicology Folklore Geography History Sociolinguistics Political Science Sociology PUBLISHED BY T HE INSTITUTE FOR POLYNESIAN STUDIES BRIGHAM YOUNG UNIVERSITY-HAWAI‘I IN ASSOCIATION WITH THE POLYNESIAN CULTURAL CENTER EDITORIAL BOARD Paul Alan Cox Brigham Young University Roger Green University of Auckland Francis X. Hezel, S.J. Micronesian Seminar Rubellite Johnson University of Hawai‘i Adrienne Kaeppler Smithsonian Institution Robert Kiste University of Hawai‘i Robert Langdon Australian National University Stephen Levine Victoria University Barrie Macdonald Massey University Cluny Macpherson University of Auckland Leonard Mason University of Hawai‘i Malama Meleisea University of Auckland Norman Meller University of Hawai‘i Richard M. Moyle University of Auckland Colin Newbury Oxford University Douglas Oliver University of Hawai‘i Karen Peacock University of Hawai‘i Nancy Pollock Victoria University Seigio A. Rapu Easter Island Karl Rensch Australian National University Bradd Shore Emory University Yosihiko Sinoto Bishop Museum William Tagupa Honolulu, Hawai‘i Francisco Orrego Vicuña Universidad de Chile Edward Wolfers University of Wollongong Articles and reviews in Pacific Studies are abstracted or indexed in Sociolog- ical Abstracts, Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts, America: His- toy and Life, Historical Abstracts, Abstracts in Anthropology, Anthropo- logical Literature, PAIS Bulletin, International Political Science Abstracts, International Bibliography of Periodical Literature, International Bibliogra- phy of Book Reviews, and International Bibliography of the Social Sciences. PACIFIC STUDIES Editor DALE B. ROBERTSON Associate Editor Book Review Editor GLORIA L. CRONIN MAX E. STANTON Book Review Forum Editor Book Review Forum Editor ROBERT BOROFSKY LAMONT LINDSTROM Books Noted Editor Technical Editor RILEY M.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Diversity and Human Nature: the Life and Times of a False Dichotomy
    Human Diversity and Human Nature: The Life and Times of a False Dichotomy Bradd Shore Anthropology Department Emory University Atlanta, Georgia In Being Human, Neil Roughley, ed. , Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 2001 1 I affirm, that if we consult History, both Ancient and Modern, and take a view of what has past in the World, we shall find that Human Nature since the fall of Adam has always been the same, and that the Strength and Frailties of it have ever been conspicuous in one part of thew Globe or another, without any regard to Ages, Climate or Religion . As Human nature always continues the same, as it has been for so many thousand years, so we have no great Reason to suspect a future Change in it, while the World endures. (Bernard Mandeville)1 It is very strange how certain modes of thought become general, and can, for some length of time, be maintained, and for long be regarded as something actually existing in human nature. (Johan Wolfgang Von Goethe)2 At first glance, it seems like a straightforward proposition. Anthropology is the study of human nature in light of human variation. Specifically, anthropologists focus on group variation, which, depending on the subfield of anthropology, entails some combination of biological, cultural or social differentiation. But what exactly does it mean to study human nature in light of human variation? A moment’s reflection suggests that defining human nature in light of human variation appears to be more a paradox than a simple definition. It defines not so much a discipline as a dilemma, one that has been at the heart of anthropology since its inception.
    [Show full text]
  • UNIVERSITY of CALIFORNIA, SAN DIEGO Divination and Decision
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SAN DIEGO Divination and Decision-Making: Ritual Techniques of Distributed Cognition in the Guatemalan Highlands A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Anthropology and Cognitive Science by John J. McGraw Committee in charge: Professor Steven Parish, Chair Professor David Jordan, Co-Chair Professor Paul Goldstein Professor Edwin Hutchins Professor Craig McKenzie 2016 Copyright John J. McGraw, 2016 All rights reserved. The dissertation of John J. McGraw is approved, and it is acceptable in quality and form for publication on microfilm and electronically: ___________________________________________________________ ___________________________________________________________ ___________________________________________________________ ___________________________________________________________ Co-chair ___________________________________________________________ Chair University of California, San Diego 2016 iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Signature Page …....……………………………………………………………… iii Table of Contents ………………….……………………………….…………….. iv List of Figures ….…………………………………………………….…….……. vii List of Tables …......……………………………………………………………… viii List of Maps ………………………………………………………….……….…. ix Acknowledgments …..………………………………………..………………….. x Vita .………………….………………………………….………..……………… xiii Abstract of the Dissertation ..……………………………………………………. xv Chapter 1: INTRODUCTION …….………………………………..................... 1 Theoretical Background …………….…………..….……………… 2 Distributed Cognition
    [Show full text]
  • Developments in Polynesian Ethnology
    Developments in Polynesian Ethnology Developments in Polynesian Ethnology Edited by Alan Howard and Robert Borofsky Open Access edition funded by the National Endowment for the Humanities / Andrew W. Mellon Foundation Humanities Open Book Program. Licensed under the terms of Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 In- ternational (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0), which permits readers to freely download and share the work in print or electronic format for non-commercial purposes, so long as credit is given to the author. Derivative works and commercial uses require per- mission from the publisher. For details, see https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/. The Cre- ative Commons license described above does not apply to any material that is separately copyrighted. Open Access ISBNs: 9780824881962 (PDF) 9780824881955 (EPUB) This version created: 20 May, 2019 Please visit www.hawaiiopen.org for more Open Access works from University of Hawai‘i Press. ©1989 University of Hawaii Press All rights reserved For Jan and Nancy CONTENTS Title Page ii Copyright 3 Dedication iv Acknowledgments vi 1. Introduction ALAN HOWARD AND ROBERT BOROFSKY 1 2. Prehistory PATRICK V. KIRCH 14 3. Social Organization ALAN HOWARD AND JOHN KIRKPATRICK 52 4. Socialization and Character Development JANE RITCHIE AND JAMES RITCHIE 105 5. Mana and Tapu BRADD SHORE 151 6. Chieftainship GEORGE E. MARCUS 187 7. Art and Aesthetics ADRIENNE L. KAEPPLER 224 8. The Early Contact Period ROBERT BOROFSKY AND ALAN HOWARD 257 9. Looking Ahead ROBERT BOROFSKY AND ALAN HOWARD 290 Bibliography 307 Contributors 407 Notes 410 v ACKNOWLEDGMENTS A VARIETY of people contributed to the preparation of this book.
    [Show full text]
  • Downloaded From: Ebooks.Adelaide.Edu.Au/D/Descartes/Rene/D44dm/Part2.Html, Sept
    A History of Anthropology Eriksen HOA3 00 pre 1 16/04/2013 16:04 Anthropology, Culture and Society Series Editors: Professor Vered Amit, Concordia University and Dr Jon P. Mitchell, University of Sussex Published titles include: Claiming Individuality: Discordant Development: The Aid Effect: The Cultural Politics of Global Capitalism and the Giving and Governing in Distinction Struggle for Connection in International Development EDITED BY VERED AMIT AND Bangladesh EDITED BY DavID MOSSE AND NOEL DYCK KATY GARDNER DavID LEWIS Community, Cosmopolitanism Anthropology, Development Cultivating Development: and the Problem of Human and the Post-Modern An Ethnography of Aid Policy Commonality Challenge and Practice VERED AMIT AND KATY GARDNER AND DavID MOSSE NIGEL RAPPORT DavID LEWIS Contesting Publics Home Spaces, Street Styles: Border Watch: Feminism, Activism, Contesting Power and Identity Cultures of Immigration, Ethnography in a South African City Detention and Control LYNNE PHILLIPS AND SALLY COLE LESLIE J. BANK ALEXANDRA HALL Terror and Violence: In Foreign Fields: Corruption: Imagination and the The Politics and Experiences Anthropological Perspectives Unimaginable of Transnational Sport EDITED BY DIETER HALLER AND EDITED BY ANDREW STRATHERN, Migration CRIS SHORE PAMELA J. STEWART AND THOMAS F. CARTER Anthropology’s World: NEIL L. WHITEHEAD On the Game: Life in a Twenty-First Century Anthropology, Art and Women and Sex Work Discipline Cultural Production SOPHIE DAY ULF HANNERZ MAruškA SvašEK Slave of Allah: Humans and Other Animals Race
    [Show full text]