On Persuasion and Paternalism: Lawyer Decisionmaking and the Questionably Competent Client Paul R

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

On Persuasion and Paternalism: Lawyer Decisionmaking and the Questionably Competent Client Paul R Boston College Law School Digital Commons @ Boston College Law School Boston College Law School Faculty Papers January 1987 On Persuasion and Paternalism: Lawyer Decisionmaking and the Questionably Competent Client Paul R. Tremblay Boston College Law School, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://lawdigitalcommons.bc.edu/lsfp Part of the Legal Ethics and Professional Responsibility Commons, Legal Profession Commons, and the Litigation Commons Recommended Citation Paul R. Tremblay. "On Persuasion and Paternalism: Lawyer Decisionmaking and the Questionably Competent Client." Utah Law Review no.3 (1987): 515-584. This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Digital Commons @ Boston College Law School. It has been accepted for inclusion in Boston College Law School Faculty Papers by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ Boston College Law School. For more information, please contact [email protected]. On Persuasion and Paternalism: Lawyer Decisionmaking and the Questionably Competent Client Paul R. Tremblay* The increasing recognition that the attorney-client relation- ship is governed by considerations of informed consentl inevitably creates considerable uncertainty for attorneys representing clients whose competence is in question. The ideology of informed consent asserts that lawyers may act only, or primarily, on the direction of their clients. Lawyers serve, in this fundamental sense, as agents of their clients. The supposition that lawyers know what is best for their clients is no longer as accepted as it may have been in the * Assistant Clinical Professor of Law, Boston College Law School; J.D. 1978, UCLA; B.A. 1973, Boston College. A somewhat different version of this Article was presented at the UCLA-University of Warwick International Clinical Conference in Los Angeles, California, in October, 1986. I wish to thank Mark Spiegel and Alexis Anderson for their helpful and insightful comments on earlier drafts of this work. To infer that each agrees with all of the conclusions drawn here, however, would be a mistake. I also wish to thank Andrew Sharp, Maris Abbene, and Michele Lerner for their valuable research assistance. 1. Much has been written over the past two decades about informed consent and its applicability to lawyering. Several commentators and scholars have explored the general question of who controls whom within the attorney-client dyad. See, e.g., J. HEINZ & E. LAUMANN, CHICAGO LAWYERS: THE SOCIAL STRUCTURE OF THE BAR (1982); D. ROSENTHAL, LAWYER AND CLIENT, WHO'S IN CHARGE? (1974); Heinz, The Power of Lawyers, 17 GA. L. REV. 891 (1983); Kritzer, The Dimensions of Lawyer-Client Relationships: Notes Toward a The- ory and a Field Study, 1984 AM. B. FOUND. RES. J. 409; Mazor, Power and Responsibility in the Attorney-Client Relation, 20 STAN. L. REV. 1120 (1968). Other writers have focused that theme by addressing the "doctrine" of informed consent in law. See, e.g., Martyn, Informed Consent and the Legal Profession, 48 GEO. WASH. L. REV. 307 (1980); Maute, Allocation of Decisionmaking Authority Under the Model Rules of Professional Conduct, 17 D.C. DAVIS L. REV. 1049 (1984); Peck, A New Tort Liability for Lack of Informed Consent in Legal Matters, 44 LA. L. REV. 1289 (1984); Spiegel, Lawyering and Client Decisionmaking: In- formed Consent and the Legal Profession, 128 U. PA. L. REV. 41 (1979); Strauss, Toward a Revised Model of Attorney-Client Relationship: The Argument for Autonomy, 65 N.C.L. REV. 315 (1987); cf. S. GILLERS & N. DORSEN, REGULATION OF LAWYERS: PROBLEMS OF LAW & ETHICS 319 (1985) (informed consent an "emerging right"). Still others have approached the issue from a more philosophical perspective, with particular reference to the virtues of "cli- ent-centeredness." See, e.g., D. BINDER & S. PRICE, LEGAL INTERVIEWING AND COUNSELING: A CLIENT CENTERED APPROACH (1977); T. SHAFFER, ON BEING A CHRISTIAN AND A LAWYER 3-20 (1981); Katz, On Professional Responsibility, 80 COMM. L.J. 380 (1975); Lehman, The Pur- suit of a Client's Interest, 77 MICH. L. REV., 1078 (1979); Shaffer, Christian Theories of Professional Responsibility, 48 S. CAL. L. REV. 721, 728-38 (1974). Despite this increasing attention to informed consent, the literature thus far has failed to address informed consent considerations in the context of less-than-competent clients. 515 516 UTAH LAW REVIEW [1987: 515 past; instead, the profession has grown in the realization that the most effective lawyering decisions are made by clients themselves.2 There are times, however, when a lawyer adhering to this ideology believes that the client, whether because of advanced age, mental or physical illness, or similar disability, is not capable of. making informed choices, and if permitted to do so will injure himself or, at a minimum, fail to achieve the objectives of the representation. This Article attempts, at least in a preliminary fashion, to explore the avenues that might be open to an attorney in these circum- stances. The term "preliminary" should be stressed, as the nature of the relationship between a lawyer and her possibly incompetent client is difficult to discern. The thoughts developed here can only begin to structure a workable theory for that interaction. The issue of informed consent and the impaired client has been addressed seldom, if ever, in the legal context. The matter has been explored somewhat more fully in the medical context,3 where the ramifications of the informed consent doctrine in general have been debated more intensely.4 This Article's discussion of the role of the lawyer representing an impaired client, therefore, will 2. See, e.g., D. BINDER & S. PRICE, supra note I, at 147-53; Freedman, A Lawyer Doesn't Always Know Best, 7 HUM. RTS. 28, 52 (1978); Shaffer, supra note 1, at 728; see also infra notes 31-48 and accompanying text. 3. See, e.g., PRESIDENT'S COMM'N FOR THE STUDY OF ETHICAL PROBLEMS IN MEDICINE AND BIOMEDICAL AND BEHAVIORAL RESEARCH, 1 MAKING HEALTH CARE DECISIONS: THE ETHI- CAL AND LEGAL IMPLICATIONS OF INFORMED CONSENT IN THE PATIENT-PRACTITIONER RELATION- SHIP 167-88 (1982) [hereinafter MAKING HEALTH CARE DECISIONS]; S. JORDAN, DECISIONMAK- ING FOR INCOMPETENT PERSONS: THE LAW AND MORALITY OF WHO SHALL DECIDE (1985); Brant, Last Rights: An Analysis of Refusal and Withholding of Treatment Cases, 46 Mo. L. REV. 337, 347-61 (1981); Meisel, The "Exceptions" to the Informed Consent Doctrine: Striking a Balance Between Competing Values in Medical Decisionmaking, 1979 WIS. L. REV. 413, 439; Solnick, Proxy Consent for Incompetent Non-Terminally III Adult Patients, 6 J. LEGAL MED. 1 (1985). This is but a small sampling of the voluminous literature on treatment and competence in the medical field. It is not surprising that this issue has received greater attention in medical circles, be- cause medical decisions are made almost invariably in the context of illness, which may impair consent and create hard choices for physicians. This fundamental difference between doctoring and lawyering, however, cannot justify the paucity of literature on competent in- formed consent in the legal context. Lawyers also represent ill clients, and the crises that precipitate a decision to seek legal help might impair rational decisionmaking. 4. See, e.g., P. ApPELBAUM, C. LIDZ & A. MEISEL, INFORMED CONSENT (1987); R. BURT, TAKING CARE OF STRANGERS: THE RULE OF LAW IN DOCTOR-PATIENT RELATIONS (1979); J. CHILDRESS, WHO SHOULD DECIDE? PATERNALISM IN HEALTH CARE (1982); J. KATZ & A. CAPRON, CATASTROPHIC DISEASES: WHO DECIDES WHAT? (1975); Hagman, The Medical Pa- tient's Right to Know: Report on a Medical-Legal-Ethical Empirical Study, 17 UCLA L. REV. 758 (1970); Meisel & Roth, Toward an Informed Discussion of Informed Consent: A Review and Critique of the Empirical Studies, 25 ARIZ. L. REV. 265 (1983); Plant, The De- cline of "Informed Consent," 35 WASH. & LEE L. REV. 91 (1978). No.3] LAWYER DECISIONMAKING 517 draw on the suggestions developed for physicians, I) but for several reasons what works for medical professionals may not work equally well for lawyers. The lawyer's great difficulty in proceeding with representation of a confused client should be apparent. The most obvious problem is the considerable tension between the ethical requirement6 that the lawyer permit the client to make decisions and the lawyer's considered judgment that to do so not only would fail to achieve the purposes of the representation but also would be to follow an instruction that the client would not give in more lucid times.' Looming in the 1?ackground is the doctrine of presumed compe- tence, asserting that it is not permissible ethically or legally for one person (the lawyer) unilaterally to usurp the authority of another (the client) without either that person's consent or court permis- sion.8 For lawyers in particular this dilemma is heightened because they face a peculiar role tension that may serve to prohibit any attempt on their part to obtain court permission to usurp a client's decisionmaking authority. A lawyer who seeks to establish a guard- ianship or similar proxy for her client will run afoul of numerous professional responsibility concerns, most notably those of loyalty,9 5. This is especially true when the role of the family in proxy decisionmaking, and the role of persuasion and "fraternal correction" in obtaining consent, are addressed. See infra notes 220-31 and 276-85 and accompanying text. 6. To characterize the doctrine of informed consent as an "ethical requirement"
Recommended publications
  • Social Norms and Social Influence Mcdonald and Crandall 149
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Social norms and social influence Rachel I McDonald and Christian S Crandall Psychology has a long history of demonstrating the power and and their imitation is not enough to implicate social reach of social norms; they can hardly be overestimated. To norms. Imitation is common enough in many forms of demonstrate their enduring influence on a broad range of social life — what creates the foundation for culture and society phenomena, we describe two fields where research continues is not the imitation, but the expectation of others for when to highlight the power of social norms: prejudice and energy imitation is appropriate, and when it is not. use. The prejudices that people report map almost perfectly onto what is socially appropriate, likewise, people adjust their A social norm is an expectation about appropriate behav- energy use to be more in line with their neighbors. We review ior that occurs in a group context. Sherif and Sherif [8] say new approaches examining the effects of norms stemming that social norms are ‘formed in group situations and from multiple groups, and utilizing normative referents to shift subsequently serve as standards for the individual’s per- behaviors in social networks. Though the focus of less research ception and judgment when he [sic] is not in the group in recent years, our review highlights the fundamental influence situation. The individual’s major social attitudes are of social norms on social behavior. formed in relation to group norms (pp. 202–203).’ Social norms, or group norms, are ‘regularities in attitudes and Address behavior that characterize a social group and differentiate Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 66045, it from other social groups’ [9 ] (p.
    [Show full text]
  • Persuasive Communication and Advertising Efficacy for Public Health Policy: a Critical Approach
    Persuasive communication and advertising efficacy for public health policy : A critical approach. Vincent Coppola, Odile Camus To cite this version: Vincent Coppola, Odile Camus. Persuasive communication and advertising efficacy for public health policy : A critical approach.. 2009. halshs-00410054 HAL Id: halshs-00410054 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-00410054 Preprint submitted on 17 Aug 2009 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Communication Research Persuasive communication and advertising efficacy for public health policy: a critical approach For Peer Review Journal: Communication Research Manuscript ID: CR-09-118 Manuscript Type: Original Research Article Persuasive communication, Pragmatics, Social advertising, Health Keywords: communication Our study is at the juncture of two themes largely investigated by researchers in communication, namely persuasion and pragmatics. First, we recall some tenets of these two topics. In particular, we refer to the dual process theories of the persuasion and the inferential model of pragmatics. Second we develop the argument that studies in persuasive communication have been undertaken Abstract: until now within the Encoding/Decoding paradigm without considering enough the main ideas of the pragmatics and intentionalist paradigm about communication and language.
    [Show full text]
  • Managing Social Norms for Persuasive Impact
    SOCIAL INFLUENCE, 2006, 1 (1), 3–15 Managing social norms for persuasive impact Robert B. Cialdini and Linda J. Demaine Arizona State University, USA Brad J. Sagarin Northern Illinois University, USA Daniel W. Barrett Western Connecticut State University, USA Kelton Rhoads University of Southern California, USA Patricia L. Winter US Forest Service, CA, USA In order to mobilise action against a social problem, public service communicators often include normative information in their persuasive appeals. Such messages can be either effective or ineffective because they can normalise either desirable or undesirable conduct. To examine the implications in an environmental context, visitors to Arizona’s Petrified Forest National Park were exposed to messages that admonished against the theft of petrified wood. In addition, the messages conveyed information either about descriptive norms (the levels of others’ behaviour) or injunctive norms (the levels of others’ disapproval) regarding such thievery. Results showed that focusing message recipients on descriptive normative information was most likely to increase theft, whereas focusing them on injunctive normative information was most likely to suppress it. Recommendations are offered for optimising the impact of normative messages in situations characterised by objectionable levels of undesirable conduct. After decades of debate concerning their causal impact, (e.g., Berkowitz, 1972; Darley & Latane´, 1970; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; Krebs, 1970; Krebs & Address correspondence to: Robert B. Cialdini, Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104, USA. Email: [email protected] This research was partially funded through grants from the United States Forest Service (PSW-96-0022CA) and the Arizona Department of Environmental Quality (No.-98-154C/97- 0152AI).
    [Show full text]
  • Society Persuasion In
    PERSUASION IN SOCIETY HERBERT W. SIMONS with JOANNE MORREALE and BRUCE GRONBECK Table of Contents List of Artwork in Persuasion in Society xiv About the Author xvii Acknowledgments xix Preface xx Part 1: Understanding Persuasion 1. The Study of Persuasion 3 Defining Persuasion 5 Why Is Persuasion Important? 10 Studying Persuasion 14 The Behavioral Approach: Social-Scientific Research on the Communication-Persuasion Matrix 15 The Critical Studies Approach: Case Studies and “Genre-alizations” 17 Summary 20 Questions and Projects for Further Study 21 2. The Psychology of Persuasion: Basic Principles 25 Beliefs and Values as Building Blocks of Attitudes 27 Persuasion by Degrees: Adapting to Different Audiences 29 Schemas: Attitudes as Knowledge Structures 32 From Attitudes to Actions: The Role of Subjective Norms 34 Elaboration Likelihood Model: Two Routes to Persuasion 34 Persuasion as a Learning Process 36 Persuasion as Information Processing 37 Persuasion and Incentives 38 Persuasion by Association 39 Persuasion as Psychological Unbalancing and Rebalancing 40 Summary 41 Questions and Projects for Further Study 42 3. Persuasion Broadly Considered 47 Two Levels of Communication: Content and Relational 49 Impression Management 51 Deception About Persuasive Intent 51 Deceptive Deception 52 Expression Games 54 Persuasion in the Guise of Objectivity 55 Accounting Statements and Cost-Benefit Analyses 55 News Reporting 56 Scientific Reporting 57 History Textbooks 58 Reported Discoveries of Social Problems 59 How Multiple Messages Shape Ideologies 59 The Making of McWorld 63 Summary 66 Questions and Projects for Further Study 68 Part 2: The Coactive Approach 4. Coactive Persuasion 73 Using Receiver-Oriented Approaches 74 Being Situation Sensitive 76 Combining Similarity and Credibility 79 Building on Acceptable Premises 82 Appearing Reasonable and Providing Psychological Income 85 Using Communication Resources 86 Summary 88 Questions and Projects for Further Study 89 5.
    [Show full text]
  • Advocacy Toolbox and the Art of Persuasion
    Module 5 Lesson Advocacy Toolbox and 1 the Art of Persuasion Lesson at a Glance To create a public lands advocacy toolbox and Objective: Legislative Process Review (10 min): practice persuasive letter writing and arguments. Participants will put process cards in order and discuss where they can get involved in the legislative process. (10 min) The Advocacy Toolbox (10 min): Legislative Process Review Participants will brainstorm tangible tools 1. Explain to participants that having a basic understanding of the federal decision- in the advocacy toolbox. making process can help you be actively involved in public lands protection. Practicing Persuasive Writing (30 min): Participants will choose issues to practice 2. Divide participants into groups of 2-5 people and pass out sets of legislative writing letters to their representatives and process cards to each group. Allow 2-3 minutes for each group to put the local policy makers. process cards in order. Allow each group to report out and discuss the results. As you review the correct order, ask the group where they think it’s possible for Conclusion (10 min): Share out, constituents to get involved in the process, noted by the **. legislative process and toolbox review, and questions. a. Idea for change - an individual or group is motivated to make their community better. **These come from you! b. Coalition building - a group of concerned and/or affected people, businesses, Learner Outcomes and organizations work together to create a shared vision for a legislative Participants will: proposal. **Get involved with organizations that are part of coalitions looking to put forward legislation, attend informational meetings, tell businesses that • Review the basic legislative process you support their work, help gather letters of support and petition signatures.
    [Show full text]
  • The Relationship Between Religious Persuasion and Climate Change Attitudes in Australia
    The Relationship between Religious Persuasion and Climate Change Attitudes in Australia Mark Morrisona,b, Roderick Duncanb,c, Kevin Partona,b and Chris Sherleya a School of Management and Marketing, Charles Sturt University b Institute for Land, Water and Society, Charles Sturt University c School of Accounting and Finance, Charles Sturt University Contributed paper prepared for presentation at the 57th AARES Annual Conference, Sydney, New South Wales, 5th-8th February, 2013 © Copyright 2013 by Authors’ names. All rights reserved. Readers may make verbatim copies of this document for non- commercial purposes by any means, provided that this copyright notice appears on all such copies. ii The Relationship between Religious Persuasion and Climate Change Attitudes in Australia Mark Morrisona,b, Roderick Duncanb,c, Kevin Partona,b and Chris Sherleya a School of Management and Marketing, Charles Sturt University b Institute for Land, Water and Society, Charles Sturt University c School of Accounting and Finance, Charles Sturt University Abstract Previous research has demonstrated that religious persuasion can have an impact on environmental attitudes, however less research of this kind has focused on the relationship between religious persuasion and climate change attitudes. Using a survey of 1,927 Australians we examined links between membership of five religious groupings and climate change attitudes, as well as membership of climate change household segments that differ in their acceptance of human induced climate change and the need for policy responses. Differences were found across religious groups in terms of their belief in human induced climate change, consensus among scientists, their own efficacy and the need for policy responses.
    [Show full text]
  • Analyzing the Effect of Persuasion Component on the Audience
    Journal of Politics and Law; Vol. 9, No. 10; 2016 ISSN 1913-9047 E-ISSN 1913-9055 Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education Analyzing the Effect of Persuasion Component on the Audience Mehran Ebrahimi Nejad1 & Mahdi Sharifi2 1 Payame Noor University, Tehran, Iran 2 Faculty Member of Tehran University, Tehran, Iran Correspondence: Mehran Ebrahimi Nejad, Payame Noor University, Tehran, Iran. E-mail: [email protected] Received: July 19, 2016 Accepted: August 4, 2016 Online Published: November 30, 2016 doi:10.5539/jpl.v9n10p138 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/jpl.v9n10p138 Abstract Nowadays, persuasion is considered as one of the most pivotal components of social influence. It can promote or change the current attitude of the audience. Considering this component, the main objective of persuasion is creating motivation in the audience to meet their needs. Therefore, the current research aims at analyzing the effect of persuasion on the audience. Both qualitative and quantitative research methods have been used in this research. The researcher has used both library and field methods as well as Delphi method to gather data. Also, one-sample t-test has been used to analyze the validity of the findings. The results indicate that in addition to the attraction criterion, some other criteria including excitement, feelings, and verbal skills affect the attitudes of the audience. Also it can be concluded that both persuasion model and validity have significant effects on persuasion component. On the other hand, the objective and validity of the audience don’t have significant effect on the audiences’ persuasion. Keywords: media, persuasion, validity 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Information Design, Bayesian Persuasion, and Bayes Correlated Equilibrium†
    American Economic Review: Papers & Proceedings 2016, 106(5): 586–591 http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.p20161046 INFORMATION DESIGN AND BAYESIAN PERSUASION ‡ Information Design, Bayesian Persuasion, and Bayes Correlated Equilibrium† By Dirk Bergemann and Stephen Morris* A set of players have preferences over a set However, in each case, there is a revelation of outcomes. Consider the problem of an “infor- principle argument that allows for the analysis mation designer” who can choose an informa- of all information structures or all mechanisms, tion structure for the players to serve his ends, respectively. For the mechanism design prob- but has no ability to change the mechanism lem, we can restrict attention to direct mecha- or force the players to make particular action nisms where the players’ action sets are equal choices( . A mechanism here describes the set to their possible types. Conversely, for the infor- of players,) their available actions, and a map- mation design problem, we can restrict attention ping from action profiles to outcomes. Contrast to information structures where the players’ type this “information design” problem with the sets are equal to their action sets. In this note, “mechanism design” problem, where a “mech- using this observation, we consider information anism designer” can choose a mechanism for design problems when all information structures the players to serve his ends, but has no ability are available to the designer. to choosing the information structure or force When there are many players, but the infor- the players to make particular action choices( .1 mation designer or “mediator” has no informa- In each case, the problem is sometimes studied) tional advantage( over the players,) this problem with a restricted choice set.
    [Show full text]
  • DR Persuasion TG
    A TEACHER’S GUIDE TO THE PENGUIN EDITION OF JANE AUSTEN’S PERSUASION By DIANA MITCHELL, Ph.D. SERIES EDITORS: W. GEIGER ELLIS, ED.D., UNIVERSITY OF GEORGIA, EMERITUS and ARTHEA J. S. REED, PH.D., UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA, RETIRED A Teacher’s Guide to Jane Austen’s Persuasion 2 INTRODUCTION Persuasion by Jane Austen is a novel rich in intrigue and romance. Although Austen’s focus seems to be the manners and morals of the time, this concern is embedded in the tentative relationship between Anne Elliot and Captain Frederick Wentworth and in whether the relationship will flower again to the heights of the love they once had. On the most basic level Persuasion is a love story, both interesting and entertaining. On a deeper level it examines human foibles and societal flaws. The question of the importance of propriety is raised frequently as is the issue of appearance vs. reality. Family relationships and duty to family are both foci of the story. Within this family context relationships between men and women are examined, and it is interesting to see how male and female relationships have changed since the late 18th century. Of course, love and marriage are at the center of the story since it is through love that Austen brings into play most of the other issues. For fans of television soap operas, the subtleties of the novel will leap to life. For those raised on action/adventure stories, the plot will seem slow. The novel’s lack of action and antiquated language and mores will probably be the biggest hurdles teachers will face in involving students in reading Austen’s novel.
    [Show full text]
  • Information Technology Enabled Persuasion: an Experimental Investigation of the Role of Communication Channel, Strategy and Affect
    AIS Transactions on Human-Computer Interaction Volume 9 Issue 4 Article 2 12-2017 Information Technology Enabled Persuasion: An Experimental Investigation of the Role of Communication Channel, Strategy and Affect Haiqing Li City of Hope, [email protected] Samir Chatterjee Claremont Graduate University, [email protected] Ozgur Turetken Ryerson University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://aisel.aisnet.org/thci Recommended Citation Li, H., Chatterjee, S., & Turetken, O. (2017). Information Technology Enabled Persuasion: An Experimental Investigation of the Role of Communication Channel, Strategy and Affect. AIS Transactions on Human- Computer Interaction, 9(4), 281-300. https://doi.org/10.17705/1thci.00099 DOI: 10.17705/1thci.00099 This material is brought to you by the AIS Journals at AIS Electronic Library (AISeL). It has been accepted for inclusion in AIS Transactions on Human-Computer Interaction by an authorized administrator of AIS Electronic Library (AISeL). For more information, please contact [email protected]. ransactions on T uman — omputer nteraction H C I Research Note Information Technology Enabled Persuasion: An Experimental Investigation of the Role of Communication Channel, Strategy and Affect Haiqing Li City of Hope, USA Samir Chatterjee Ozgur Turetken Claremont University, USA [email protected] Ryerson University, Canada Abstract: With advances in information and communication technologies (ICT), organizations of various forms now deploy an increasing numBer of ICT-enabled persuasive systems in several domains. Traditional computer-mediated communication (CMC) theories mainly focus on the effectiveness of media in the synchronous/asynchronous spectrum for effectively matching medium with communication task. The contemporary communication environment is rich with asynchronous channels such as email, WeB, and teXt messaging, which makes it important to go Beyond synchronicity and determine the nuances among various asynchronous channels.
    [Show full text]
  • Pragmatic Persuasion
    1 RUNNING HEAD: Pragmatic persuasion Pragmatic Persuasion or the persuasion Paradox Michaela Wänke University of Basel, Switzerland Leonie Reutner University of Basel, Switzerland Draft for the Sydney Symposium on Attitudes and Persuasion, March 2009 Pragmatic persuasion 2 PRAGMATIC PERSUASION OR THE PERSUASION PARADOX There are many ways to change attitudes as this book attests and many involve presenting arguments (see for example chapters X). Consequently, the impact of arguments has figured prominently in the history of attitude research where the characteristics of persuasive messages (Hovland, Janis & Kelley, 1953) and the processes by which they affect attitudes (Chen & Chaiken, 1999; Petty & Wegener, 1999; Kruglanski, Thompson & Siegel, 1999; Greenwald, 1968) have been investigated (for reviews see Bohner & Wänke, 2002; Crano & Prislin, 2006). That a convincing argument comes in handy when attempting to change attitudes is, of course, common knowledge and is practiced widely in social interaction, education, politics, marketing and other areas. But what is a convincing argument? In logic, an argument is a set of premises and a conclusion, with the characteristic that the truth of the conclusion is supported by the premises. “You should eat vegetables (conclusion) because they contain lots of vitamins (premise)”, sounds like a reasonable argument to most people whereas “you should eat vegetables because they are green”, does not. The intake of vitamins is considered beneficial by most people but the advantages of eating green food are less obvious. But note, that it is not the information per se which is convincing or not, but what receivers make of it. For information to change a person’s attitude in the desired direction it is essential that the receiver draws the adequate inferences about its implications.
    [Show full text]
  • Persuasion with Motivated Beliefs
    Persuasion with Motivated Beliefs David Hagmann and George Loewenstein Department of Social and Decision Sciences, Carnegie Mellon University December 21, 2018 Abstract Considerable empirical research nds that people derive utility not only from consump- tion, but also from their beliefs about themselves and the world. Rather than dispas- sionately updating their views in response to new information, such belief-based utility implies that people at times avoid information and use other strategies to protect their existing beliefs. We present a two-stage model of persuasion in the presence of belief- protecting strategies and test it in an incentive-compatible task. In the experiment, persuaders seek to shift receivers' subjective numeric estimates related to emotionally charged topics, such as abortion and racial discrimination. We manipulate whether the persuader rst acknowledges her own lack of certainty and whether she rst has an op- portunity to build rapport with the receiver. Though these elements of communication ought to be irrelevant or even backre under the standard account, our theory predicts they will enhance persuasiveness. We nd that acknowledging doubt leads to a greater change in the receivers' beliefs, but nd no eect for building rapport. Moreover, we nd that persuaders end up changing their own estimates after writing a persuasive message. Those who presented a strong argument (as judged by third party raters) end up revising their own estimate in the direction of their argument, while those who provided a weak argument update in the opposite direction. 1 1 Introduction The acquisition of information holds the promise to improve the quality of our decisions and, hence, our well-being.
    [Show full text]