Neurohistory'

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Neurohistory' Comment on Rob Boddice, 'Neurohistory' The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Smail, Daniel Lord. "Comment on Rob Boddice, 'Neurohistory'," in Debating New Approaches to History, ed. Marek Tamm and Peter Burke (London: Bloomsbury Academic, 2019), 313–318. Citable link https://nrs.harvard.edu/URN-3:HUL.INSTREPOS:37366512 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Open Access Policy Articles, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#OAP Preprint MS version 15 April 2018. Published as Daniel Lord Smail, "Comment on Rob Boddice, 'Neurohistory'," in Debating New Approaches to History, ed. Marek Tamm and 1Peter Burke (London: Bloomsbury AcademiC, 2019), 313–18. When I first suggested the idea of a neurohistory some years ago it seemed obvious to me that history could learn something from neurosCienCe and vice versa. It still seems obvious to me. One of history’s great contributions to the human sCienCes springs from the historian’s instinct to suppose that nearly everything in the human world is subject to change. Looking around, it would be easy enough for a casual observer to imagine that things like the nation-state, sex roles, and ketchup are timeless givens. The work of history is to show that the modern instantiations of these things, appearances notwithstanding, actually came into being in the past, have present-day forms that are different from those of the past, and will continue to change in the future. OnCe upon a time, it seemed perfectly obvious that the brain-body system was one of those timeless givens, and therefore uninteresting to practitioners of a discipline concerned only with things that can change. Then Came the revolution. Over the last two or three deCades, Cognitive neuroscience and related fields, by virtue of what they have demonstrated about the plasticity of neurons, receptors, and other features of the Central nervous system, have made it possible to add the brain-body system to the list of things that we can usefully historicize. The brain-body system, in the neurohistorical approach, is a system whose form can change significantly over time even as the Components remain stable. The changes to the form are not random; they emerge instead from a CompleX danCe involving the body on the 1 1 2one hand and human soCiety and Culture on the other. As individual bodies continuously adapt and respond to their surroundings from one era to the neXt, the aggregate body, something we Can define heuristiCally as a statistiCally averaged set of bodily traits characteristic of a sub-population, is different from the aggregate body of eras both before and after. Crucially, the human brain-body system, in its actions and reactions to its cultural surroundings, is not simply passive, like clay in the hands of a potter. Neurohistory instead treats the body as an actor in history. Through interactions with Cultural forms and social practices, the body constantly generates unpredictable new patterns or historical trajeCtories. The body, therefore, is one of the sources of contingency in the human past. Since On Deep History and the Brain was published in 2008, the theoretical resourCes available for the framing of neurohistory have grown apaCe. ApproaChes or bodies of theory inCluding epigenetiCs, Actor-Network Theory, the history of emotions, miCrobiomiCs, and the arChaeology of entanglement, some of whiCh have been synthesized in the emerging field known as the New Materialism, have transformed the theoretiCal grounds on which we can base a neurohistorical approach to the past (Coole and Frost 2010; LeCain 2017). Understandably, certain ideas or phrases used in the book no longer sit well with current scholarship. Metaphors such as ‘wiring’ were inapt even at the time, and although I used the word ‘brain’ both in the title and in the book, this was a too- convenient shorthand for a muCh more CompleX and interesting entity, the brain-body system. From time to time, I have used small thought-pieCes to make pieCemeal adjustments to the basiC model (e.g. Smail 2014). But it is clear that neurohistory is overdue for the kind of theoretical overhaul proposed by Rob Boddice in his contribution. 2 2 In this response, I will summarize some of the highlights of Boddice’s own approach to neurohistory and the challenging and interesting revision he proposes, and will use the oCCasion to amplify or eXtend some of his important observations. As I shall suggest, the approach he has laid out here runs the risk of removing the keystone that supports the edifice of neurohistory. That keystone consists of a materialist understanding according to whiCh the body is a CompleX assemblage of eleCtriCal impulses and ChemiCals, all of whiCh influenCe mood, feeling, emotion, and behavior. This assemblage, by virtue of its material properties, is susceptible to environmental influenCes. Recent work has shown that our own miCrobiome is not the least of these faCtors (Cryan and Dinan 2012). In this materialist understanding, neurohistory should be seen as a branCh of environmental history, allowing the brain-body system to be viewed as a node in a CompleX entanglement of historical actors. Ultimately, I believe it may be more appropriate to interpret BoddiCe’s model not as a revision to neurohistory so muCh as a neCessary and useful Complement that expands the approach to inClude forms of historical explanation that operate at miCrohistoriCal sCales. Boddice has proposed the possibility of reframing neurohistory in a way that will allow it to harmonize with the history of eXperienCe and the history of emotion. His approach is designed in part to solve one of the CruCial methodologiCal problems assoCiated with neurohistory. Any approaCh to the past informed by neurosCienCe runs up against the fact that we Cannot aCtually know anything about the movement of ChemiCals or the action of synapses in the bodies of actors in the past. Even if we could devise a way to assemble such evidence, perhaps via ChemiCal analyses of bone or tissue, it is diffiCult to imagine how any of these findings Could be meaningful, given the fact that neurohistory treats only with 3 aggregates and never with individuals. What ‘evidence’ we have, therefore, is rarely more than suggestive, and one is reduced to constructing plausible scenarios or models grounded in very little that any would regard as evidence. The modern evidenCe generated by neurosCientifiC researCh is robust, but eXtrapolating from this to the past in a simple- minded way would violate the very premise of the approaCh. Boddice has offered an intriguing way to skirt this methodologiCal pitfall, namely, by taking experience itself as the subject of analysis. If we proCeed from the assumption that human eXperienCe is shaped by ConteXt, then we Can take the Changing forms of individual eXperienCe as the direCt subjeCt of historiCal inquiry without the need to worry overmuCh about whether that Change has any measurable material dimension. To use Jeremy Burman’s terminology, we Can seek to develop a ‘history from within’ (Burman 2014). BoddiCe’s fasCinating disCussion of pain offers a useful model of this approach. Pain research in recent years has shown that the relationship between an injury and the experienCe of pain is not nearly as automatiC as the Commonsense understanding suggests. Pain can be experienced in very different ways, including (sometimes) not at all. To some degree, individuals are Capable of managing their own pain. To the eXtent that people take their cues from Culture, this research suggests that whole cultures may have tools or devices that modify the pain thresholds of those who partiCipate in the culture. Furthemore, Cultural norms Can assoCiate eXperienCes of pain with stimuli that vary from one society to the next. Given these insights, we can take historical accounts of pain that seemed bizarre or ineXpliCable to previous historians and treat them as reasonably accurate reports of individual experience. 4 In an important section, Boddice appropriately loCates neurohistory’s grounding principle in the understanding that the human brain-body system, far from being fiXed or hard-wired in some genetiC way, is partially open to environmental or developmental influences and permeable to Culture. That he chose to label this section ‘Genetics’ rather than ‘Epigenetics’ is something of a misdireCtion, in my view, but the ensuing disCussion conveys well why it is that most historians have ceased to use the word ‘biology’ as if it were synonymous with ‘universal’, ‘unchanging’, and ‘hardwired’. One of the problems with this syllogism, which used to be widespread, always lay in the fact that this is not what ‘biology’ means to evolutionary biologists, a group whose opinion on the matter is worth listening to. As ConCeived by Charles Darwin, evolutionary biology is the science of change. The Darwinian revolution challenged the reCeived idea that species and phenotypes were timeless givens, created by God and incapable of change. The putative distinction between biology and Culture, in other words, should never have been framed as the difference between fixity and changeability. It should have been understood instead as a matter of differenCe at the level of temporal sCales, distinguishing the deep time of genetic change from the short time of reCent human history. The differenCe in temporal sCales, of Course, is not insignifiCant. In the last two decades of the twentieth Century, some sCholars in the fields of soCiobiology and evolutionary psychology developed an approach based on the assumption that the human brain and body undergoes adaptation over time but at a paCe so slow as to be irrelevant for short-term historical analysis.
Recommended publications
  • Neuroscience and Critique
    NEUROSCIENCE AND CRITIQUE Recent years have seen a rapid growth in neuroscientific research, and an expansion beyond basic research to incorporate elements of the arts, humanities and social sciences. Some have suggested that the neurosciences will bring about major transformations in the understand- ing of our selves, our culture and our society. Ongoing debates within psychology, philoso- phy and literature about the implications of these developments within the neurosciences, and the emerging fields of educational neuroscience, neuroeconomics and neuro-aesthetics also bear witness to a “neurological turn” which is currently taking place. Neuroscience and Critique is a groundbreaking edited collection that reflects on the impact of neuroscience in contemporary social science and the humanities. It is the first book to consider possibilities for a critique of the theories, practices and implications of contemporary neuroscience. Bringing together leading scholars from several disciplines, the contributors draw upon a range of perspectives, including cognitive neuroscience, critical philosophy, psychoanalysis and feminism, and also critically examine several key ideas in contemporary neuroscience, including: • The idea of “neural personhood” • Theories of emotion in affective neuroscience • Empathy, intersubjectivity and the notion of “embodied simulation” • The concept of an “emo-rational” actor within neuroeconomics The volume will stimulate further debate in the emerging field of interdisciplinary studies in neuroscience and will appeal to researchers and advanced students in a number of disciplines, including psychology, philosophy and critical studies. Jan De Vos is a post-doctoral FWO Research Fellow at the Centre for Critical Philosophy at Ghent University, Belgium. His main research area is that of the neurological turn and its implications for ideology critique.
    [Show full text]
  • Program Final (Sept 17, 2012)
    Program Final (Sept 17, 2012) Where not otherwise indicated, the chair of the session is the final speaker. Session 1 - Thurs 1:30pm - 3pm Session 1 (D) Frontier 202A Disease and Subjectivity M.K. Nixon. Keep Bleeding: Hemorrhagic Sores, Trade, and the Necessity of Leaky Boundaries in Defoe’s Journal of the Plague Year This paper considers the way in which nonhuman contagious disease is crucial in shaping human subjectivity. To this end, this essay probes notions of two boundaries that are both political and personal—the border between the national subject and the international other, and the boundary between the self and all that lies outside of it—in a consideration of Daniel Defoe’s 1722 text, A Journal of the Plague Year. Operating from the notion that the personal is indeed political, this essay asserts that these two borders are largely intertwined, particularly when considering infectious diseases. By way of examining attitudes towards national and personal boundaries, this paper focuses in large part on Defoe’s representation of the eponymous buboes of the bubonic plague, juxtaposing and exploring his depiction of suppurated and calcified bubonic sores as metonymic signifiers of both personal and national boundaries that are transgressed or fortified, respectively. Examination of Defoe’s illustration of bubonic sores shows that Defoe depicted the Great Plague of 1665 in ways that I assert were thoroughly influenced by his conceptualization of international trade—a conceptualization which resisted the nationalistic xenophobia typical of his day and instead embraced a type of individualistic mercantilism. Defoe’s views on economics and trade, then, influenced his understanding of the Great Plague and cycled back to result in a view of man and nation that advocates permeable boundaries even in response to the hugely threatening potential of a complete breakdown of self- and nation-constituting borders.
    [Show full text]
  • Critical Neuroscience
    Choudhury_bindex.indd 391 7/22/2011 4:08:46 AM Critical Neuroscience Choudhury_ffirs.indd i 7/22/2011 4:37:11 AM Choudhury_ffirs.indd ii 7/22/2011 4:37:11 AM Critical Neuroscience A Handbook of the Social and Cultural Contexts of Neuroscience Edited by Suparna Choudhury and Jan Slaby A John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., Publication Choudhury_ffirs.indd iii 7/22/2011 4:37:11 AM This edition first published 2012 © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Blackwell Publishing was acquired by John Wiley & Sons in February 2007. Blackwell’s publishing program has been merged with Wiley’s global Scientific, Technical, and Medical business to form Wiley-Blackwell. Registered Office John Wiley & Sons Ltd, The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex, PO19 8SQ, UK Editorial Offices 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148-5020, USA 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford, OX4 2DQ, UK The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex, PO19 8SQ, UK For details of our global editorial offices, for customer services, and for information about how to apply for permission to reuse the copyright material in this book please see our website at www.wiley.com/wiley-blackwell. The right of Suparna Choudhury and Jan Slaby to be identified as the authors of the editorial material in this work has been asserted in accordance with the UK Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, except as permitted by the UK Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988, without the prior permission of the publisher.
    [Show full text]
  • Environment, Culture, and the Brain New Explorations in Neurohistory
    Perspectives Environment, Culture, and the Brain New Explorations in Neurohistory Edited by EDMUND RUSSELL 2012 / 6 RCC Perspectives Environment, Culture, and the Brain New Explorations in Neurohistory Edited by Edmund Russell 2012 / 6 Environment, Culture, and the Brain 3 About the Authors Peter Becker teaches modern history at the University of Vienna. His main research interests are in the history of public administration, criminology, and policing. More recently he has embarked on a research project on the recurrence of biological think- ing in social research and social policy with a focus on the role of neurosciences in public discourse. Benedikt Berninger is an associate professor of physiological chemistry at the Johan- nes Gutenberg University Mainz. His research in neurobiology focuses on forcing fate conversion of somatic cells into neurons by a process called “reprogramming.” The essay “Causality and the Brain” in this volume was inspired by his interest in history and philosophy of history. Kirsten Brukamp is a research fellow in theoretical medicine at RWTH Aachen Uni- versity. She specializes in bioethics and neuroethics and holds degrees in medicine, philosophy, and cognitive science. Carlos Collado Seidel is a professor of modern and contemporary history at the Uni- versity of Marburg. His main research fields are comparative European history and the contemporary history of Spain. Together with Karin Meissner, he is currently working on a research project on neurological processes during decision-making in politics and diplomacy. Steve Fuller is Auguste Comte Professor of Social Epistemology in the Department of Sociology at the University of Warwick, UK. Originally trained in history and philosophy of science, he is closely associated with the field of “social epistemology,” which is also the name of a quarterly journal he founded in 1987.
    [Show full text]
  • Violence on the Brain: a Critique of Neuroscience in Criminal Law
    VIOLENCE ON THE BRAIN: A CRITIQUE OF NEUROSCIENCE IN CRIMINAL LAW Amanda C. Pustilnik* Is there such a thing as a criminally "violent brain"? Does it make sense to speak of "the neurobiology of violence" or the "psychopathology of crime"? Is it possible to answer on a physiological level what makes one person engage in criminal violence and another not, under similar circumstances? Current research in law and neuroscience is promising to answer these questions with a "yes." Some legal scholars working in this area claim that we are close to realizing the "early criminologists' dream of identifying the biological roots of criminality." These hopes for a neuroscientific transformation of the criminal law, although based in the newest research, are part of a very old story. Criminal law and neuroscience have been engaged in an ill-fated and sometimes tragic affair for over two hundred years. Three issues have recurred that track those that bedeviled earlierefforts to ground criminal law in brain sciences. First is the claim that the brain is often the most relevant or fundamental level at which to understand criminal conduct. Second is that the various phenomena we call "criminal violence" arise causally from dysfunction within specific locations in the brain ("localization"). Third is the related claim that, because much violent criminality arises from brain dysfunction, people who commit such acts are biologically different from typical people ("alterity"or "otherizing"). This Article first demonstrates parallels between certain current claims about the neurobiology of criminal violence and past movements that were concerned with the law and * Climenko Fellow & Lecturer on Law, Harvard Law School.
    [Show full text]
  • The 'Biological Turn' in History Writing
    Exchanges : the Warwick Research Journal The ‘Biological Turn’ in History Writingi Josh Patel* Department of History, University of Warwick *Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract In recent history writing, there has been an acceleration of interdisciplinary projects drawing from life sciences, a movement which has been identified as a ‘biological turn’, taking perspectives from diverse fields such as biology, evolutionary psychology, and neurobiology to provide insights into traditional written sources. While this provides numerous new understandings, current use of life sciences is often uncritical. I argue that the biological turn in history writing uses life sciences not to create challenging insights, but to make naturalised claims of human behaviour, and carries with it the current epistemological and socio-political preferences for economically and politically ‘useful’ scientific knowledge. Yet the claims of the biological turn are proposed as divorced from any political context. This is at best naïve, and delegitimises alternative sources of knowledge production. Such an approach has serious implications for writing history, undermines the programme of the history of science, and should be challenged in order to assist in the creation of more helpful and introspective Peer review: This article knowledge when engaging with interdisciplinary material. In this review has been subject to a article I argue that the biological turn is an unsatisfactory response to the double blind peer review linguistic turn, and discuss
    [Show full text]
  • September-2015
    Clio’s Psyche Understanding the “Why” of Culture, Current Events, History, and Society Psychohistory at the Crossroads Symposium The Past, Present, and Future of Psychohistory The Baltimore Riots Freud’s Analysis of His Daughter The Ordeal of Alfred Dreyfus Memorials: Peter Gay and Eli Sagan Psychohistory Meeting Reports Volume 22 Numbers 1-2 June-September 2015 Clio’s Psyche Vol. 22 No. 1-2 June-Sept 2015 ISSN 1080-2622 Published Quarterly by the Psychohistory Forum 627 Dakota Trail, Franklin Lakes, NJ 07417 Telephone: (201) 891-7486 E-mail: [email protected] Editor: Paul H. Elovitz Book Review Editor: Ken Fuchsman Editorial Board Herb Barry, PhD University of Pittsburgh • C. Fred Alford, PhD University of Maryland • James W. Anderson, PhD Northwestern University • David Beisel, PhD RCC-SUNY • Donald L. Carveth, PhD, York University • Ken Fuchsman, EdD University of Connecticut • Bob Lentz • Peter Loewenberg, PhD UCLA • Peter Petschauer, PhD Appalachian State University Subscription Rate: Free to members of the Psychohistory Forum $72 two-year subscription to non-members $65 yearly to institutions (Add $60 per year outside U.S.A. & Canada) Single issue price: $21 We welcome articles of psychohistorical interest of 500 - 1,500 words and a few longer ones. Copyright © 2015 The Psychohistory Forum Psychohistory at the Crossroads Symposium The Past, Present, and Future of Psychohistory The Baltimore Riots Freud’s Analysis of His Daughter The Ordeal of Alfred Dreyfus Memorials: Peter Gay and Eli Sagan Psychohistory Meeting Reports Volume 22 Numbers 1-2 June-September 2015 Clio’s Psyche Understanding the “Why” of Culture, Current Events, History, and Society Volume 22 Number 1-2 June-Sept 2015 _____________________________________________________________________ Psychohistory at the Crossroads Symposium Psychohistory Is an Approach Rather than a Discipline.
    [Show full text]
  • How to Cite: Brukamp, Kirsten. “Neurohistory: Being in Time.” In: “Environment, Culture, and the Brain: New Explorations
    How to cite: Brukamp, Kirsten. “Neurohistory: Being in Time.” In: “Environment, Culture, and the Brain: New Explorations in Neurohistory,” edited by Edmund Russell, RCC Perspectives 2012, no. 6, 75–78. All issues of RCC Perspectives are available online. To view past issues, and to learn more about the Rachel Carson Center for Environment and Society, please visit www.rachelcarsoncenter.de. Rachel Carson Center for Environment and Society Leopoldstrasse 11a, 80802 Munich, GERMANY ISSN 2190-8087 © Copyright is held by the contributing authors. Environment, Culture, and the Brain 75 Kirsten Brukamp Neurohistory: Being in Time What is neurohistory?—The ambitious endeavors suggested by the novel term pos- sess the potential to result in unexpected perspectives on both history and neurosci- ence. Neurohistory projects fall into one of three categories. Focus on history: Neurohistory may first be history informed by neuroscience. Ac- cordingly, neuroscience is here understood as a support for achieving historical un- derstanding: historical events and processes are assessed in light of insights from neuroscience. Examples for projects in this field comprise all those that would benefit from a deeper understanding of individual actions, collective intentions, and social behaviors—that is, goals that may be reached with psychology as well as social and cultural neuroscience (Adolphs 2009; Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia 2010; Martinez Mateo et al. 2011). In particu- lar, research can unearth new perspectives on gender differences, personal factors in history, conflicts, conflict resolution, hierarchy, and power structures. In select cases, the behavior of influential individuals may be explained by disorders of the nervous system (Toole 1995). For prehistory, comparative biology and neuroscience could identify distinct stages of behavior and culture in the evolution of human ancestors (Stout et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Munich Center for Neurosciences
    MUNICH CENTER FOR NEUROSCIENCES Munich Center for Neurosciences – Brain & Mind LMU Biocenter Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Munich (LMU) Grosshaderner Straße 2 www.mcn.lmu.de EUROSCIENCES 82152 Planegg-Martinsried +49 (0)89 / 2180-74303 N [email protected] ENTER FOR C MUNICH 4 | 5 The Free State of Bavaria is a first-rate location for science and research, All these results are both promising and encouraging. With innovative where life sciences play an eminent role. They find one of their strongest projects like the “Munich Center for Neurosciences“, we can successfully bases in Munich. The Bavarian capital is home to two great research advance the further development of our research landscape and maintain our universities – both winners of the Germany- wide Initiative for Excellence – as leading position in the life sciences. well as numerous highly-specialized and well-known non-university research institutions. It is no wonder that Munich is the ideal place for committed I am sure that the great work of the scientists at the “Munich Center for scientists to deliver prime scientific results at the highest international level. Neurosciences“ will continue its success and wish the centre all the best for the future. Today, top science demands close co-operation – beyond the limits of institutes and disciplines. The “Munich Center for Neurosciences“ provides the integrative platform to take the strong interdisciplinary approach the Munich, August 2011 complexity of the different fields of neurosciences asks for. Its being open to integrating perspectives and findings from neighbouring fields of research Dr. Wolfgang Heubisch even allows a holistic reflection of burning neuroscientific questions.
    [Show full text]
  • Enabling and Enhancing Neuroscience Advances for Society
    THE BRAIN INITIATIVE AND NEUROETHICS: ENABLING AND ENHANCING NEUROSCIENCE ADVANCES FOR SOCIETY TABLE OF CONTENTS EXECUTIVE SUMMARY CHAPTER 1. NEUROETHICS PAST, PRESENT, AND FUTURE • Neuroethics Questions for Neuroscientists (NeQN) • Neuroethics Guiding Principles • Types of Neuroethics Research CHAPTER 2. STUDYING OURSELVES: THE UNIQUENESS OF NEUROSCIENCE • The moral significance of the brain • Assumptions about the brain and about neuroscience CHAPTER 3. NEUROETHICS IMPLICATIONS OF NEUROTECHNOLOGIES • Priority Area 1. Discovering Diversity • Priority Area 2. Maps at Multiple Scales • Priority Area 3. Brain in Action • Priority Area 4. Demonstrating Causality • Priority Area 5. Identifying Fundamental Principles • Priority Area 6. Human Neuroscience CHAPTER 4. NEUROETHICS AND RESEARCH WITH ANIMAL MODELS • Ethics and the use of research animal models in BRAIN Initiative research CHAPTER 5. BEYOND THE BENCH: REAL-WORLD TRANSLATION OF NEUROSCIENCE RESEARCH • Brain privacy • Advances in neural data collection • Brain enhancement • Dual use of BRAIN Initiative neuroscience • Neuroscience and law • Neuroscience and education • Neuromarketing CHAPTER 6. INTEGRATING NEUROETHICS AND NEUROSCIENCE • Guidance for researchers: The critical role of neuroethics in neuroscience research • Neuroethics scholarship and training • Professional/Institutional support • Global stage for neuroethics • Public engagement: Meaningful and bidirectional 1 NEUROETHICS TRANSFORMATIVE PROJECT: Revolutionizing BRAIN: Understanding the Bases of Consciousness: Intersection
    [Show full text]
  • The Historical Emergence and Conceptual Context of Neuro-Art
    Art and Neuroscience: The Historical Emergence and Conceptual Context of Neuro-Art by Carin Laura D’Souza A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of: Doctor of Philosophy in Art History Approved Dissertation Committee: Prof. Dr. Paul Crowther, chair Prof. Dr. Claus Hilgetag Dr. Timothy Senior Prof. Dr. Robert Zwijnenberg Date of Defense: November 13, 2012 School of Humanities and Social Science, Jacobs University, Bremen i ii Abstract This study has developed on the premise that neuroscience has a significant impact on contemporary art, and on the observation that, from the dialogue with neuroscience, a new artistic tendency has been emerging. The aim of this research was therefore to investigate the roots, the emergence, and development of what I generically call neuro art. The main objective of the research was to initiate the history of neuro art. This history of neuro art investigates, for the first time, the relationship between neuroscience and contemporary practice of the visual arts by identifying and examining those artworks that rely on knowledge of the brain and the nervous system. The study begins with a broad analysis of the role neuroscience plays in contemporary culture. The analysis situates neuro art within the larger context of cultural interactions with neuroscience, defines neuro art, and frames the history of neuro art in relation with two other disciplines: neuroaesthetics and neuroarthistory. The core of the research addresses in detail the history of art objects about the brain and the nervous system. Setting the scene, the thesis first describes the earlier manifestations of neuro art and identifies its conceptual and historical roots.
    [Show full text]
  • Thesis (Complete)
    UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) The politics of plasticity: Sex and gender in the 21st century brain Kleinherenbrink, A.V. Publication date 2016 Document Version Final published version Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Kleinherenbrink, A. V. (2016). The politics of plasticity: Sex and gender in the 21st century brain. General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) Download date:24 Sep 2021 T HE t the beginning of this century, the idea that the brain is plastic—that its P Astructure and function can change in response to stimulation or injury— OLITICS began to replace the belief that adult brains are fixed. The claim that sex differences are hardwired into the brain, however, is still frequently endorsed.
    [Show full text]