Introduction

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Introduction See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/285591389 PRE-ORALIZED NASAL CODAS IN MAMAINDÊ And the Oral Vowel Enhancement Proposal Conference Paper · January 2011 CITATIONS READS 2 48 1 author: David Eberhard SIL International 21 PUBLICATIONS 387 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Community Based Language Development View project Language ecology View project All content following this page was uploaded by David Eberhard on 04 December 2015. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. PRE-ORALIZED NASAL CODAS IN MAMAINDÊ And the Oral Vowel Enhancement Proposal david m eberhard sil INTRODUCTION The various Nambikwara nations gained short lived notoriety in the 1940s thru the writings of the French anthropologist Claude Levi Strauss, who dubbed them the ‘earth people’, for their characteristic aversion to sleeping anywhere other than on the ground. These ‘earth peoples’ constitute a loose-knit cluster of bands, native to an area covering some 50,000 square kilometers in northwest Brazil, not far from the Bolivia border. When considered altogether, they number some 1200 individuals. However, each Nambikwara band considers itself quite independent from the others, with its own individual identity and language. Due to distinct variables of social contact, these small languages find themselves at various stages along the road to endangerment and extinction. In the midst of this rather fragile family is Mamaindê, spoken by some 250 individuals. It can be classified as a member of the Guapore cluster of the Northern Nambikwara branch of the Nambikwara language family. The Mamaindê live in 4 communities, which are having increased contact with the outside world, resulting in a high rate of bilingualism with Portuguese. A few of the younger children are only passive speakers of the language, signaling that the highly endangered stage has already begun. As far as its phonology is concerned, Mamaindê has proved to be quite fascinating. Some of the major points of interest include 4 contrastive vowel qualities (oral, nasal, creaky voice, and nasal & creaky voice - yielding 16 vowel phonemes); it also has an intricate place feature spreading process from vowels to consonants (requiring a new look at feature geometry); a tone system with 2 lexical tones, tone plateauing and floating tones; a stress system that is predicated on syllable weight, syllable position, and 4 lexical strata; and a coda position which routinely employs biphasic oral/nasal segments. OBJECTIVES OF THIS STUDY It is this last point which will be the focus of our study today. The behavior of the nasal coda is one of the more salient features of the Mamaindê sound system. This behavior results in the formation of three biphasic oral/nasal segments in syllable final positions: labial [bm], coronal [dn], and dorsal [gŋ]. It will be shown that in the Mamaindê language these complex codas require a phonology which treats their primary articulation as nasal [dn, bm, and gŋ] rather than oral [dn, bm, gŋ], and thus the process involved is one of pre-oralization as opposed to post-nasalization. In this presentation, my objective will be to explore three alternatives for the analysis of this pre-oralization process, first from a phonological (autosegmental) perspective, and then two analyses from a phonetic implementation approach. This paper will show that one of the phonetic alternatives is the more satisfactory of the three. It will also demonstrate that adopting a specific set of parameters proposed by Wetzels (2008) is helpful in predicting the type of phonetic enhancement found in this language. DISTRIBUTION OF ORAL/NASAL BIPHASIC SEGMENTS Biphasic oral/nasal segments (as well as their counterpart nasal/oral segments) have been documented in a large number of languages and language families around the world, many of these endangered. They seem to be concentrated in four general areas; South America, Africa, Australia, and Austronesia (Ladefoged, 1993:165-166; Wetzels, 2009:1; see also Rosendall, 1992, and Mills, 1984 for data on the African languages Gwari and Senoufo respectively). However, other parts of the world are not without them. Chen and Clumeck give examples of post-stopped nasals in Cantonese (1975:128). In Borneo, the Bonggi language (Boutin 1993, 2000), as well as the Malayic and Land Dayak language families (Blust 1997; Court 1972; Scott; 1964) are reported to have this feature, which is also commonly referred to in the literature as pre- or post-nasalization, or, when looked at another way, pre-oralization, denasalization, or pre-plosion. Oral-nasal contours are also quite prevalent among Amazonian languages. Wetzels (2009), whose recent study focuses on nasal/oral contour consonants in South America, gives examples from eight Amazonian languages with this feature: Yuhup, Dâw, Wari, Wansajot, Kaingang, Maxacali, Barasana, and Mebengokre. In Amazonia, the language families which have been most documented in terms of oral/nasal contour segments appear to be the Macro-Je family (specifically Apinaye, Kaingang, Maxacali, and Xokleng) and the Maku family (Yuhup and Dâw). To this list the Nambikwara family must also be added (specifically Southern Nambikwara and all the Northern Nambikwara lects, namely Mamaindê, Negarotê, Latundê and Lakondê), as it becomes evident that oral/nasal contours occupy a prominent place in most of the phonologies of Nambikwara languages outside of Sabanê. In the Nambikwara languages, biphasic oral/nasal segments are the result of oralization, being output variants of simple nasals. These languages also limit the oralization of nasal stops to the coda position, and thus only the pre-oralized nasals are present. Post-oralized nasals, such as those found in Kaingang and Maxacali, are not a Nambikwara phenomenon, these being more characteristic of Macro-Je languages (See Wetzels 2009). Of the three pre-oralized nasal forms found in the family - [bm], [dn] and [gŋ] - Southern Nambikwara (Kroeker, 2001:79), Mamaindê (Eberhard, 2003a), and Negarotê1 possess all three, while Latundê and Lakondê (Telles, 2002:56-57) have only the labial [bm] and coronal [dn] varieties, apparently lacking the dorsal counterpart.2 Antunes (2004) does not mention any pre-oralized nasal variants for Sabanê. Mamaindê pre-oralized nasals, however, can be distinguished from all the other Nambikwara biphasic segments by the unique manner in which they acquire their place features (we will touch on this later - see Eberhard, 2009). 1 The Negarotê data comes from my own Mamaindê database. These two Lects are very sImILar and there are a number of Negarotê lIvIng wIthIn the Mamaindê communIty who continue to use some Negarotê forms. 2 TeLLes (2002:56‐57) notes that the pre‐stopped nasals, whILe common in Lakondê, are quIte rare in Latundê. THE MAMAINDÊ BIPHASIC ORAL/NASAL SEGMENTS We will start our discussion of the MD biphasic oral/nasal segments by defining their phonological status. Here is a sampling of the abundant examples found in the language, organized according to the variant involved: [bm] /kadeunta-latʰa-wa/ [gadeubmdalatʰwa] it is alive /waun-latʰa-wa/ [waubmlatʰwa] it is red /taun-tu/ [daubmdu] tail /leunʔ-tu/ [leubmʔdu] tapir [gŋ] /sin-tu/ [sigŋdu] meat /jalinʔja̰-tu/ [jaligŋʔja̰ɾu] puberty flute music /walinʔ-tu/ [waligŋʔdu] anteater: type ‘mirim’ /wainsi-tu/ [waigŋtʃiɾu] medicine [dn] /jalakwatun-tu/ [jalakwadudndu] howler monkey /ʔon-latʰa-wa/ [ʔodnlatʰwa] he is lazy /nahon-sa̰-tu/ [nahodnsa̰ɾu] sweet beverage /nakajanʔ-tu/ [nagajadnʔdu] person/indian While these sequences appear at first glance to be the result of intrusive stops, I intend to show that there is no epenthesis occurring here at all – it is simply a case of gestural timing. In articulatory terms, the opening of the velum is simply delayed until after the tongue has assumed its place of articulation for the nasal. One of the basic questions being asked in this study is whether this timing delay is phonological or phonetic in character. First I give distributional evidence that these biphasic coda segments in Mamaindê are in fact simple nasals underlyingly. The forms listed here show biphasic segments alternating with simple nasal stops. The conditioning environment has to do with syllable position and the quality of the preceding vowel. When the simple nasal consonant is in the coda position and preceded by a tautosyllabic oral vowel, it is systematically realized as a biphasic nasal. (01) /alain-sihtaʔ/ [a.laigŋ.sih.r̥aʔ] cross.over-IN.ORDER.TO.DS in order for him/her to cross over (02) /naʔtun-hĩʔ/ [naʔ.dudn.hĩʔ] to.be.full-THEN.DS he/she was full, then… (03) /kateun-kʰatoʔ/ [ka.deubm.kʰə.ɾəʔ] be.alive-THEN.SS he/she was still alive, then… In the intervocalic position, however, or after nasal vowels, only the simple nasal appears. Note that the addition of the first person marker /a/ in the first three forms forces the resyllabification of the nasal from coda to onset, and the biphasic variant is lost. (04) /alain-a-sihtaʔ/ [a.lai.na.sih.ɾ̥aʔ] cross.over-S1‐IN.ORDER.TO.DS in order for me to cross over (05) /naʔtun-a-hĩʔ/ [naʔ.duː.na.hĩʔ] to.be.full-S1‐THEN.DS I was full, then… (06) /kateun-a-hĩʔ/ [ka.deu.na.hĩʔ] be.alive-S1‐THEN.DS I was (still) alive, then… (07) /wanũn-sa/ [wanũnsa] good-speech good speech (08) /na-ʔmĩn/ [naʔmĩn] 3Poss-skin his skin (09) /hãn-latha-wa/ [hãnlathwa] white-S3-Decl it is white Oral stops, on the other hand, are never found alternating with these contour segments. This distribution pattern, then, provides sufficient evidence for establishing the simple nasal consonant as the lexical form for each of these biphasic nasals. Additional support comes from the phonetic data. In most tokens during natural speech, the nasal phase of biphasic stops is from 10% to 100% longer than the oral phase (range of oral phase: from .042ms - .090ms / range of nasal phase: from .081ms - .110ms).
Recommended publications
  • An Introduction to Three Nambikwara Ethnohistories
    An Introduction To Three Nambikwara Ethnohistories January, 2019: Edwin B. Reesink – Allegories of Wildness – Three Nambikwara ethnohistories of sociocultural and linguistic change and continuity –Rozenberg Publishers – ISBN 978 90 3610 173 8 – 2010 – will be online soon. International fame for the Nambikwara came from the central role they play in the work of the late Claude Lévi-Strauss. In his most popular book, Tristes Tropiques, over fifty years ago, he already had painted a particular picture of the Nambikwara way of life. Until the end of his long life this great scholar reminisced about his formative years in anthropology and the influence of the several Indian peoples he encountered during his travels in the interior of Brazil. In effect, the Nambikwara, as seen in the quote, occupy a special place, one not just of scientific interest but particularly one of a sympathy and feeling sometimes not readily associated with this anthropologist. At the time of the travels by Lévi- Strauss and the other people of his expedition, the dry season of 1938, the Nambikwara happened to be in an intermediate phase of their history: enjoying their political autonomy yet already shaped and devastated by the encroachment of Brazilian society. The sparse, intermediate but regular contacts with Brazilians made them susceptible to being devastated yet further by epidemics. Previously, from 1907 onward, the Brazilians by means of the so-called Rondon Mission penetrated into the vast territories in which lived a large number of different local groups who each considered themselves to be different from their neighbours. Rondon, the State and later researchers recognized this diversity but ultimately still tended to maintain the idea of “one Nambikwara people”.
    [Show full text]
  • Current Studies on South American Languages, [Indigenous Languages of Latin America (ILLA), Vol
    This file is freely available for download at http://www.etnolinguistica.org/illa This book is freely available for download at http://www.etnolinguistica.org/illa References: Crevels, Mily, Simon van de Kerke, Sérgio Meira & Hein van der Voort (eds.). 2002. Current Studies on South American Languages, [Indigenous Languages of Latin America (ILLA), vol. 3], [CNWS publications, vol. 114], Leiden: Research School of Asian, African, and Amerindian Studies (CNWS), vi + 344 pp. (ISBN 90-5789-076-3) CURRENT STUDIES ON SOUTH AMERICAN LANGUAGES INDIGENOUS LANGUAGES OF LATIN AMERICA (ILLA) This series, entitled Indigenous Languages of Latin America, is a result of the collaboration between the CNWS research group of Amerindian Studies and the Spinoza research program Lexicon and Syntax, and it will function as an outlet for publications related to the research program. LENGUAS INDÍGENAS DE AMÉRICA LATINA (ILLA) La serie Lenguas Indígenas de América Latina es el resultado de la colabora- ción entre el equipo de investigación CNWS de estudios americanos y el programa de investigación Spinoza denominado Léxico y Sintaxis. Dicha serie tiene como objetivo publicar los trabajos que se lleven a cabo dentro de ambos programas de investigación. Board of advisors / Consejo asesor: Willem Adelaar (Universiteit Leiden) Eithne Carlin (Universiteit Leiden) Pieter Muysken (Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen) Leo Wetzels (Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam) Series editors / Editores de la serie: Mily Crevels (Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen) Simon van de Kerke (Universiteit
    [Show full text]
  • David Eberhard Ph.D
    David Eberhard Ph.D. Linguistics, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2009 David Eberhard is passionate about minority languages and those who speak them. Before coming to Thailand in 2013, he spent 18 years doing linguistic research and language development for the Mamaindê, a small endangered Nambikwara language of west-central Brazil. His descriptive work heavily focuses on the phonology and morphology of this highly poly-synthetic language family. Besides field linguistics in Amazonian languages (and the concomitant task of learning to speak those languages), his research has more recently concentrated on new approaches to language vitality assessment, language conservation and language development of endangered languages in general. This emphasis not only values language diversity, but it stresses the need for community ownership, and the importance of realistic, sustainable language use goals that take into account the current vitality of all languages in a community’s repertoire. Dr. Eberhard has developed and offered community based language development workshops to promote language awareness among mother tongue speakers of endangered language communities in numerous countries / regions, including the following: Brazil (Amazonia), Malaysia (Sabah, Sarawak, West Malaysia); Papua New Guinea (Sepik, Central Highlands); Indonesia (Central Java), and São Tomé e Príncipe (West Africa). Responsible language description and language development must both begin with the desires of the community of speakers at the center. When this is ensured, both researcher and community gain a greater appreciation and wonder for all languages, in particular the smallest ones. As Eberhard reminds his students, language is the ‘sound of relationship’. Current projects: • Minority Language Communities, Language Shift, and Identity: Exploring factors which condition identity when a traditional language is lost.
    [Show full text]
  • Descriptive and Comparative Research on South American Indian Languages
    Historical overview: Descriptive and comparative research on South American Indian languages Willem F. H. Adelaar 1. Introduction The extreme language diversity that was characteristic for South America must have been a challenge to native groups throughout the subcontinent, struggling to maintain commercial and political relations with each other. Due to the absence of phonetically based writing systems in pre-European times there is hardly any documentation about the way cross-linguistic communication was achieved. How- ever, the outlines of a conscious linguistic policy can be assumed from the Incas’ success in imposing their language upon a millenary multilingual society. Second- language learning, often by users of typologically widely different languages, must have been an everyday concern to the subjects of the Inca empire. Sixteenth-cen- tury chroniclers often report in a matter-of-fact way on the ease and rapidity with which native Americans mastered the language of their conquerors, be it Quechua, Spanish or any other language. Apart from such cases of political necessity, there are indications that language played an essential role in many South American native societies and that it could be manipulated and modified in a deliberate way. The use of stylistic speech levels among the Cuna (Sherzer 1983) and of ceremo- nial discourse among the Mbyá (Cadogan 1959; Clastres 1974), the Shuar (Gnerre 1986) and the Trio (Carlin 2004), the appreciation of rhetorical skill as a requisite for leadership among the Mapuche, the distinction of female and masculine speech among the Karajá (Rodrigues 2004) and the Chiquitano (Galeote 1993), the associ- ation of language choice and family lineage among the peoples of the Vaupés region (Sorensen 1967; Aikhenvald 2002), and the association of language choice and professional occupation in highland Bolivia (Howard 1995) appear to indicate an awareness of linguistic functionality not limited to daily communication alone.
    [Show full text]
  • Environmental Shielding Is Contrast Preservation
    Phonology 35 (2018). Supplementary materials Environmental shielding is contrast preservation Juliet Stanton New York University Supplementary materials These supplementary materials contain four appendices and a bibli- ography: Appendix A: List of shielding languages 1 Appendix B: Additional information on shielding languages 4 Appendix C: List of non-shielding languages 37 Appendix D: Summary of vowel neutralisation survey 45 References 49 The materials are supplied in the form provided by the author. Appendices for “Environmental shielding is contrast preservation” Appendix A: list of shielding languages Key for appendices A-C Shaded = shielding occurs in this context Not shaded = shielding not known to occur in this context The language names provided in appendices A-C are those used by SAPhon. Evidence = type of evidence found for a vocalic nasality contrast, in addition to the author’s description. (MP = minimal or near-minimal pairs; NVNE: nasal vowels in non-nasal environments; –: no additional evidence available) Shielding contexts V-V?˜ (Evidence) Language Family Source Appendix B NV VN]σ V]σN Yes MP Ache´ Tup´ı Roessler (2008) #1, p. 4 Yes MP Aguaruna Jivaroan Overall (2007) #2, p. 4 Yes MP Amahuaca Panoan Osborn (1948) #3, p. 5 Yes MP Amarakaeri Harakmbet Tripp (1955) #4, p. 5 Yes MP Amundava Tup´ı Sampaio (1998) #5, p. 6 Yes MP Andoke (Isolate) Landaburu (2000a) #6, p. 6 Yes MP Apiaka´ Tup´ı Padua (2007) #7, p. 7 Yes MP Apinaye´ Macro-Ge Oliveira (2005) #8, p. 7 Yes – Arara´ do Mato Grosso Isolate da Rocha D’Angelis (2010) #9, p. 8 Yes MP Arikapu´ Macro-Ge Arikapu´ et al.
    [Show full text]
  • PDF Hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen
    PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen The following full text is a publisher's version. For additional information about this publication click this link. http://hdl.handle.net/2066/42006 Please be advised that this information was generated on 2021-09-24 and may be subject to change. Kwaza in a Comparative Perspective Author(s): Hein van der Voort Reviewed work(s): Source: International Journal of American Linguistics, Vol. 71, No. 4 (October 2005), pp. 365- 412 Published by: The University of Chicago Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/501245 . Accessed: 13/07/2012 09:37 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to International Journal of American Linguistics. http://www.jstor.org KWAZA IN A COMPARATIVE PERSPECTIVE1 Hein van der Voort Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi In view of the previous sparsity of data, the existing claims with regard to a genea- logical classification of the Aikanã, Kanoê, and Kwaza languages of Rondônia, on the Brazilian side of the Guaporé River, are premature and unconvincing.
    [Show full text]
  • Verbal Synthesis in the Guaporé-Mamoré Linguistic Area: a Contact Feature? Rik Van Gijn University of Zürich
    Verbal Synthesis in the Guaporé-Mamoré Linguistic Area: a Contact Feature? Rik van Gijn University of Zürich In their discussion of the linguistic area Guaporé-Mamoré (eastern Bolivia and Rondônia) Crevels & Van der Voort (2008) propose ‘polysynthetic morphology’ as one of the structural features shared by most languages in the area. In their approach the feature is regarded as binary (present versus absent) and it is not entirely clear what the basis for their categorization is. In this paper I try to come to a more precise understanding of the nature of the morphological similarities between the Guaporé-Mamoré languages by looking at a range of formal and semantic factors related to the verbal templates of these languages. In this way we can locate the cross-linguistic morphological similarities more precisely, setting the stage for a deeper understanding of the processes of contact-induced diffusion in the area. 1. Introduction Although (morphological) structure is often thought to be relatively impervious to borrowing (e.g. Weinreich 1953, Thomason & Kaufman 1988, Dunn et al. 2005) the geographical skewing of certain morphological parameters is, at least at first sight, suggestive of a sensitivity to contact (see van Gijn, this volume).1 In this paper I examine the question of potential contact-induced diffusion of morphological structure by zooming in on a putative linguistic area in western South America, called the Guaporé-Mamoré (GM), proposed by Crevels & Van der Voort (2008). They argue that the languages of this area, which belong to a number of different families, share a number of structural traits due to a history of contact between the speakers of the languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Mamainde Grammar Volume 2
    VU Research Portal Mamainde Grammar Eberhard, D. 2009 document version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link to publication in VU Research Portal citation for published version (APA) Eberhard, D. (2009). Mamainde Grammar: a Northern Nambikwara language and its cultural context. LOT. General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal ? Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. E-mail address: [email protected] Download date: 01. Oct. 2021 Mamaindê Grammar: A Northern Nambikwara language and its cultural context Volume II Published by LOT phone: +31 30 253 6006 Janskerkhof 13 fax: +31 30 253 6406 3512 BL Utrecht e-mail: [email protected] The Netherlands http://www.lotschool.nl Cover illustration: “Beyond the Threshold” – portrait of a Mamaindê child by Micah Eberhard (graphite on paper). ISBN: 978-94-6093-012-6 NUR 616 Copyright © 2009: David M. Eberhard. All rights reserved. VRIJE UNIVERSITEIT Mamaindê Grammar: a Northern Nambikwara language and its cultural context ACADEMISCH PROEFSCHRIFT ter verkrijging van de graad Doctor aan de Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, op gezag van de rector magnificus prof.dr.
    [Show full text]
  • The Noun Phrase in the Languages of South America
    301220 Olga Krasnoukhova Olga Krasnoukhova Olga Krasnoukhova The Noun Phrase in the The Noun Phrase in the Languages of South America Languages of South America This dissertation presents the first cross-linguistic study of the Noun Phrase in the indigenous languages of South America. It builds upon a considerable amount of data that have recently become available for languages in this continent. Based on a sample of 55 languages, this study gives a novel account of the syntactic, morphosyntactic, and semantic properties of the NP. For example, the analysis shows that personal pronouns commonly receive the same possessive markers as nominal possessors, which implies that a fully grammaticalized category of possessive pronouns is rare in South American languages. In addition, the new South American data only partly confirm typological claims Languages of South America The Noun Phrase in the for tendencies in the NP domain. For instance, a morphologically distinct class of adjectives is found in many languages of the sample; however, this class is often small, and the dominant way to encode property concepts is with verbs. Finally, this study also includes a discussion of the geographic patterning of structural features in the NP, evaluating the assumption that there is a major typological split between so-called Andean and Amazonian languages. The analysis shows that most of the features cannot be attributed to either of these larger areas. It also demonstrates, however, that there is some evidence for a broad structural division of languages into the western part of the continent (corresponding to the Andean sphere) and the rest of the continent.
    [Show full text]
  • Guia De Fontes E Bibliografia Sobre Línguas Indígenas E Produção Associada: Documentos Do CELIN 1
    Guia de Fontes e Bibliografia sobre Línguas Indígenas e Produção Associada: Documentos do CELIN 1 Referências Completas Código da Base Autor/Organizador Título e outras informações SÉRIE LIVROS DIGITAL 1 Guia de Fontes e Bibliografia sobre Línguas Indígenas e Produção Associada Documentos do CELIN Organizadora Marília Facó Soares Colaboradores Lourdes Cristina Araújo Coimbra Thays Lacerda Adilson Moreira Fontenele Museu Nacional/ UFRJ Rio de Janeiro 2013 Editores Universidade Federal Miguel Angel Monné Barrios e Ulisses Caramaschi do Rio de Janeiro Normalização Edson Vargas da Silva e Leandra de Oliveira Reitor Diagramação e Arte-final Carlos Antônio Levi Lia Ribeiro Capa Museu Nacional Edney Souza Editoração Eletrônica Diretora William de Lima Claudia Rodrigues Ferreira de Carvalho Antonio Clovis Britto de Araujo Guilherme de Oliveira Lima Serviços de Secretaria Thiago Macedo dos Santos Editores de Área Adriano Brilhante Kury, Ficha Catalográfica Ciro Alexandre Ávila Claudia Petean Bove G943 Guia de fontes e bibliografia sobre línguas indígenas e produção Débora de Oliveira Pires associada: documentos do CELIN / organizadora, Marília Facó Guilherme Ramos da Silva Muricy Soares; colaboradores, Lo urdes Cristina Araújo Coimbra ... [et ai.]. - Rio de Janeiro: Museu Nacional, 2013. lzabel Cristina Alves Dias 3.581 Kb; PDF João Alves de Oliveira João Wagner de Alencar Castro Com a colaboração de: Thays Lacerda, Adilson Moreira Fontenele. Marcela Laura Monné Freire ISBN 978-85-7427-047-0 Marcelo de Araújo Carvalho 1. Línguas indígenas - Bibliografias. 1. Soares, Marília Facó. 11. Marcos Raposo Coimbra, Lourdes Cristina Araújo. Ili. Lacerda, Thays. IV. Fontenele, Maria Dulce Barcellos Gaspar de Oliveira Adilson Moreira. V. Museu Nacional (Brasil). VI. Série.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Chapter 29 Evidentiality in Nambikwara Languages David M
    1 Chapter 29 Evidentiality in Nambikwara Languages David M. Eberhard 29.1 Introduction The epistemological world of the Nambikwara peoples of west-central Brazil is reflected in a robust system of evidentials. All of the Nambikwara languages documented thus far have them, each with a slightly different set. These various sets of ‘knowledge markers’ orally codify how it is the Nambikwara speakers know what they know, making this knowledge immediately accessible to addressee as well as to the speaker. This chapter will give a broad outline of evidentiality as it is expressed in the various Nambikwara languages. The focus will be on the distinct components of these evidential systems, while pointing out the similarities and differences found in each. In the conclusion I will review the distinctives of this morphological category within the Nambikwara family, and what this family contributes to the global mosaic we call evidentiality. 29.2 Language Family The Nambikwara language family is comprised of two major branches, Northern and Southern, and a single independent speech variety, Sabanê. The Northern Branch in turn has two clusters. The first of these is the Roosevelt River cluster, home to the Lakondê and Latundê languages, and the second is the Guaporé River cluster, where we find Mamaindê and Negarotê. The 2 languages within each of these two clusters are mutually intelligible, but communication across the clusters is difficult and uncommon.1 The Southern Branch, on the other hand, is comprised of a larger set of closely related lects, all mutually intelligible. As the variation between these is slight, we will refer to these jointly as Southern Nambikwara.
    [Show full text]
  • A Comparative Study of Language Ecologies in Asia and the Americas
    Vol. 10 (2016), pp. 601–628 http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/ldc http://hdl.handle.net/10125/24719 Revised Version Received: 23 July 2016 A Tale of Two Worlds: A comparative study of language ecologies in Asia and the Americas Stan Anonby Simon Fraser University David M. Eberhard Payap University Language use patterns of individual speech communities are largely conditioned by the different language ecologies in which they are immersed. We believe this ecological stance helps explain why minority languages of Asia are more likely to be sustainable than those in the Americas. We have identified fourteen traits which characterize ecologies in general, describing how they play out differently in the Americas versus Asia. Each trait is considered to be on a continuum, with opposing values that measure whether conditions are more or less favorable to language maintenance. On one side of the continuum, we discuss the values in the Americas, and explain how these are more favorable to language shift. On the other side of the scale, we talk about the values in Asia, and explain how these are more conducive to language maintenance. To show the application of these traits, the paper also includes two in-depth case studies as prototypical examples from each area, one from the Americas and one from Asia. We conclude with some comments about how these traits can be useful for those engaged in language development work. The backstory This paper was birthed out of our life experiences. In the following paragraphs we will tell our stories. They are our accounts of field work among par- ticular tribes in two very distinct places.
    [Show full text]