1 Chapter 29 Evidentiality in Nambikwara Languages David M

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

1 Chapter 29 Evidentiality in Nambikwara Languages David M 1 Chapter 29 Evidentiality in Nambikwara Languages David M. Eberhard 29.1 Introduction The epistemological world of the Nambikwara peoples of west-central Brazil is reflected in a robust system of evidentials. All of the Nambikwara languages documented thus far have them, each with a slightly different set. These various sets of ‘knowledge markers’ orally codify how it is the Nambikwara speakers know what they know, making this knowledge immediately accessible to addressee as well as to the speaker. This chapter will give a broad outline of evidentiality as it is expressed in the various Nambikwara languages. The focus will be on the distinct components of these evidential systems, while pointing out the similarities and differences found in each. In the conclusion I will review the distinctives of this morphological category within the Nambikwara family, and what this family contributes to the global mosaic we call evidentiality. 29.2 Language Family The Nambikwara language family is comprised of two major branches, Northern and Southern, and a single independent speech variety, Sabanê. The Northern Branch in turn has two clusters. The first of these is the Roosevelt River cluster, home to the Lakondê and Latundê languages, and the second is the Guaporé River cluster, where we find Mamaindê and Negarotê. The 2 languages within each of these two clusters are mutually intelligible, but communication across the clusters is difficult and uncommon.1 The Southern Branch, on the other hand, is comprised of a larger set of closely related lects, all mutually intelligible. As the variation between these is slight, we will refer to these jointly as Southern Nambikwara. The list of Southern Nambikwara languages in Figure 1 is taken from Telles and Wetzels (2011). However, defining the exact number of Southern Nambikwara speech varieties is confusing, as the list varies according to author. While Telles and Wetzels list 12 such varieties in 4 groupings, Price (1972: 111) includes 18 speech varieties, grouped into 3 clusters (Juruena – 9 lects, Galera/Guaporé – 8 lects, and Sararé – 1 lect), Lowe (1999: 269) lists 12 varieties without any subgroupings, and Kroeker (2001:1) gives a list of 11 varieties in 2 clusters, 5 Guaporé valley lects, and 6 Juruena valley lects.2 1 All Nambikwara language communities are small. The total population of the entire Nambikwara family was 2232 in 2013 (Socioambiental- http://pib.socioambiental.org/pt/c/quadro-geral). Of the four languages documented thus far, one exhibits strong oral vitality (Southern Nambikwara)̠ another threatened vitality (Mamaindê), while the other two are either moribund or extinct. There was only one elderly speaker of Lakondê in 2002. This scenario reminds us not only of the urgency of describing and documenting such languages before they are gone, but also of the importance of guiding endangered language communities along the challenging journey of making their own informed decisions about the future of their traditional language repertoires and unique identities in the face of massive language and cultural shift. 2 In figure 1, I have intentionally omitted those variants which are virtually extinct and whose structures we know very little about or nothing at all (Tawandê, Sowaintê, Tawendê, and Yalapmundu). 3 <insert Figure 1 here> Figure 1. The Nambikwara language family tree 29.3 Shared traits of the Nambikwara evidential systems Evidentials have been attested in all four of the Nambikwara speech varieties documented thus far: Southern Nambikwara (Kroeker 2001:62-65; Lowe 1999: 274-276); Lakondê (Telles and Wetzels 2006; Telles 2002: 288-290); Sabanê (Araujo 2004:138-140); and Mamaindê (Eberhard 2012: 468-487; 2009; Kingston 1971a/b).3 Note that both north and south branches of the family exhibit evidentials, as well as the one language which stands alone, Sabanê. The well- documented use of evidentials across such a representative portion of languages instantiates this grammatical category as a salient characteristic of this family.4 So far, the Nambikwara family 3 As data on evidentiality from Latundê is not yet available, it is not included here. We are also missing data from a number of other languages in the family. A few evidentials have been found in some of the Negarotê texts collected by the author, but this system (apparently similar to Mamaindê) has not been documented sufficiently to include at this time. Likewise, there is data missing from many dialects of Southern Nambikwara. Lowe’s description of Southern Nambikwara (1999:270) focuses exclusively on the Kithãulhu lect, and Kroeker’s (2001:2) study is a compilation of data from six Southern Nambikwara dialects: Kithãulhu, Halotesu, Saxwentesu, Wakiletesu, Wasusu, and Katitaulhu. Even so, these omit any mention of a large number of other varieties, leaving us with questions as to the differences between the various Southern lects in terms of much of the grammar, including any possible differences in their use of evidentials. 4 Lakondê and Latundê are distinct but closely related languages in the northern branch of this family. Telles (2002) includes many elements of Latundê in her in-depth description of Lakondê grammar (as her title ‘Fonologia e Gramática Latundê/Lakondê’ suggests). However, her 4 includes only large evidential systems (6 evidentials or more: Lakondê, Southern Nambikwara, Mamaindê) or mid-range systems (4 evidentials: Sabanê). The remainder of this chapter will be a comparison of the evidentials found in these four languages: Southern Nambikwara, Lakondê, Sabanê, and Mamaindê. Table 1 lists the evidentials of each, as well as the secondary extensions and other distinctive properties.5 Characteristics in the table will be touched upon at various points in the discussion that follows. Table 1. Evidential Systems of four Nambikwara Languages <insert Table 1 here > 29.4 Southern Nambikwara Marking a fairly common set of four sources of information (visual, inferred, reported, and general knowledge)6, Southern Nambikwara evidentials do not at first stand out (Kroeker treatment of evidentiality in that work is restricted to the Lakondê language alone (Telles, p.c.). Thus the study of Latundê evidentiality is still incomplete and will not be considered here. As they are very similar languages in many other respects, the Latundê system could well be the same as the one in Lakondê, but that has not been confirmed. 5 A [] in the table indicates that researchers have identified the item as a property of a given system. The lack of a [] does not necessarily mean that it doesn’t occur in that system, but simply that this property was not included in that language's description. If a language has only a single reported evidential, this is indicated in the reported 2nd hand row, with the implication that it combines both 2nd and 3rd hand. 6 Kroeker (2001:62-65) refers to these four evidentials respectively as ‘observation’, ‘deduction’, ‘narration’ and ‘customary’. I have used a set of standardized terms to make the comparison 5 2001:62-65). However, the remarkable complexity of this system lies in the fact that most sources of information are also inflected for the perspectives of two different ‘knowers’, effectively creating two paradigms with separate evidentials. These will be referred to as the ‘individual perspective’ and the ‘dual perspective’.7 Individual perspective refers to information known only to the speaker, and dual perspective to information known to both speaker and addressee.8 Aikhenvald (2004: 234) points out that systems which grammaticalize this level of differentiation between a single first person observer and a first plus second person observer are quite rare in the world. between languages more straightforward. Kroeker’s ‘customary’ evidential I am recasting as ‘general knowledge’, a term that is similar (but not identical) to ‘assumed’ in the literature. A more detailed account of this evidential within the Nambikwara languages will be discussed at the end of this article. 7 Kroeker (2001) uses the terms ‘individual verification’ and ‘collective verification’ instead, but these simply refer respectively to an individual speaker’s point of view versus a speaker + addressee combined point of view. These terms are also not to be confused with the double and joint perspectives found in Evans (2005: 103). 8 Lowe’s (1999: 274-276) analysis of Southern Nambikwara, somewhat different from Kroeker’s, involves 4 evidentials (visual, inferred, reported, and internal support), as well as two subcategories of inferred (inferred from actions or from circumstances), and an interaction between evidentiality and a sub-system which marks given and new information. He makes no mention of Kroeker’s individual versus collective verification paradigms. Unquestionably the most interesting of Lowe’s claims is his ‘internal support’ evidential, which he defines as a ‘gut feeling’ that something is true. Unfortunately, the single example given is not glossed in sufficient detail, and we can only be left wondering how this interesting evidential might fit in with Kroeker’s more comprehensive analysis. Due to this lack of information, I do not include ‘internal support’ as an attested Southern Nambikwara evidential in Table 2. It is enclosed in parenthesis in Table 1 to indicate its ‘incomplete’ status, suggesting an area for further study. 6 To better understand such evidentials, it is instructive to consider the broader notion of multiple perspective. Evans (2005: 99) introduces multiple-perspective constructions as “constructions that encode potentially distinct values, on a single semantic dimension, that reflect two or more distinct perspectives or points of reference”. Due to this very broad definition it can apply to temporal, spatial, social, or epistemic domains. Epistemic perspective, the viewpoint from which something is known, is most often realized in speech through a single perspective, that of the speaker (Evans 2005: 93). This single (or individual) perspective can shift from speaker to addressee, and from first person to second person, as is the case in certain declaratives and interrogatives.
Recommended publications
  • An Introduction to Three Nambikwara Ethnohistories
    An Introduction To Three Nambikwara Ethnohistories January, 2019: Edwin B. Reesink – Allegories of Wildness – Three Nambikwara ethnohistories of sociocultural and linguistic change and continuity –Rozenberg Publishers – ISBN 978 90 3610 173 8 – 2010 – will be online soon. International fame for the Nambikwara came from the central role they play in the work of the late Claude Lévi-Strauss. In his most popular book, Tristes Tropiques, over fifty years ago, he already had painted a particular picture of the Nambikwara way of life. Until the end of his long life this great scholar reminisced about his formative years in anthropology and the influence of the several Indian peoples he encountered during his travels in the interior of Brazil. In effect, the Nambikwara, as seen in the quote, occupy a special place, one not just of scientific interest but particularly one of a sympathy and feeling sometimes not readily associated with this anthropologist. At the time of the travels by Lévi- Strauss and the other people of his expedition, the dry season of 1938, the Nambikwara happened to be in an intermediate phase of their history: enjoying their political autonomy yet already shaped and devastated by the encroachment of Brazilian society. The sparse, intermediate but regular contacts with Brazilians made them susceptible to being devastated yet further by epidemics. Previously, from 1907 onward, the Brazilians by means of the so-called Rondon Mission penetrated into the vast territories in which lived a large number of different local groups who each considered themselves to be different from their neighbours. Rondon, the State and later researchers recognized this diversity but ultimately still tended to maintain the idea of “one Nambikwara people”.
    [Show full text]
  • Current Studies on South American Languages, [Indigenous Languages of Latin America (ILLA), Vol
    This file is freely available for download at http://www.etnolinguistica.org/illa This book is freely available for download at http://www.etnolinguistica.org/illa References: Crevels, Mily, Simon van de Kerke, Sérgio Meira & Hein van der Voort (eds.). 2002. Current Studies on South American Languages, [Indigenous Languages of Latin America (ILLA), vol. 3], [CNWS publications, vol. 114], Leiden: Research School of Asian, African, and Amerindian Studies (CNWS), vi + 344 pp. (ISBN 90-5789-076-3) CURRENT STUDIES ON SOUTH AMERICAN LANGUAGES INDIGENOUS LANGUAGES OF LATIN AMERICA (ILLA) This series, entitled Indigenous Languages of Latin America, is a result of the collaboration between the CNWS research group of Amerindian Studies and the Spinoza research program Lexicon and Syntax, and it will function as an outlet for publications related to the research program. LENGUAS INDÍGENAS DE AMÉRICA LATINA (ILLA) La serie Lenguas Indígenas de América Latina es el resultado de la colabora- ción entre el equipo de investigación CNWS de estudios americanos y el programa de investigación Spinoza denominado Léxico y Sintaxis. Dicha serie tiene como objetivo publicar los trabajos que se lleven a cabo dentro de ambos programas de investigación. Board of advisors / Consejo asesor: Willem Adelaar (Universiteit Leiden) Eithne Carlin (Universiteit Leiden) Pieter Muysken (Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen) Leo Wetzels (Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam) Series editors / Editores de la serie: Mily Crevels (Katholieke Universiteit Nijmegen) Simon van de Kerke (Universiteit
    [Show full text]
  • David Eberhard Ph.D
    David Eberhard Ph.D. Linguistics, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2009 David Eberhard is passionate about minority languages and those who speak them. Before coming to Thailand in 2013, he spent 18 years doing linguistic research and language development for the Mamaindê, a small endangered Nambikwara language of west-central Brazil. His descriptive work heavily focuses on the phonology and morphology of this highly poly-synthetic language family. Besides field linguistics in Amazonian languages (and the concomitant task of learning to speak those languages), his research has more recently concentrated on new approaches to language vitality assessment, language conservation and language development of endangered languages in general. This emphasis not only values language diversity, but it stresses the need for community ownership, and the importance of realistic, sustainable language use goals that take into account the current vitality of all languages in a community’s repertoire. Dr. Eberhard has developed and offered community based language development workshops to promote language awareness among mother tongue speakers of endangered language communities in numerous countries / regions, including the following: Brazil (Amazonia), Malaysia (Sabah, Sarawak, West Malaysia); Papua New Guinea (Sepik, Central Highlands); Indonesia (Central Java), and São Tomé e Príncipe (West Africa). Responsible language description and language development must both begin with the desires of the community of speakers at the center. When this is ensured, both researcher and community gain a greater appreciation and wonder for all languages, in particular the smallest ones. As Eberhard reminds his students, language is the ‘sound of relationship’. Current projects: • Minority Language Communities, Language Shift, and Identity: Exploring factors which condition identity when a traditional language is lost.
    [Show full text]
  • Descriptive and Comparative Research on South American Indian Languages
    Historical overview: Descriptive and comparative research on South American Indian languages Willem F. H. Adelaar 1. Introduction The extreme language diversity that was characteristic for South America must have been a challenge to native groups throughout the subcontinent, struggling to maintain commercial and political relations with each other. Due to the absence of phonetically based writing systems in pre-European times there is hardly any documentation about the way cross-linguistic communication was achieved. How- ever, the outlines of a conscious linguistic policy can be assumed from the Incas’ success in imposing their language upon a millenary multilingual society. Second- language learning, often by users of typologically widely different languages, must have been an everyday concern to the subjects of the Inca empire. Sixteenth-cen- tury chroniclers often report in a matter-of-fact way on the ease and rapidity with which native Americans mastered the language of their conquerors, be it Quechua, Spanish or any other language. Apart from such cases of political necessity, there are indications that language played an essential role in many South American native societies and that it could be manipulated and modified in a deliberate way. The use of stylistic speech levels among the Cuna (Sherzer 1983) and of ceremo- nial discourse among the Mbyá (Cadogan 1959; Clastres 1974), the Shuar (Gnerre 1986) and the Trio (Carlin 2004), the appreciation of rhetorical skill as a requisite for leadership among the Mapuche, the distinction of female and masculine speech among the Karajá (Rodrigues 2004) and the Chiquitano (Galeote 1993), the associ- ation of language choice and family lineage among the peoples of the Vaupés region (Sorensen 1967; Aikhenvald 2002), and the association of language choice and professional occupation in highland Bolivia (Howard 1995) appear to indicate an awareness of linguistic functionality not limited to daily communication alone.
    [Show full text]
  • Environmental Shielding Is Contrast Preservation
    Phonology 35 (2018). Supplementary materials Environmental shielding is contrast preservation Juliet Stanton New York University Supplementary materials These supplementary materials contain four appendices and a bibli- ography: Appendix A: List of shielding languages 1 Appendix B: Additional information on shielding languages 4 Appendix C: List of non-shielding languages 37 Appendix D: Summary of vowel neutralisation survey 45 References 49 The materials are supplied in the form provided by the author. Appendices for “Environmental shielding is contrast preservation” Appendix A: list of shielding languages Key for appendices A-C Shaded = shielding occurs in this context Not shaded = shielding not known to occur in this context The language names provided in appendices A-C are those used by SAPhon. Evidence = type of evidence found for a vocalic nasality contrast, in addition to the author’s description. (MP = minimal or near-minimal pairs; NVNE: nasal vowels in non-nasal environments; –: no additional evidence available) Shielding contexts V-V?˜ (Evidence) Language Family Source Appendix B NV VN]σ V]σN Yes MP Ache´ Tup´ı Roessler (2008) #1, p. 4 Yes MP Aguaruna Jivaroan Overall (2007) #2, p. 4 Yes MP Amahuaca Panoan Osborn (1948) #3, p. 5 Yes MP Amarakaeri Harakmbet Tripp (1955) #4, p. 5 Yes MP Amundava Tup´ı Sampaio (1998) #5, p. 6 Yes MP Andoke (Isolate) Landaburu (2000a) #6, p. 6 Yes MP Apiaka´ Tup´ı Padua (2007) #7, p. 7 Yes MP Apinaye´ Macro-Ge Oliveira (2005) #8, p. 7 Yes – Arara´ do Mato Grosso Isolate da Rocha D’Angelis (2010) #9, p. 8 Yes MP Arikapu´ Macro-Ge Arikapu´ et al.
    [Show full text]
  • PDF Hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen
    PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen The following full text is a publisher's version. For additional information about this publication click this link. http://hdl.handle.net/2066/42006 Please be advised that this information was generated on 2021-09-24 and may be subject to change. Kwaza in a Comparative Perspective Author(s): Hein van der Voort Reviewed work(s): Source: International Journal of American Linguistics, Vol. 71, No. 4 (October 2005), pp. 365- 412 Published by: The University of Chicago Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/501245 . Accessed: 13/07/2012 09:37 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to International Journal of American Linguistics. http://www.jstor.org KWAZA IN A COMPARATIVE PERSPECTIVE1 Hein van der Voort Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi In view of the previous sparsity of data, the existing claims with regard to a genea- logical classification of the Aikanã, Kanoê, and Kwaza languages of Rondônia, on the Brazilian side of the Guaporé River, are premature and unconvincing.
    [Show full text]
  • Verbal Synthesis in the Guaporé-Mamoré Linguistic Area: a Contact Feature? Rik Van Gijn University of Zürich
    Verbal Synthesis in the Guaporé-Mamoré Linguistic Area: a Contact Feature? Rik van Gijn University of Zürich In their discussion of the linguistic area Guaporé-Mamoré (eastern Bolivia and Rondônia) Crevels & Van der Voort (2008) propose ‘polysynthetic morphology’ as one of the structural features shared by most languages in the area. In their approach the feature is regarded as binary (present versus absent) and it is not entirely clear what the basis for their categorization is. In this paper I try to come to a more precise understanding of the nature of the morphological similarities between the Guaporé-Mamoré languages by looking at a range of formal and semantic factors related to the verbal templates of these languages. In this way we can locate the cross-linguistic morphological similarities more precisely, setting the stage for a deeper understanding of the processes of contact-induced diffusion in the area. 1. Introduction Although (morphological) structure is often thought to be relatively impervious to borrowing (e.g. Weinreich 1953, Thomason & Kaufman 1988, Dunn et al. 2005) the geographical skewing of certain morphological parameters is, at least at first sight, suggestive of a sensitivity to contact (see van Gijn, this volume).1 In this paper I examine the question of potential contact-induced diffusion of morphological structure by zooming in on a putative linguistic area in western South America, called the Guaporé-Mamoré (GM), proposed by Crevels & Van der Voort (2008). They argue that the languages of this area, which belong to a number of different families, share a number of structural traits due to a history of contact between the speakers of the languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Mamainde Grammar Volume 2
    VU Research Portal Mamainde Grammar Eberhard, D. 2009 document version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link to publication in VU Research Portal citation for published version (APA) Eberhard, D. (2009). Mamainde Grammar: a Northern Nambikwara language and its cultural context. LOT. General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal ? Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. E-mail address: [email protected] Download date: 01. Oct. 2021 Mamaindê Grammar: A Northern Nambikwara language and its cultural context Volume II Published by LOT phone: +31 30 253 6006 Janskerkhof 13 fax: +31 30 253 6406 3512 BL Utrecht e-mail: [email protected] The Netherlands http://www.lotschool.nl Cover illustration: “Beyond the Threshold” – portrait of a Mamaindê child by Micah Eberhard (graphite on paper). ISBN: 978-94-6093-012-6 NUR 616 Copyright © 2009: David M. Eberhard. All rights reserved. VRIJE UNIVERSITEIT Mamaindê Grammar: a Northern Nambikwara language and its cultural context ACADEMISCH PROEFSCHRIFT ter verkrijging van de graad Doctor aan de Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, op gezag van de rector magnificus prof.dr.
    [Show full text]
  • The Noun Phrase in the Languages of South America
    301220 Olga Krasnoukhova Olga Krasnoukhova Olga Krasnoukhova The Noun Phrase in the The Noun Phrase in the Languages of South America Languages of South America This dissertation presents the first cross-linguistic study of the Noun Phrase in the indigenous languages of South America. It builds upon a considerable amount of data that have recently become available for languages in this continent. Based on a sample of 55 languages, this study gives a novel account of the syntactic, morphosyntactic, and semantic properties of the NP. For example, the analysis shows that personal pronouns commonly receive the same possessive markers as nominal possessors, which implies that a fully grammaticalized category of possessive pronouns is rare in South American languages. In addition, the new South American data only partly confirm typological claims Languages of South America The Noun Phrase in the for tendencies in the NP domain. For instance, a morphologically distinct class of adjectives is found in many languages of the sample; however, this class is often small, and the dominant way to encode property concepts is with verbs. Finally, this study also includes a discussion of the geographic patterning of structural features in the NP, evaluating the assumption that there is a major typological split between so-called Andean and Amazonian languages. The analysis shows that most of the features cannot be attributed to either of these larger areas. It also demonstrates, however, that there is some evidence for a broad structural division of languages into the western part of the continent (corresponding to the Andean sphere) and the rest of the continent.
    [Show full text]
  • Guia De Fontes E Bibliografia Sobre Línguas Indígenas E Produção Associada: Documentos Do CELIN 1
    Guia de Fontes e Bibliografia sobre Línguas Indígenas e Produção Associada: Documentos do CELIN 1 Referências Completas Código da Base Autor/Organizador Título e outras informações SÉRIE LIVROS DIGITAL 1 Guia de Fontes e Bibliografia sobre Línguas Indígenas e Produção Associada Documentos do CELIN Organizadora Marília Facó Soares Colaboradores Lourdes Cristina Araújo Coimbra Thays Lacerda Adilson Moreira Fontenele Museu Nacional/ UFRJ Rio de Janeiro 2013 Editores Universidade Federal Miguel Angel Monné Barrios e Ulisses Caramaschi do Rio de Janeiro Normalização Edson Vargas da Silva e Leandra de Oliveira Reitor Diagramação e Arte-final Carlos Antônio Levi Lia Ribeiro Capa Museu Nacional Edney Souza Editoração Eletrônica Diretora William de Lima Claudia Rodrigues Ferreira de Carvalho Antonio Clovis Britto de Araujo Guilherme de Oliveira Lima Serviços de Secretaria Thiago Macedo dos Santos Editores de Área Adriano Brilhante Kury, Ficha Catalográfica Ciro Alexandre Ávila Claudia Petean Bove G943 Guia de fontes e bibliografia sobre línguas indígenas e produção Débora de Oliveira Pires associada: documentos do CELIN / organizadora, Marília Facó Guilherme Ramos da Silva Muricy Soares; colaboradores, Lo urdes Cristina Araújo Coimbra ... [et ai.]. - Rio de Janeiro: Museu Nacional, 2013. lzabel Cristina Alves Dias 3.581 Kb; PDF João Alves de Oliveira João Wagner de Alencar Castro Com a colaboração de: Thays Lacerda, Adilson Moreira Fontenele. Marcela Laura Monné Freire ISBN 978-85-7427-047-0 Marcelo de Araújo Carvalho 1. Línguas indígenas - Bibliografias. 1. Soares, Marília Facó. 11. Marcos Raposo Coimbra, Lourdes Cristina Araújo. Ili. Lacerda, Thays. IV. Fontenele, Maria Dulce Barcellos Gaspar de Oliveira Adilson Moreira. V. Museu Nacional (Brasil). VI. Série.
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction
    See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/285591389 PRE-ORALIZED NASAL CODAS IN MAMAINDÊ And the Oral Vowel Enhancement Proposal Conference Paper · January 2011 CITATIONS READS 2 48 1 author: David Eberhard SIL International 21 PUBLICATIONS 387 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Community Based Language Development View project Language ecology View project All content following this page was uploaded by David Eberhard on 04 December 2015. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. PRE-ORALIZED NASAL CODAS IN MAMAINDÊ And the Oral Vowel Enhancement Proposal david m eberhard sil INTRODUCTION The various Nambikwara nations gained short lived notoriety in the 1940s thru the writings of the French anthropologist Claude Levi Strauss, who dubbed them the ‘earth people’, for their characteristic aversion to sleeping anywhere other than on the ground. These ‘earth peoples’ constitute a loose-knit cluster of bands, native to an area covering some 50,000 square kilometers in northwest Brazil, not far from the Bolivia border. When considered altogether, they number some 1200 individuals. However, each Nambikwara band considers itself quite independent from the others, with its own individual identity and language. Due to distinct variables of social contact, these small languages find themselves at various stages along the road to endangerment and extinction. In the midst of this rather fragile family is Mamaindê, spoken by some 250 individuals. It can be classified as a member of the Guapore cluster of the Northern Nambikwara branch of the Nambikwara language family.
    [Show full text]
  • A Comparative Study of Language Ecologies in Asia and the Americas
    Vol. 10 (2016), pp. 601–628 http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/ldc http://hdl.handle.net/10125/24719 Revised Version Received: 23 July 2016 A Tale of Two Worlds: A comparative study of language ecologies in Asia and the Americas Stan Anonby Simon Fraser University David M. Eberhard Payap University Language use patterns of individual speech communities are largely conditioned by the different language ecologies in which they are immersed. We believe this ecological stance helps explain why minority languages of Asia are more likely to be sustainable than those in the Americas. We have identified fourteen traits which characterize ecologies in general, describing how they play out differently in the Americas versus Asia. Each trait is considered to be on a continuum, with opposing values that measure whether conditions are more or less favorable to language maintenance. On one side of the continuum, we discuss the values in the Americas, and explain how these are more favorable to language shift. On the other side of the scale, we talk about the values in Asia, and explain how these are more conducive to language maintenance. To show the application of these traits, the paper also includes two in-depth case studies as prototypical examples from each area, one from the Americas and one from Asia. We conclude with some comments about how these traits can be useful for those engaged in language development work. The backstory This paper was birthed out of our life experiences. In the following paragraphs we will tell our stories. They are our accounts of field work among par- ticular tribes in two very distinct places.
    [Show full text]