What Is Political Psychology?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

What Is Political Psychology? What is Political Psychology? Rajiv S. Jhangiani, Kwantlen Polytechnic University Peter Suedfeld, University of British Columbia Reference: Suedfeld, S., & Jhangiani, R. (forthcoming). What is political psychology? In O. Feldman & S. Zmerli (Eds.), Politische Psychologie: Handbuch für Studium und Wissenschaft [Political Psychology: Handbook for Study and Science]. Baden-Baden, Germany: Nomos Verlagsgesellschaft. 2 What is Political Psychology? Politics permeates the world, and in the Broad sense proBaBly had done so even Before human Beings came on the stage. If politics has to do with such phenomena as power, access to resources, group cohesion, relations with other groups, and the like, then certainly pre-hominids too engaged in political Behavior (as most animals still do). As for Homo sapiens, we have engaged in political activities as far Back as our records and even legends show, and much of our present time is devoted to them. In fact, although in times past people tried to draw a line Between their private lives and their politics, some ideologically committed people go so far as to argue that even the personal is political. This is, obviously, an untestable assertion of belief, albeit one that shows the importance of politics in the minds of some. Nonetheless, it is fair to say that the issues mentioned aBove are relevant to every phase of human interaction from the family up through the gloBal international community. Philosophers appear to have considered the role of psychology in political behavior long Before psychology emerged as a discipline, as indicated by perspectives ranging from Plato’s prescription for disinterested and justice-seeking philosopher-kings to Machiavelli’s pragmatic, amoral analysis of an early form of Realpolitik -- advice prescribing how a leader can succeed by deceiving, manipulating, and when appropriate, destroying those who have challenged him or might do so in the future. What is often ignored is that Machiavelli, too, meant his advice to Be taken in the pursuit of good government. But although such topics are well within the concerns of today’s political psychologists, contemporary political 3 psychology is a much Broader enterprise. As the interdisciplinary study of the psychological aspects of human political Behavior, political psychology “explores the border that runs between the intellectual nations of political science and psychology” (Jost & Sidanius, 2004, p. 1). In doing so it encompasses such topics as electoral politics, the shaping of public policy, leadership, elite decision-making, intergroup relations, personality and political ideology, political violence, and conflict resolution. Political psychology addresses the political behaviors of the elite as well as the masses, in both cases exploring the reciprocal determinism Between political behavior, individual psychology, and the political context. Thus Marx’s revolutionary philosophy, Freud’s notion of Thanatos, or death instinct, and the research of Adorno and his colleagues on the development of authoritarianism can all be seen as products of their respective political contexts as much as they fueled political discourse for decades to come. Political psychology considers personal and environmental factors as intertwined rather than as distinct influences, with the psychology of the individual nested within the many layers of their political context. We cannot presume to understand the political behavior of either a Chancellor or a terrorist without first studying his or her personality, situational constraints, prevailing social and cultural norms, and the wider economic, historical, and geopolitical context. Understanding political behavior in turn allows us to make predictions, including, for example, about the likely impending decisions and actions of individual political actors. Of 4 course, given the vagaries and multiple determinants of human Behavior, those predictions are not necessarily correct. Finally, as with all of science, with understanding and prediction comes the ability to influence, including, for example, through developing effective re-election campaign strategies or de-radicalization programs. Who are Political Psychologists? Within the general discipline of psychology, political psychologists include a diverse group of social, personality, developmental, organizational, environmental, clinical, evolutionary, and more recently neuropsychologists. Their theoretical foundations come from all sources: depth psychology, Behaviorism, cognitive psychology, existential psychology, and others. However, although the field principally involves the application of psychological knowledge, not all political psychologists are psychologists. Many in the field identify themselves as political scientists, sociologists, economists, historians, literary critics, psychiatrists, media experts, legal scholars, policy analysts, or military strategists. As a result, the methods employed By political psychologists vary as widely as their disciplinary affiliations and topics of study, running the gamut from quantitative and statistical to qualitative and narrative, and including laboratory and field experiments, archival research, surveys, longitudinal studies, meta- analyses, interviews, and case studies. Although this may appear chaotic at first, the diversity within the field and the resultant ability to draw on insights from a multitude of disciplines and specialties is arguably one of the great strengths of political psychology. 5 The premier organization in the field is the International Society of Political Psychology (ISPP). It was founded in 1978, and has reached a membership of more than 800 at the present time. Those members come from all of the disciplines enumerated earlier, and from all over the world. To enhance its multinational reach, the ISPP moves its annual meetings around the world, rotating around North America (including the U.S., Canada, and Mexico), Europe, and locations outside Both of those. This enaBles political psychologists and interested students living anywhere to attend conferences at least occasionally with relatively little cost or travel. The diversity of the field and its outlets is also reflected in the wide patterns of origins of its adherents and their global concerns. For instance, an analysis of the last five years of issues of Political Psychology (January 2010 – December 2014) reveals that aBout half of the empirical articles puBlished each year were written by researchers from outside the United States and Canada, including from countries in Western and Eastern Europe, the Middle East, South America, Asia, Africa, and Australasia. Topics addressed during this period included victimhood experiences in Northern Ireland, competitive victimhood in Kosovo, anti-Semitism among American Jews, RomaphoBia among SerBian adolescents, religious Bias in Côte d’Ivoire, hereditary rights in England, indigenous rights in Chile, political participation in South Korea, political trust in China, Palestinian autobiographical memories, social cohesion in the Israeli military, and an evolutionary account of Kamikaze suicide attacks. The diversity of this list alone attests to the heterogeneity 6 and wide-ranging applications of the discipline, factors that also make it difficult to point to a common core. If political psychology has any general theoretical foundation, it may Be its acceptance of Kurt Lewin’s famous formulation that Behavior is a function of the interaction Between the person and the environment [B = f (P x E)], or to put it another way, between internal and external factors. Relevant internal factors include personality traits, temperament, heredity, demographic characteristics, attitudes, Beliefs, ideology, values, temporary states such as mood, emotional and physiological arousal, energy level, and health; external ones include the physical, social, political, and informational environment. Thus, for example, voters’ level of conservatism vs. liBeralism is a product of earlier learning, the opinions of their social circle, their gender, age, and socioeconomic class, staBle personality components (e.g., cognitive flexiBility, adventurousness, altruism, threat tolerance), philosophy of life, religious Beliefs, and – according to some new data – genetic makeup. In turn, conservatism/liBeralism interacts with economic conditions, international threat, the perceived personality of political leaders and media coverage to affect the person’s reaction to social and political programs and candidates. This general interactive expression can fit a wide variety of specific theories and models, although the relative roles of the internal and external components are weighted differently from one theory to the next. For example, The Authoritarian Personality (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswick, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950), a seminal work in political psychology, argues that child-rearing customs in families and 7 cultures are primarily responsiBle for people’s susceptiBility to Fascist propaganda and policies that emphasize suBmissiveness to authority figures, hostility toward dissenters and “outsider” ethnic groups, and aversion to change and uncertainty. Although external forces are paramount in shaping the individual’s outlook, the result is internalized. The now internal characteristic takes on a life of its own to produce not only a preference for an ethnocentric, orderly, unquestioning, top- down sociopolitical structure But for similar preferences in the workplace, in morality, entertainment,
Recommended publications
  • Reading List for SGR001F Political Psychology, 7.5 Credits, Third Cycle
    READING LIST 1 9 October 2019 Reg. no. U 2019/471 Reading list for SGR001F Political Psychology, 7.5 credits, third cycle The reading list was approved by the vice dean 9 October 2019 and is valid from the autumn semester 2019. Books Huddy, Leonie, David O. Sears and Jack S. Levy (eds.). 2013. Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology: Second Edition. New York: Oxford University Press. (Selected chapters, available online) Articles and book chapters Altemeyer, Bob. 2004. Highly dominating, highly authoritarian personalities. The Journal of Social Psychology 144: 421-447. Bäck, Emma A., Hanna Bäck and Holly Knapton. 2015. Group Belongingness and Collective Action: Effects of Need to Belong and Rejection Sensitivity on Willingness to Participate in Protests Activities. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 56: 537-544. Bäck, Hanna and Marc Debus. 2019. When do women speak? A comparative analysis of the role of gender in legislative debates. Political Studies 67 (3): 576-596. Barzegar, Abbas, Shawn Powers and Nagham El Karhili. 2016. Civic Approaches to Confronting Violent Extremism. Sector Recommendations and Best Practices. Georgia University. (59 pages) Borum, Randy. 2011. Radicalization into Violent Extremism I: A Review of Social Science Theories. Journal of Strategic Security 4: 7-36. Borum, Randy. 2011. Radicalization into Violent Extremism II: A Review of Conceptual Models and Empirical Research. Journal of Strategic Security 4: 37-62. Brader, Ted and George E. Marcus. 2013. Emotion and Political Psychology. In Huddy, Leonie, David O. Sears and Jack S. Levy (eds.). Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology: Second Edition. New York: Oxford University Press. Carney, Dana R., John T.
    [Show full text]
  • Stanford Prison Experiment - Wikipedia, the Free Encyclopedia
    Stanford prison experiment - Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Log in / create account Article Discussion Read Edit View history Stanford prison experiment From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Main page Contents The Stanford prison experiment was a study of the psychological effects of becoming a Featured content prisoner or prison guard. The experiment was conducted from August 14th to 20th[citation needed], Current events 1971 by a team of researchers led by Psychology professor Philip Zimbardo at Stanford University. Random article Twenty-four students were selected out of 75 to play the prisoners and live in a mock prison in the Donate to Wikipedia basement of the Stanford psychology building. Roles were assigned randomly. The participants adapted to their roles well beyond what even Zimbardo himself expected, leading the "Officers" to Interaction display authoritarian measures and ultimately to subject some of the prisoners to torture. In turn, Help many of the prisoners developed passive attitudes and accepted physical abuse, and, at the request About Wikipedia of the guards, readily inflicted punishment on other prisoners who attempted to stop it. The Community portal experiment even affected Zimbardo himself, who, in his capacity as "Prison Superintendent," lost Recent changes sight of his role as psychologist and permitted the abuse to continue as though it were a real prison. Contact Wikipedia Five of the prisoners were upset enough by the process to quit the experiment early, and the entire Toolbox experiment was abruptly stopped after only six days. The experimental process and the results remain controversial. The entire experiment was filmed, with excerpts made publicly available.
    [Show full text]
  • PS4417G: Special Topics in Political Psychology Course Description Course Text Course Assessment
    PS4417G: Special Topics in Political Psychology Department of Political Science – Western University, Winter 2020 Wednesday 1:30pm-3:30pm, SSC 4255 Instructor: Dr. Mathieu Turgeon Email: [email protected] Office hours: Tuesday from 10am-12pm or by appointment Course description The field of political psychology is vast and cuts through many subfields of political science. The focus in this course is about how theories of psychology apply to explain people’s political at- titudes and behaviours. In particular, the course is about how people receive, process, and use information they receive from their environment, interactions with others, the news media, and political elites to develop, change or maintain their political attitudes and make political decisions. Topics to be explored include candidate evaluation and choice, political knowledge and misinfor- mation, media effects, political polarization, and racial prejudice. Students will also be introduced to basic notions of the experimental design, a requisite to understanding the work produced in political psychology. Course text The required text for this course is: Druckman, J. N., Green, D. P., Kuklinski, J. H., & Lupia, A. (Eds.). 2011. Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science. Cambridge University Press. Other readings are available electronically through Western Libraries and the course’s OWL site. Course assessment Students will be assessed in many different ways, including class participation, reading quizzes, short essays, and a final take-home exam. • Class participation (12%): students will be responsible to sign up for leading class discussion at least three (3) times during the semester. • 3 reading quizzes each worth 6%: I expect students to do all the required readings.
    [Show full text]
  • Bilali, Rezarta -CV-Jan 2021
    Rezarta Bilali CV- 2021 1 Rezarta Bilali, PhD Department of Applied Psychology New York University 246 Greene Street, Kimball Hall, rm 801 New York, NY10003 tel: +1(212)9985155 email: [email protected] ACADEMIC EMPLOYMENT 2020- Associate Professor, Psychology and Social Intervention Department of Applied Psychology New York University, USA 2013-2020 Assistant Professor, Psychology and Social Intervention Department of Applied Psychology New York University, USA 2009-2013 Assistant Professor, Department of Conflict Resolution, Human Security, and Global Governance, McCormack Graduate School of Policy Studies University of Massachusetts Boston, Mass. EDUCATION 2009 University of Massachusetts at Amherst PhD Social Psychology Concentration: Psychology of Peace and Violence Minor: Quantitative Methods 2004 Sabanci University, Istanbul, Turkey MA Conflict Resolution 2001 Bogazici University, Istanbul, Turkey BA Psychology HONORS/AWARDS 2020 Otto Klineberg Intercultural and International Relations Award, Honorable Mention Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues 2017 Daniel E. Griffiths Award for Outstanding Research New York University – Steinhardt 2017 Goddard Fellowship Award New York University – Steinhardt 2016 Roberta Sigel Early Career Scholar Paper Award International Society for Political Psychology 2014 Michele Alexander Early Career Award APA Division 9, Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues 2014 W. Gabriel Carras Research Award New York University – Steinhardt Rezarta Bilali CV- 2021 2 2010 Outstanding Dissertation Award Finalist The International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology 2010 Unique Honorable Mention for Dissertation Thesis International Society for Political Psychology 2008/2004 Network Supplementary Grant Fellowship Open Society Institute and SOROS Foundation PUBLICATIONS (* denotes student co-authors) Journal Articles (peer reviewed) Ulug, M., Bilali, R., Karasu, M., & Malo, L.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognition and Emotion in Extreme Political Action: Individual Differences and Dynamic Interactions
    Cognition and Emotion in Extreme Political Action: Individual Differences and Dynamic Interactions Leor Zmigrod1,2,3 & Amit Goldenberg4 Affiliations: 1Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK 2Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK 3Churchill College, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK 4Harvard Business School, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA Contact: Dr Leor Zmigrod, [email protected], +44 7522193934. Paper accepted and in press at Current Directions in Psychological Science. Can be tempo- rarily cited as: Zmigrod, L., & Goldenberg, A. (2020). Cognition and Emotion in Extreme Political Action: Individual Differences and Dynamic Interactions. Current Directions in Psychological Sci- ence. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/w3hj6 1 Abstract Who is most likely to join and engage in extreme political action? While traditional theories have focused on situational factors or group identity attributes, an emerging science illustrates that tendencies for extreme political action may also be rooted in individuals’ idiosyncratic cognitive and affective dispositions. This paper synthesizes cutting-edge evidence demonstrating that an individual’s cognitive and affective architecture shape their willingness to support ideological violence. From a cognitive perspective, traits such as cognitive rigidity, slower perceptual strategies, and poorer executive functions are linked to heightened endorsement for ideological violence. From an emotional standpoint, characteristics associated with emotional reactivity and impaired emotional regulation, such as sensation-seeking and impulsivity, can facilitate readiness for extreme political action. The review hones in on the roles of cognitive rigidity and sensation-seeking as traits heightening proclivities for extreme pro-group behavior, and recommends that future research should aim to assess cognition-emotion interactions to reveal different sub-profiles of political actors.
    [Show full text]
  • Operantsissue III, 2017 from the President
    ISSN 2476-0293 OperantsISSUE III, 2017 from the president was recently talking with a researcher in child development. Naturally, I mentioned Skinner. The researcher said, “of course his science works in the lab, but I have to work in the real world.” Scientific principles, I pointed Iout, are universal. Gravity does not work just in a lab. Nor do the laws of operant conditioning. Skinner made his discovery in a lab, but selection by consequences operates with all organisms and in all settings. This issue of Operants illustrates places around the world where behaviorology is “working.” Julie S. Vargas, Ph.D. President, B. F. Skinner Foundation Chinese Traditional Translated by Kiwiya Zhang 最近我与一位儿童发展领域的研究者聊天。很自然地,我提起了Skinner。这位研究者说:“当然他的科学在实验室里说得通,但我却是要在真实 世界里工作。”我指出,科学的原则是具有普遍性的。万有引力不仅仅在实验室里有用,操作制约也是。Skinner在实验室里发现了后效对行为的 作用,但该作用在所有场合、对所有生命体都有效。本期Operants就阐释了行为学在哪些地方“有用”。 Czech Translated by Helena Vadurova Nedávno jsem mluvila s jedním výzkumníkem, který se zabývá vývojem dítěte. Přirozeně jsem zmínila Skinnera. Ten výzkumník mi řekl: „jeho práce samozřejmě funguje v laboratoři, ale já musím pracovat ve skutečném světě.“ Vědecké principy, poznamenala jsem, jsou uni- verzální. Gravitace funguje nejen v laboratoři. Stejné je to se zákony operantního podmiňování. Skinner svůj objev učinil v laboratoři, ale výběr na základě následků funguje u všech organizmů a ve všech prostředích. Toto vydání časopisu Operants představuje různá místa na světě, kde behaviorální věda „funguje“. French Translated by MarieCeline Clemenceau J’ai récemment échangé avec un chercheur dans le développement de l’enfant. Naturellement, j’ai mentionné Skinner. Le chercheur a déclaré: «Bien sûr, sa science fonctionne dans un laboratoire, mais je dois travailler dans le monde réel».
    [Show full text]
  • Voting: a Behavioral Analysis
    Behavior and Social Issues, Volume 6, No. 1, Spring 1996 Voting: A Behavioral Analysis Max Visser University of Twente ABSTRACT: The behavior of voting for a party in an election has important social implications, yet, due to strong mentalistic influences in electoral research, it has rarely been studied from the behavior analytical perspective. In this article a behavioral analysis of voting is presented and a derived behavioral model is empirically tested on data from a Dutch election survey. It is concluded that such an analysis is both theoretically fruitful and empirically relevant. Introduction Of all possible behaviors a person emits in the course of his life, few may have as many implications as the behavior of voting for a party in an election. By casting a vote, the individual person, directly or indirectly, lays down a course of action for parliaments and governments, and, thus, for the sociopolitical and physical organization of the country he or she inhabits. Through elections, leaders have risen to power who dramatically changed the course of world politics and instigated policies affecting the daily lives of millions. While socially relevant, voting behavior seldom has been researched from a behavior analytical perspective (an exception is Lamal & Greenspoon, 1992). For a long time the study of voting has been dominated by normative considerations, ascribing to the average voter a strong sense of rationality with regard to political matters. The first psychological investigations of electoral behavior quickly shattered this optimistic vision, but they replaced it by a form of semi-rationality, according to which the voter's choices at the voting booths are determined by more or less stable set of mental constructs (political beliefs, attitudes, values, etc.) (Visser, 1994a).
    [Show full text]
  • Fall 2015 PSYCHOLOGY of POLITICAL
    Fall 2015 PSYCHOLOGY OF POLITICAL BEHAVIOR 790-586 Mondays, 3:00 - 5:40, Hickman 313 Professor: Richard R. Lau Phone: (848) 932-6685 Email: [email protected] Office Hours: Tues, 2 - 4, 505 Hickman Hall; and by appointment. To the extent that political scientists study individual political beliefs and behavior, they rely heavily on theories from psychology. Studies of international relations, political culture, public opinion and voting behavior, race, ethnicity and gender, etc., all rely to a greater or lesser extent on some psychological theory of individual behavior. This course looks explicitly at the interface between psychology and politics, especially public opinion and voting behavior (as American politics is the area of politic science I know best). We will occasionally all read applications of political psychology to other subfields, however, and I encourage each of you to explore some of the political psychology literature from other subfields. I have chosen seven broad topic areas in which interesting research is being conducted in political psychology. For each of these seven areas, we will initially spend some class time -- in several cases an entire class period -- obtaining a general overview of psychological theory, and then spend a class or two looking at the research applying those psychological theories to political science. Again, the political science research will generally be in the area of American politics, but the psychology theories we learn should be applicable to research in other areas of political science as well, including certainly all of the subfields in our department. Requirements The course assumes at least a passing acquaintance with research in American politics (as one would learn from the American politics proseminar), comparative politics, international relations, and/or women and politics, as these are the substantive areas of political science which have most strongly utilized ideas and theories from psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology As a Historical Science
    HISTORICAL PSYCHOLOGY 1 PSYCHOLOGY AS A HISTORICAL SCIENCE Michael Muthukrishna Department of Psychological anD Behavioral Science LonDon School of Economics anD Political Science Joseph Henrich Department of Human Evolutionary Biology HarvarD University EdwarD SlingerlanD Department of Asian StuDies University of British Columbia HISTORICAL PSYCHOLOGY 2 Abstract Psychology has traDitionally seen itself as the science of universal human cognition, anD has only recently begun seriously grappling with the issue of cross-cultural variation. Here we argue that the roots of cross-cultural variation often lie in the past. Therefore, to unDerstanD not just the way, but also why psychology varies, we also neeD to grapple with cross-temporal variation. Psychology neeDs to become a historical science. The traces of past human cognition accessible through historical texts anD artifacts can also serve as a valuable, anD yet currently almost completely unutilizeD, source of psychological Data. This data from DeaD minDs opens up a new anD untappeD, highly Diverse subject pool. The cohort effects we measure are a sliver of the temporal variation we shoulD expect over history. We review examples of research that may be classifieD as historical psychology, introDuce sources of Data anD methoDs for analyzing historical Data, explain for the critical role of theory, anD Discuss the variety of ways in which psychologists can aDD historical depth and nuance to their work. Historical psychology is a critical next step towarD becoming a genuinely universal science. Keywords: cultural evolution, cultural psychology, culture, historical Databases, large-scale textual analysis, science-humanities integration Introduction Our psychology is shapeD by our societies (Henrich 2016; Henrich et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Political Psychology Syllabus
    Special Topics in American Politics Political Psychology Dr. Christopher Weber Department of Political Science Manship School of Mass Communication 204 Stubbs Hall [email protected] 8-6395 Office Hours: Monday 1-4PM (and by appointment) Objective The purpose of this course is to introduce students to historical and contemporary perspectives in political psychology. As a sub-discipline straddling political science and psychology, a thorough understanding of political psychology necessitates theoretical and methodological diversity. Many of the readings in this course will be drawn from social and cognitive psychology, as well as scholarship in political science. This course serves as a foundation for the study of political psychology, especially as it pertains to mass political behavior. What is political psychology? Is it an application of psychological theory to political processes? Is it applied social and cognitive psychology? Or, is political psychology an application of political theory to psychology? The answer to all these is, “yes and no!” Political psychology is the interdisciplinary pursuit of psychological and political questions; it brings together psychological understandings to political questions, as well as exploring how political contexts influence the ways people behave politically. There is a heavy degree of cross-fertilization in political psychology. Psychological theories have been defined and refined after being tested in the political domain. Likewise, how we understand politics and political processes has been influenced by psychological theory. Thus, in order to thoroughly understand the state of political psychology, it is necessary to sample from both historical and contemporary works in both political science and psychology. In this course we will begin with an overview of the field of political psychology and the ways that questions within political psychology have been studied.
    [Show full text]
  • Political Bulletin
    Political Psychology Section Bulletin December 2019 Welcome to your second issue of the Political Psychology Section Bulletin. You are receiving this as one of almost 200 members who have joined since the Section was approved in November 2019. Thank you so much for your support and participation over the last year and we look forward to seeing you in upcoming events in Birmingham and London, after a busy year which has attracted members to Salford and Harrogate and seen two elections. See inside this issue for news of upcoming events and an exciting winter line up and please get in touch with your ideas for the spring issue. From the Editorial team Pressure on our politicians to perform In this Issue 1. From the Editorial team o, we promised you another We know that even for up to 80 general election! Yes, those who MPs the emotions aroused by the 2. Upcoming events Shave attended the Psychology EU referendum were the tipping of Democracy conference in Salford point which led to their choice to 3. Featured articles from will recall that each time we hold this stand down. While we might expect members Saskia Perriard- biennial event, a general election them to have a more cushioned Abdoh, Steve Myers, takes place in the same year. political afterlife, this is not always Jill Ruddock and the case, especially following the Antigonos Sochos Of course, this has to be psychological battering so many coincidence, doesn’t it? Hmmm… have taken on the tumultuous Brexit 4. Book overview by well, we couldn’t possibly comment.
    [Show full text]
  • 50Th Anniversary Reflections on the Stanford Prison Experiment and Related Research Philip G
    1 50th Anniversary Reflections on the Stanford Prison Experiment and Related Research Philip G. Zimbardo, Ph.D. Professor Emeritus of Psychology, Stanford University (August 14, 2021) In August 1971, I undertook what was destined to become the landmark Stanford Prison Experiment. It demonstrated the power of situational forces to overwhelm otherwise good people to become cruel, when role-playing prison guards, or emotionally overwhelmed as prisoners. Its takeaway message is that the majority of ordinary people, even good ones, can be seduced, recruited, initiated into behaving in evil ways under the sway of powerful systematic and situational forces, both the evil of action—to do bad things against others, as well as the evil of inaction—to do nothing when they could/should do the right thing, being helpful and compassionate. The weeklong experiment quickly gained international notoriety following two prison riots that erupted shortly after its conclusion. The day after my study was concluded, there was an alleged prison escape at San Quentin Prison in California by Black activist George Jackson, who was murdered along with several prison guards. Three weeks later, in upstate New York, riots rocked Attica Prison and took over the entire prison, in part as a reaction to the loss of George Jackson. The governor of New York, Nelson Rockefeller, later ordered the National Guard to shoot to kill all prisoners and guards being held hostage in the open prison yard. I was invited as an expert witness in Congressional hearings on prisons held in Washington, D.C., and also in San Francisco. My testimony on how prisons need to be reformed became part of the congressional record.
    [Show full text]