<<

ISSN: 2161-0231 (ONLINE)

Confabulation: Connections between Damage, , and Testimony

Jerrod Brown, Jeffrey Long-McGie, Pamela Oberoi, Judge Anthony Wartnik, Janina Wresh, Erv Weinkauf, & Gennae Falconer

ABSTRACT

Confabulation is the act of honestly lying, providing information based on inaccurate whether those memories were provoked by questions or arose spontaneously (Moscovitch, 1989). Confabulation is not a function of the disturbed but an action that is part of the human condition. It is not uncommon to create and hold false memories, or even participate in the act of self-deception. Confabulation is simply an act of filling in the holes, creating a semblance of coherency, and improving the narrative (Gallagher, 2003). However, when this simple act enters the criminal justice system as the form of testimony, it can have a perverse long-term corrupting impact on the system resulting in an unintentional loss of liberty, and potentially even death. While there is no way to quantify how often confabulation results in false testimony, it is known that significant groups of individuals with brain damage predisposed to confabulate are regularly in contact with the system as perpetrators, witnesses, bystanders, and victims.

www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5

Confabulation is important in the context of the criminal justice system, since the veracity and reliability of testimony is crucial in proceedings geared towards understanding actions and events that have taken place during criminal evaluations and criminal trials. Recently, confabulation has been redefined as a wide range of errors in memory, including distortions, descriptions of false realities, and less dramatic errors such as the description of intrusions, embellishments and elaborations, and the paraphrasing of existing memories (Guerri et al., 2009). Simply, this is not the same thing as lying. Liars are aware of the falsehood of their statements; confabulators are not. This differs from malingering, which is commonly described as a conscious fraudulent production of physical or psychological symptoms to secure something desired (LoPiccolo et al., 1999; Gorman, 1982). In the forensic setting, the individual would fake , psychotic symptomology, or cognitive difficulty in an effort to escape interrogation or any criminal responsibility (Parwatikar, 1985). A testimonial confession is a critical piece of the conviction process and the most convincing evidence to juries (Kassin & Neumann, 1997). “The introduction of a confession makes other aspects of a trial in court superfluous,” according to Supreme Court Justice William Brennan (Colorado v. Connelly, 1986). While it remains difficult to determine the exact number of individuals who have falsely confessed, in Europe 12% of prisoners allege to have falsely confessed (Gudjonsson, 2003), and a study of Icelandic students with - Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) identified 261 individuals that had falsely confessed to law enforcement (Gudjonsson et al., 2009). In the United States, “Perske’s List” in its most recently updated form has grown to 75 documented false confessions of individuals with intellectual disability (Perske, 2011), and the Innocence Project reported as of April 17, 2014 that 25% of the 316 exonerated individuals were convicted based on false confessions (http://www.innocenceproject.org). Further, Kassin et al., found that false confessions appeared to have a corrupting effect on the entire criminal investigation resulting in subsequent errors associated with other evidence including; forensic science, eyewitness, and snitch/informant (Kassin et al., 2012). The limitations of testimony, however does not end with the study of the false confession. In fact, testimony from the accused, the eyewitness, or the informant remains integral to the criminal justice system; despite knowing it is less than flawless (Wells & Loftus, 1984). This testimony often constitutes an important piece of information or evidence from which judicial cases are constructed. Differentiating confabulation from deliberate lying may aid in the process of uncovering truth and differentiating valuable from useless information. In order to identify these differences, a basic understanding is intended to create an awareness of the nature and existence of situations that could be more complex than they might first appear. What sometimes appears to be a deliberate lie may be, in reality, unintentional. There is heightened importance in the criminal justice system to identify confabulation by potential witnesses, and defendants themselves, in the form of false confessions. False testimony (false confession, false eyewitness, false informant/cooperating witness) was integral to the conviction of 195 of the 200 individuals that later had their convictions overturned as a result of the work of the Innocence Project (Garrett, 2008). While it is beyond reason to assume that all of the convictions were related to confabulation, thus far, it remains an unanswered question that needs to be explored. In working with psychiatric patients, the presence of confabulation needs to be strongly considered.

“Honestly Lying“ - What is Confabulation? Confabulation is the communication of falsely constructed answers and information by an individual recounting something he or she genuinely believes to be the truth (Anastasi, www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5

2006). This phenomenon occurs through the replacement of gaps in memory with imaginary information (Gudjonsson, 1992), causing accounts that lack correspondence to real events. Essentially, a falsification of memories through guessing or the imagining of an event occurs; backfilling and assuming the reality of the imagined event turn into a “final product,“ which is believed to be true by the individual recounting the events (Zannino et al., 2008). In effect, the appears to be filling in the missing memory with information that it recognizes should fit, thus explaining the situation and creating a robust memory of the event. Loftus and Palmer evaluated the importance of word choice when questioning a witness (1974). The word “smashed” was substituted for the word “hit” while questioning witnesses of car accident videos. Participants falsely recalled seeing broken glass at a greater rate when the word smashed was used (32%) than when the word hit was used (14%). They also judged the speed of the car to be nearly 7 mph faster when it was “smashed.” This is contrary to liars who are well aware of the falsehood of their statements (Zannino et al., 2008). The range of confabulation can be broad and contain errors in memory, distortions, and descriptions of false realities, as well as less dramatic errors such as the description of intrusions, embellishments, elaborations, and the paraphrasing of existing memories (Guerri et al., 2009). Of course, a general awareness exists about the fallibility of human memory in regards to omission and commission, constituting either the of details and experiences or leading to the distortion of memories. While amnesia is a well-known and researched condition of memory loss, confabulation constitutes the pathologically exaggerated form of memory distortion and is less well understood (Fotopoulou et al., 2008). Memories of events that have not actually taken place may include vivid contextual and perceptual details, making the accounts appear as though they were true. Often, individuals claim that they are able to relive these memories and, in fact, sometimes act on or respond to their (Ciaramelli et al., 2007). Examples of this behavior may be seen in the bereavement over the death of an imaginary friend. Other emotional responses may include constructed memories and situations, such as the search for a phone number of an imagined person (Schnider 2003). These types of behaviors suggest confabulators subjectively experience memories as being real, and potentially base actions and decisions on false or distorted experiences (Goel et al., 2003).

Causes of Confabulation Many existing theories attempt to explain the reasons and etiology of confabulation. A of events due to a disturbed sense of chronology may cause confabulation when memories from an older time period are judged as currently relevant (Turner & Coltheart, 2010). Spontaneous confabulation, which does not result from an external cue, may be rooted in an impaired ability to filter information. Habits and recurring events may also play a role (Nahum et al., 2009). Another problem may be found in the difficulty to differentiate between incoming new information and information that has been stored in old memories. This old information, regardless of its relevance to the current topic, can often not be suppressed or filtered, and adds to the creation of the story. This process appears to be directly linked to confabulation (Schnider, 2008). Spontaneous confabulation has been linked to an inability to recognize the temporal order of stored information, which results in erroneous recollections of different elements of memories that originally did not correspond (Schnider et al., 1996). In these cases, a persistent outpouring of erroneous memories occurs (Kopelman, 2010). Provoked confabulation depends on the search for information in deficit memory (Schnider et al., 1996), which results in fleeting intrusion errors or distortions that essentially arise in response to a challenge of the individual’s memory (Kopelman, 2010). Provoked confabulation occurs www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5

when the individual is prompted for a response or has received external cues (Kessels et al., 2008). Another form of confabulation is provoked, wherein the memory incorporates errors or distortions as a response to a challenge (Kopelman, 1987). While provoked confabulation occurs more frequently after brain damage, spontaneous confabulation has been linked to changes and lesions in the brain structure, particularly pertaining to changes in the , hypothalamus, and the dorsomedial thalamic nucleus (Schnider, 2008). Damage to the can lead to impaired memory of perceptual context features (Ciaramelli & Spaniol, 2009) and to difficulties in distinguishing memories from other mental products (Fotopoulou et al., 2007). This often occurs in confabulation with autobiographical content, such as the man who questioned his wife one evening about why she keeps telling other people that they are married, when they are not; the hospital employee who recounted regular visits from her parents although they had been dead for 4 and 20 years; or the 61 year old man who talked about having recently taken his girlfriend home to meet his parents (Kopelman, 2010).

Clinical Relevance A number of neurologic and non-neurologic reasons exist that can potentially present causes for confabulation. Developmental impairment may constitute one of the many complex causes of confabulation (Gibbard et al., 2003). From a developmental perspective, telling tales is typical in younger children. Older individuals suffering from Fetal Spectrum Disorder (FASD), or other neurological damages, may exhibit similar symptoms due to an arrested development thereby exhibiting behaviors that are appropriate for much younger individuals (Nedjam et al., 2004) and are congruent with appropriate age-related confabulation behaviors. We will turn to a brief discussion of some of the relevant clinical disorders that appear to be implicated in confabulation.

Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder. Actual prevalence rates pertaining to the percentage of individuals with FASD who are incarcerated in the United States are unknown (Burd et al., 2004). One study estimated the prevalence of individuals with FASD to be as high as 10% in one federal correctional facility (MacPherson & Chudley, 2006). In another study of a selected population of delinquents undergoing mandated psychiatric or psychological assessments, the rate of FASD was 23.3% (Conry et al., 1999). Further, these individuals are often neglected, abused, and mistreated (LaDue et al., 1992). Prenatal alcohol exposure has been associated with widespread neuropsychological deficits across several domains, causing significant impairment and disadvantages for affected individuals (Gibbard et al., 2003). Damages and deficits can impact general intelligence, memory, language, attention, learning, visuospatial abilities, executive functioning, fine and gross motor skills, and social and adaptive functioning (Guerri et al., 2009; Schnider, 2008). Impairments in verbal and nonverbal learning and memory may be connected to low IQ levels (Guerri et al., 2009). However, the individual does not have to suffer from a low IQ to express significant impairment, highlighting why many refer to FASD as an invisible disease. The lack of visible cues even after the first few meetings/interactions can lead to unreasonable assumptions and expectations (Sanders & Buck, 2010). Diminished intellectual capacity is a common neurocognitive finding (Mattson et al., 2011). Studies suggest that long-term retention of verbal information is intact; however, processes may be impaired (Pei et al., 2008). Abnormalities in the frontal-subcortical pathway and damage to the left hemisphere have been linked to visuospatial processing deficits (Guerri et al., 2009; Mattson et al., 1996). Executive function and higher order cognitive processes are often impacted by www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5

prenatal exposure to alcohol. Frontal-subcortical circuits that involve projections from other parts of the brain have been found to be highly vulnerable to the exposure of ethanol (Mattson et al., 2011; Fryer et al., 2007). Executive functioning is necessary to maintain adequate problem solving abilities for the attainment of future goals, and involves the ability to plan, organize, strategize, notice, contextualize details, and manage time and space (Welsh et al., 1988). Impairment of executive functioning can have devastating effects on individuals and the ability to comprehend or communicate effectively (Schonfeld et al., 2006; Jirikowic et al., 2008). These individuals have issues with , inhibitory control, and generally fail to understand consequences (Rasmussen, 2005). Difficulties are related to keeping track of time and simultaneous tasks, as well as skills related to planning, structure, and organization. Difficulties can include telling stories (e.g., verbal or written) or recounting situations and experiences in a detailed, organized, and sequential manner. Many existing theories attempt to explain the reasons and etiology behind this phenomenon. It appears that the individual is suffering from a disturbed sense of chronology. This confusion of events may cause confabulation when earlier memories are judged as currently relevant (Clare & Gudjonsson, 2010; Turner & Coltheart, 2010; Pedzdek et al., 2009; Fast & Conry, 2006). These difficulties can be accentuated in stressful situations where the mind attempts to problem solve; trying to remember, and the individual may be faced with faulty evidence of guilt (Kassin & Kiechel, 1996; Horselenberg et al., 2003; Nash & Wade, 2009).

Korsakoff’s Syndrome. Korsakoff’s syndrome is a neuropsychiatric condition characterized by severe amnesia in the absence of (Krabbendam, et al., 2000). Patients suffering from Korsakoff’s syndrome may exhibit severe confabulation behavior (Kessels et al., 2008). Studies indicate that a high percentage of patients present with spontaneous confabulation due to source memory impairment caused by temporal confusion (Kessels et al., 2008; Van Oort et al., 2009). In most cases, chronic , in conjunction with malnutrition, causes lesions in the diencephalon, leading to problems with executive functioning and confabulation, due to memory issues (Van Oort et al., 2009). The identification of this , the suspected direct connection to memory impairment, and the production of confabulations serve to enhance behavior caused by memory impairment and confabulation (Van Oort et al., 2009).

Dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type. Confabulation occurs when memory is impaired (Kern et al., 1992). Often the individual is completely unaware of their disorder (Weinstein et al., 1994) confabulating when attempting to memories or make plans (Barba et al., 1999). Severity of cognitive decline appears to correlate with frequency of confabulation behavior (Tallberg & Almkvist, 2001). A study by Attali and colleagues (2009) implies that it is the encoding processes in the brain which may be disrupted in Alzheimer’s patients and not the retrieval of memories, as might be presumed. Studies were conducted with patients by presenting them with three different kinds of fairy tales and asking them to recall the content. Alzheimer’s Disease patients produced significantly more confabulations in the recall of a modified fairy tale compared with the recall of the two other stories. This points to a strong representation of over-learned and well-known information (Attali et al., 2009) and could mean that gaps in memory are filled in with experiences or knowledge pertaining to remote memory. When these patients were asked what activities they had planned for the day, their response often included well-established memories from their past (Kopelman, 2010).

www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5

Traumatic Brain and other anatomical considerations. A recent study of 384 juveniles ages 16 to 18 years old in the New York City jail system indicated the incidence of TBI was approximately 50% (Kaba et al., 2014). Studies conducted with World War II veterans who had suffered brain injury, particularly damage, found correlations between impairments and confabulation (Glowinski et al., 2008). It can be assumed that any blunt trauma causing damage to the prefrontal cortex may trigger confabulation behaviors. However, some studies have identified situations in which the patient has not actually experienced structural damage to the prefrontal cortex, as revealed through the case study of a man who suffered the rupture of the left posterior communicating artery prior to exhibiting amnesic and confabulatory symptoms (Barba et al., 1997). This implies that vulnerability of areas in the brain through indirect trauma or deprivation potentially causes damage, which can result in impaired memory and possibly confabulation. Some patients exhibiting confabulation behavior experience disturbances of the cerebral anterior artery, which supplies the frontal regions of the brain (Glowinski et al., 2008). Cases of confabulation have also been reported in patients with herpes , multiple sclerosis, and following suicide attempts involving hanging, in which the brain stem or spine are damaged or blood flow was reduced, resulting in brain damage (Glowinski et al., 2008).

Delusions. and delusional memories are not the same thing. While delusions can arise in a multitude of contexts and may be associated with or , delusional memories are rare (Kopelman, 1999). Either consisting of a true memory that is subject to deluded interpretation, or arising due to as a (Kopelman, 1997). Although the underlying pathology of the disorders differs, Fotopoulou suggests that a complex interaction between and lack of information may play a role in the creation of confabulation from a psychological perspective. Early adverse experiences and trauma may create persistent and enduring cognitive vulnerability, which might be linked to confabulation (Fotopoulou, 2010). Delusional memories can be differentiated from confabulations, because of the lack of executive dysfunction involvement (Kopelman, 1999).

Schizophrenia. Confabulation has also been witnessed in patients with schizophrenia, independent of intelligence (Nathaniel-James & Frith, 1996). Studies conducted with individuals suffering from this disorder show a significant increase of intrusion errors, typical for confabulation (Kopelman, 2010; Lorente-Rovira, 2010). A difference in the content of these confabulations was identified and they were often unrelated to the topic. The confabulations were also colored by the preoccupations typical to the individual’s delusional beliefs and content, often appearing to be bizarre (Kopelman, 2010). Morias and colleagues (2004) have hypothesized that that confabulations may be less an issue of memory impairment, and more an issue with verbal comprehension difficulties in this patient population. In addition, some studies have found that schizophrenic patients are unable to differentiate plausible from implausible errors and, as a result, are unable to correct these errors (Kopelman, 2010).

Childhood sexual abuse, trauma, and the recovery of memories. Kopelman (2010) identified difficulties in the accurate retrieval of childhood sexual abuse memories, especially for those events that occurred during early childhood. Fragments of memories are likely to be inaccurate or distorted due to difficulties in location of these memories in a temporal or special context (Kopelman, 2010). Trauma has been known to affect memory and cause a wide range of memory impairments, including amnesia and dissociation (Davies et al., 1998). www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5

Testimony: Suggestibility and Reliability Increasing understanding of definitions, possible origins, and connections to brain functioning and emotional affect is important in order to create awareness about confabulation and its connection to reliability in communication. Further understanding is also necessary to curb the risk of false confession, false testimony, and to avoid the negative consequences thereof. There is a high risk of suspects exhibiting confabulatory behavior due to neuropsychological deficits taking responsibility for uncommitted acts. The risk of being misunderstood by police officers, lawyers, and courts can be minimized if comprehensive medical and legal reports are assessed regarding the individual’s ability to testify adequately (Fast & Conry, 2006). Additionally, effort should be made by those performing the interviews to identify those with obvious deficits prior to the even unintentional disclosure of pertinent investigative data that has been kept from the public. Suspects suffering from neuropsychological impairments are at a profound disadvantage and are vulnerable when interviewed or questioned by police. Impaired understanding of legal rights; susceptibility to acquiescence, suggestibility, and compliance can lead to confabulation (Clare & Gudjonsson, 2010). Suggestive questioning is highly unreliable and can lead to false testimony (Pezdek et al., 2009), especially since confusion in the temporal context tends to provoke unchecked responses to external cues (Kopelman, 2010). Studies have documented circumstances in which individuals make confabulated false confessions due to high suggestibility, long duration of the interview, circumstances related to amnesia, and a distrust in memories or lack thereof (Kopelman, 2010). Social motivational factors and interview dynamics have proven to be relevant in the development of false memories (Zaragosa et al., 2001). When incriminating evidence is presented to an individual with confabulation behavior, the individual is likely to internalize the evidence as truth, even when false, as a result of susceptibility to suggestibility (Kassin et al., 1996). In order to obtain relevant information for the purpose of serving justice, it is important to be able to identify the difference between confabulation and lying. This short compilation of possible causes is intended to raise awareness about this complex problem. Potentially, the individual’s reality can be misrepresented, regardless of whether the person is a suspect, a witness, or simply questioned with the objective of obtaining information. Although the testimony of some individuals may be false, this does not necessarily constitute lying, which at first sight, may appear to be contradictory. However, differentiating confabulation behavior from the deliberation of lying will bring us closer to the objective of identifying the veracity and reliability of testimony. Further research is needed and more training must be provided on the nature of confabulation pertaining to suggestibility, unreliable testimony, and self-incriminating statements. Awareness of potential pathologies that influence testimony and create unreliable testimony can easily be categorized and differentiated, and the criminal justice system may be enhanced through this awareness.

References

Anastasi, J.S. (2006). Understanding confabulation: A multidisciplinary approach. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 20, 275–278, Attali, E., De Anna, F., Dubois, B., & Dalla Barba, G. (2009). Confabulation in Alzheimer’s disease: poor encoding and retrieval of over-learned information. Brain, A Journal of , 132, 204–212. Barba, G., Boissé, M., Bartolomeo, P., & Bachoud-Lévi, A. (1997). Confabulation following rupture of posterior communicating artery. Cortex; A Journal Devoted To The Study Of The Nervous System and Behavior, 33, 563-570. www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5

Barba, G. D., Nedjam, Z., & Dubois, B. (1999). Confabulation, , and source memory in Alzheimer's disease. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16, 385-398. Burd, L., Selfridge, R., Klug, M., & Bakko, S. (2004). Fetal alcohol syndrome in the United States corrections system. Addiction Biology, 9(2), 169-176. Ciaramelli, E., & Ghetti, S. (2007). What are confabulators’ memories made of? A study of subjective and objective measures of recollection in confabulation. Neuropsychologia 45, 1489–1500. Ciaramelli, E., & Spaniol, J. (2009). Ventromedial Prefrontal Damage and Memory for Context: Perceptual Versus Semantic Features. Neuropsychology, 23, 649-657. Clare, I.C.H., & Gudjonsson, G.H. (2010). The Vulnerability of Suspects with Intellectual Disabilities During Police Interviews: A Review and Experimental Study of Decision- Making. Mental Handicap, 8, 110–128. Colorado v. Connelly, 479 U.S. 157 (1986). Fast, D. K., Conry, J., & Loock, C. A. (1999). Identifying fetal alcohol syndrome among youth in the criminal justice system. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 20(5), 370-372. Davies, G., Morton, J., Mollon, P., & Robertson, N. (1998). Recovered memory in theory and practice. , Public Policy, and Law, 4, 1079-1090. Fast, D., & Conry, J. (2006). The Challenge of Fetal Alcohol Syndrome in the Criminal Legal System. Addiction Biology, 9, 161–166. Fotopoulou, A., Conway. M. A., Solms, M., Tyrer, S., & Kopelman, M. (2008). Self-serving confabulation in prose recall. Neuropsychologia 46, 1429–1441. Fotopoulou, A. (2010). The affective neuropsychology of confabulation and . CognitiveNeuropsychiatry, 15, 38-63. Fryer, S.L., Tapert, S.F., Mattson, S.N., Paulus, M.P., Spadoni, A.D., & Riley, E.P. (2007). Prenatal Alcohol Exposure Affects Frontal-Striatal BOLD Response During Inhibitory Control. : Clinical and Experimental Research, 31, 1415–1424. Gallagher, S. (2003). Self-narrative in schizophrenia. The self in and , 336-357. Garrett, B. (2008). Judging Innocence. Columbia Law Review, 1, 55-142. Gibbard, W. B., Wass, P., & Clarke, M. E. (2003). The neuropsychological implications of prenatal alcohol exposure. The Canadian child and adolescent psychiatry review, 12, 72. Goel, V., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Explaining modulation of reasoning by belief. , 87, 11–22. Gorman, W. F. (1982). Defining malingering. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 27(2), 401-407. Gowinski, R., Payman, V., & Frencham, K. (2008). Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry; 42, 932-940. Gudjonsson, G. H. (1992). The psychology of interrogations, confessions, and testimony. Chichester: John Wiley. Gudjonsson, G. H. (2003). The science of interrogations and confessions: A handbook. Chichester, UK: Wiley. Gudjonsson, G. H., Sigurdsson, J. F., Sigfusdottir, I. D., & Young, S. (2012). False confessions to police and their relationship with conduct disorder, ADHD, and life adversity. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 696-701. Guerri, C., Bazinet, A., & Riley, E.P. (2009). Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders and Alterations in Brain and Behavior. Alcohol & Alcoholism, 44, 108–114.

www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5

Horselenberg, R., Merckelbach, H., & Josephs, S. (2003). Individual differences and false confessions: A conceptual replication of Kassin and Kiechel (1996). Psychology, Crime and law, 9, 1-8. Jirikowic, T., Kartin, D., & Olson, H. C. (2008). Children with fetal alcohol spectru disorders: a descriptive profile of adaptive function. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 75, 238-248. Kaba, F., Diamond, P., Haque, A., MacDonald, R., & Venters, H. (2014). Among Newly Admitted Adolescents in the New York City Jail System. Journal of Adolescent Health, 54(5), 615-617. Kassin, S. M., & Kiechel, K. L. (1996). The of false confessions: Compliance, Internalization, and Confabulation. American Psychological Society, 7, 125-128. Kassin, S. M., Bogart, D., & Kerner, J. (2012). Confessions That Corrupt Evidence From the DNA Exoneration Case Files. Psychological science, 23, 41-45. Kern, R. S., Van Gorp, W. G., Cummings, J. L., Brown, W. S., & Osato, S. S. (1992). Confabulation in Alzheimer's disease. Brain and cognition, 19, 172-182. Kessels R., Kortrijk, H., Wester, A., & Nys, G. (2008). Confabulation behavior and false memories in Korsakoff’s syndrome: Role of source memory and executive functioning. Psychiatry and Clinical , 62, 220–225. Kopelman, M. D. (1997). Anomalies of : , confabulation, delusional memory, , and false memories. In J. D. Read & D. S. Lindsay (Eds.), Recollections of trauma: Scientific research and clinical Practice. New York and London: Plenum Press. 273-303. Kopelman, M. D. (1987). Two types of confabulation. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 50, 1482-1487. Kopelman, M.D. (2010). Varieties of confabulation and delusion. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 15, 14-37. Kopelman, M. D. (1999). Varieties of false memory. Cognitive neuropsychology, 16, 197-214. Krabbendam, L., Visser, P. J., Derix, M. M., Verhey, F., Hofman, P., Verhoeven, W., & Jolles, J. (2000). Normal cognitive performance in patients with chronic alcoholism in contrast to patients with Korsakoff's syndrome. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 12, 44-50. LaDue, R. A., Streissguth, A. P., & Randels, S. P. (1992). Clinical considerations pertaining to adolescents and adults with fetal alcohol syndrome. Perinatal Substance Abuse: Research Findings and Clinical Implications. (pp. 104-131). Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press. Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585-589. LoPiccolo, C. J., Goodkin, K., & Baldewicz, T. T. (1999). Current issues in the diagnosis and management of malingering. Annals of Medicine, 31, 166-174. Lorente-Rovira, E., Santos-Gomez, J.L., Moro, M., Villagran, J. M., & McKenna, P. J. (2010). Confabulation in schizophrenia: a neuropsychological study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc.16, 1018-1026 MacPherson, P. and A.E. Chudley. 2007. Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder (FASD): Screening and estimating incidence in an adult correctional population. Presented at the 2nd International Conference on Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: Research, Policy, and Practice Around the World. Victoria, BC. www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5

Mattson, S.N., Gramling, L., & Delis, D. (1996). Global–Local Processing in Children Prenatally Exposed to Alcohol. Child Neuropsychol, 2, 165–175. Mattson, S.N., Crocker, N., Nguyen, T.T. (2011). Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders: Neuropsychological and Behavioral Features. Neuropsychol Rev, 21, 81–101. Morais, J., Frith, C., & Dab, S. (2004). Comprehension, encoding, and monitoring in the production of confabulation in memory: A study with schizophrenic patients. Cognitive neuropsychiatry, 9(3), 153-182. Moscovitch, M. (1989). Confabulation and the frontal systems: Strategic versus associative retrieval in neuropsychological theories of memory. Varieties of memory and : Essays in honour of , 133-160. Nahum, L., Ptak, R., Leemann, B., &Schnider, A. (2009). Disorientation, Confabulation, and Extinction Capacity: Clues On How the Brain Creates Reality. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 966 – 972. Nash, R. A., & Wade, K. A. (2009). Innocent but proven guilty: Eliciting internalized false confessions using doctoredvideo evidence. Applied , 23, 624- 637. Nathaniel-James, D. A., & Frith, C. D. (1996). Confabulation in schizophrenia: Evidence of a new form? Psychological Medicine, 26, 391-400. Nedjam, Z., Devouche, E., &DallaBarba, G. (2004). Confabulation, But Not Executive Dysfunction Discriminate AD From Frontotemporal Dementia. European Journal of Neurology, 11, 728–733. Parwatikar, S. D., Holcomb, W. R., & Menninger, K. A. (1985). The detection of malingered amnesia in accused murderers. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online, 13(1), 97-103. Pei, J. R., Rinaldi, C. M., Rasmussen, C., Massey, V., & Massey, D. (2008). Memory patterns of acquisition and retention of verbal and nonverbal information in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Canadian Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 15(1), e44- e56. Perske, R. (2011). Perske's List: False Confessions From 75 Persons With Intellectual Disability. Intellectual and developmental disabilities, 49(5), 365-373. Pezdek, K., Lam, S.T., & Sperry, K. (2009). Forced Confabulation More Strongly Influences Event Memory If Suggestions Are Other-Generated Than Self-Generated. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 14, 241–252. Rasmussen, C. (2005). Executive functioning and in fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29(8), 1359-1367. Sanders, J. L., & Buck, G. (2010). A long journey: Biological and non-biological parents’ experiences raising children with FASD. Journal of Population Therapeutics and Clinical Pharmacology, 17, 308-322. Schnider, A. (2003). Spontaneous confabulation and the adaptation of to ongoing reality. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 662–671. Schnider, A. (2008). The Confabulating Mind: How the Brain Creates Reality. Oxford: Oxford University. Schnider A.,von Däniken C., & Gutbrod K. (1996). The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations. Brain, A Journal of Neuropsychology, 19, 1365-1375. Schonfeld, A. M., Paley, B., Frankel, F., & O'Connor, M. J. (2006). Executive functioning predicts social skills following prenatal alcohol exposure. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 439-452. Tallberg, I., & Almkvist, O. (2001). Confabulation and memory in patients with Alzheimer's Disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 172-184. www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5

Turner, M., & Coltheart, M. (2010). Confabulation and Delusion: A Common Monitoring Framework. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15, 346–376. Van Oort, R., & Kessels, R. (2009). Executive dysfunction in Korsakoff ’s syndrome: Time to revise the DSM criteria for alcohol-induced persisting amnestic disorder. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 13, 78-81. Weinstein, E. A., Friedland, R. P., & Wagner, E. E. (1994). Denial/unawareness of impairment and symbolic behavior in Alzheimer's disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 7, 176-184. Wells, G. L., & Loftus, E. F. (Eds.). (1984). : Psychological Perspectives. Cambridge University Press. Welsh, M.C., & Pennington, B.F. (1988). Assessing Frontal Lobe Functioning in Children: Views from . Developmental Neuropsychology, 4, 199– 230. Zannino, G.D., Barban, F., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G.A. (2008). Do Confabulators Really Try To Remember When They Confabulate? Cogn Neuropsychol, 25, 831- 852. Zaragosa, M.S., Payment, K.E., Ackil, J.K., Drivdahl, S.B., & Beck, M. (2001). Interviewing witnesses: Forced confabulation and confirmatory feedback increase false memories. American Psychological Society, 12, 473-477.

About the Authors:

Jerrod Brown, MA, MS, is the treatment director for Pathways Counseling Center, Inc. Pathways provides programs and services benefitting individuals impacted by mental illness and addictions. Mr. Brown is also the founder and CEO of the American Institute for the Advancement of Forensic Studies (AIAFS).

Jeffrey Long-McGie, MA, MBA is an intern at the AIAFS, and currently training to become a licensed police officer.

Pamela Oberoi, MA is currently the manager of the refugee mental health program at Pathways Counseling Center.

Judge Anthony (Tony) Wartnik, BA, JD, was a trial judge for 34 years in King County, Washington.

Janina Wresh has 19 years of experience in law enforcement working in forensics crime laboratories, courts, and adult detention centers. She has served as a deputy sheriff, police officer, domestic abuse response specialist, crisis intervention specialist, and crime scene technician. Janina also serves as AIAFS’ Chief Operating Officer.

Erv Weinkauf, MA is a retired 40-year law enforcement veteran with 19 years of teaching experience. He currently serves as chairperson of the Concordia University Criminal Justice Department in St. Paul.

Gennae Falconer, MA, is the Director of Community Engagement for the Greater Minneapolis Council of Churches.

www.jghcs.info (2161-0231 ONLINE) JOURNAL OF LAW ENFORCEMENT, VOLUME 3, NUMBER 5