The Mccollough Effect Reveals Orientation Discrimination in a Case of Cortical Blindness G

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Mccollough Effect Reveals Orientation Discrimination in a Case of Cortical Blindness G View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Elsevier - Publisher Connector The McCollough effect reveals orientation discrimination in a case of cortical blindness G. Keith Humphrey*, Melvyn A. Goodale*, Maurizio Corbettat and Salvatore Aglioti* *Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada N6A 5C2. tDepartment of Neurology, Washington University Medical School, St. Louis, Missouri 63110, USA. Dipartimento Di Scienze Neurologiche E Della Visione, Sezione di Fisiologia Umana, Universita Degli Studi di Verona, Verona 37134, Italy. Background: The McCollough effect is a colour after- demonstrated that the patient, P.B., has a profound effect that is contingent on the orientation of the patterns impairment in form perception and is even unable to used to induce it. To produce the effect, two differently discriminate between 900 differences in the orientation of oriented grating patterns - such as a red-and-black verti- grating stimuli. He is also unable to use orientation infor- cal grating and a green-and-black horizontal grating - mation to control his reaching or grasping. Nevertheless, are viewed alternately for a few minutes. After this period P.B. can name and discriminate different colours reliably, of adaptation, if the black-and-white test gratings are including those used to induce the McCollough effect. viewed in the same orientation as the adaptation patterns, After adaptation with red-and-green gratings, P.B. appro- the white sections of the vertical grating will appear pale priately reported the orientation-contingent aftereffect green and the white sections of the horizontal grating colours, even though he continued to be unable to will appear pink. The McCollough effect indicates that discriminate the orientations of the test patterns. colour- and orientation-coding mechanisms interact at Conclusions: These results indicate that at some level in some point during visual processing; but the question P.B.'s visual system orientation is being coded, but it is at remains as to whether this interaction occurs at an early or a level that he is unable to use in making orientation later stage in the cortical visual pathways. In an attempt to judgements or in visuomotor control. Given the massive answer this question, we studied a patient who had suf- insult to the extrastriate cortex in P.B., it is likely that the fered extensive damage to extrastriate visual areas of the anatomical locus of the mechanisms underlying the brain, which had left him able to see colour but little else. McCollough effect is within primary visual cortex or Results: Neuropsychological and perceptual tests even earlier in the visual pathway. Current Biology 1995, 5:545-551 Background a series of strategic (and answerable) questions for vision researchers, one of which was: 'Does the McCollough Colour aftereffects contingent on pattern orientation effect belong to early vision or to some higher-level were first reported in 1965 by Celeste McCollough [1]. cortical stage?' Several aspects of the McCollough effect To induce a McCollough effect, two differently oriented suggest that it involves an adaptation of mechanisms at grating patterns, such as a red-and-black vertical grating an early stage of visual processing (for reviews see [2,4]). and a green-and-black horizontal grating, are viewed In particular, it has been proposed that area 17 (or V1) alternately for a few minutes (see Fig. 1). After such of the visual cortex has the requisite properties for adaptation, black-and-white versions of the test patterns mediating the McCollough effect (for example, see are presented to subjects. White sections of the vertical [2,9,10]). McCollough [1] originally proposed that the test grating appear to the viewer to be tinted green, effect results from the colour adaptation of neurons in whereas the white sections of the horizontal test grating the visual cortex that code for local orientation - appear pink. If, however, the test patterns are rotated neurons fitting this description have been found in area 450 from the adapted orientations, colour aftereffects are V1. Michael [11] has suggested that the effect reflects not reported. adaptation of striate neurons that code simultaneously for both orientation and colour. It has also been hypoth- Since McCollough's original report, numerous studies esized that the colour-selective cells found in the 'blobs' of this aftereffect have confirmed and extended her ini- of area V1 [12,13] could be the site of the colour com- tial findings (for reviews see [2-4]), and several accounts ponent of this effect. To produce the effect, these of its functional role in vision have been proposed colour-sensitive cells would have to interact with other [2,5-7]. One of the issues that has concerned researchers cells in area V1 that are sensitive to both orientation and is the locus in the visual system of the mechanisms that spatial frequency, but which exhibit a broadband mediate the effect. Indeed, Julesz [8] has recently posed response to wavelength [9]. Correspondence to: G. Keith Humphrey. E-mail address: [email protected] © Current Biology 1995, Vol 5 No 5 545 546 Current Biology 1995, Vol 5 No 5 of objects [15,16]. Her colour perception, however, appears to be quite normal [17], and so we were able to examine her susceptibility to the McCollough effect. D.E was unable to report the orientation of simple grating stimuli, or even report whether one or two pat- terns were present. However, when she was adapted for 10 minutes with a red-and-black horizontal grating alter- nating with a green-and-black vertical grating, she reported the appropriate colour aftereffects when subse- quently presented with the black-and-white test gratings. Moreover, following a second session of adaptation, she reported the appropriate aftereffects some 20 hours later. Throughout all of this testing, she was unable to discrim- inate the orientation of any of the gratings that were successfully inducing colour percepts. In short, D.E showed McCollough effects without any ability to describe, discriminate or recognize the orientation of the inducing and test gratings. Given the pattern of D.E's brain damage, these findings are consistent with the hypothesis that primary visual cortex (or area V1) has the requisite properties for supporting the McCollough effect. It should be pointed out, however, that despite D.F's profound deficits in perception of form and orientation, she is able to orient her hand appropriately in anticipa- tion of grasping those same objects that she is unable to discriminate perceptually [15,16,18]. This suggests that, while D.F cannot make use of visual information about orientation and form for perceptual discrimination, she can use it for the control of skilled actions. Indeed, Goodale and Milner have used these observations as part of a proposal that the visual mechanisms mediating object perception are both functionally and neurally distinct from those underlying the visual control of skilled movements directed at those objects [19,20]. The sparing of form information for the control of skilled actions in D.E also suggests another possible interpretation for her ability to experience an orientation-contingent colour aftereffect. Specifically, because D.E can use orien- tation information to control her grasp and other motor Fig. 1. An illustration of typical induction and testing conditions outputs, it is possible that the orientation coding, beyond for the McCollough effect. Also illustrated is the simulated after- area V1, supporting this behaviour is somehow linked effect, given the induction conditions seen in the figure (see text for details). The colour characteristics of the aftereffect will vary with her intact colour processing. Thus, although it is somewhat as a function of adaptation conditions. In most experi- unlikely, the mediation of the McCollough effect in D.E ments, the inducing stimuli are presented as computer-generated may have occurred in areas beyond V1. What is clearly images or as projected slides. needed is a demonstration that the McCollough effect can occur in an individual with extrastriate damage of a such a Our recently published findings [14] are also consistent nature that any processing of form that may remain in area with the notion that area V1 is the site where the V1 is prevented from reaching those higher cortical areas McCollough effect is mediated. We studied a patient, D.E, that mediate perception and the visual control of action. who had developed a profound visual-form agnosia as a result of carbon monoxide poisoning. Although magnetic We recently had an opportunity to study the McCollough resonance imaging (MRI) revealed a pattern of damage effect in a patient who was essentially cortically blind. consistent with anoxia, most of the damage in the pos- Like D.F, this new patient, P.B., shows a profound terior portion of D.E's brain was evident in areas 18 and insensitivity to visual form, and is unable to recognize the 19, with area 17 (or V1) remaining largely intact. D.F has simplest of geometrical shapes. Patient P.B., however, has great difficulty recognizing even the simplest of geometric none of the spared visuomotor abilities that are seen in forms, and is very poor at discriminating the orientation D.F. Thus, we see no evidence for the processing of McCollough effect and cortical blindness Humphrey et al. RESEARCH PAPER 547 visual form for either perceptual discrimination or visuo- motor control. P.B., like D.F, does show remarkably intact colour perception, so we were able to use his spared colour perception to assess whether he was subject to the McCollough effect. The results obtained provided us with additional support for the view that the inter- action between colour and form that underlies the McCollough effect takes place in area V1 or earlier.
Recommended publications
  • Blindsight in Man and Monkey
    braini0301 Brain (1997), 120, 535–559 INVITED REVIEW Blindsight in man and monkey Petra Stoerig1 and Alan Cowey2 1Institute of Medical Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians- Correspondence to: Petra Stoerig, Institute of Medical University, Munich, Germany and 2Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Goethestraβe Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, Oxford, UK 31, D-80336 Munich, Germany Summary In man and monkey, absolute cortical blindness is caused by degeneration. While extrastriate cortical areas participate in destruction of the optic radiations and/or the primary visual the mediation of the forced-choice responses, a concomitant cortex. It is characterized by an absence of any conscious striate cortical activation does not seem to be necessary for vision, but stimuli presented inside its borders may blindsight. Whether the loss of phenomenal vision is a nevertheless be processed. This unconscious vision includes necessary consequnce of striate cortical destruction and neuroendocrine, reflexive, indirect and forced-choice whether this structure is indispensable for conscious sight responses which are mediated by the visual subsystems are much debated questions which need to be tackled that escape the direct cerebral damage and the ensuing experimentally. Keywords: cortical blindness; blindsight; residual vision; phenomenal vision; visual system Abbreviations: dLGN 5 dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus; OKN 5 optokinetic nystagmus; PGN 5 pregeniculate nucleus; ROC 5 receiver-operating-characteristic curve Introduction ‘A survey of the recent literature indicates that the roˆle of gaining a better understanding of how the visual system the occipital lobes in visually guided behavior in general works and, by exclusion, of getting a hold on the neuronal cannot be properly evaluated as long as investigators are correlate of conscious vision.
    [Show full text]
  • Color Afterimages in Autistic Adults
    Color afterimages in autistic adults Article (Published Version) Maule, John, Stanworth, Kirstie, Pellicano, Elizabeth and Franklin, Anna (2018) Color afterimages in autistic adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48 (4). pp. 1409-1421. ISSN 0162-3257 This version is available from Sussex Research Online: http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/61156/ This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies and may differ from the published version or from the version of record. If you wish to cite this item you are advised to consult the publisher’s version. Please see the URL above for details on accessing the published version. Copyright and reuse: Sussex Research Online is a digital repository of the research output of the University. Copyright and all moral rights to the version of the paper presented here belong to the individual author(s) and/or other copyright owners. To the extent reasonable and practicable, the material made available in SRO has been checked for eligibility before being made available. Copies of full text items generally can be reproduced, displayed or performed and given to third parties in any format or medium for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge, provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. http://sro.sussex.ac.uk J Autism Dev Disord DOI 10.1007/s10803-016-2786-5 S.I. : LOCAL VS. GLOBAL PROCESSING IN AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS Color Afterimages in Autistic Adults 1 1 2 1 John Maule • Kirstie Stanworth • Elizabeth Pellicano • Anna Franklin Ó The Author(s) 2016.
    [Show full text]
  • Color Appearance Models Second Edition
    Color Appearance Models Second Edition Mark D. Fairchild Munsell Color Science Laboratory Rochester Institute of Technology, USA Color Appearance Models Wiley–IS&T Series in Imaging Science and Technology Series Editor: Michael A. Kriss Formerly of the Eastman Kodak Research Laboratories and the University of Rochester The Reproduction of Colour (6th Edition) R. W. G. Hunt Color Appearance Models (2nd Edition) Mark D. Fairchild Published in Association with the Society for Imaging Science and Technology Color Appearance Models Second Edition Mark D. Fairchild Munsell Color Science Laboratory Rochester Institute of Technology, USA Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex PO19 8SQ, England Telephone (+44) 1243 779777 This book was previously publisher by Pearson Education, Inc Email (for orders and customer service enquiries): [email protected] Visit our Home Page on www.wileyeurope.com or www.wiley.com All Rights Reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning or otherwise, except under the terms of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988 or under the terms of a licence issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency Ltd, 90 Tottenham Court Road, London W1T 4LP, UK, without the permission in writing of the Publisher. Requests to the Publisher should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons Ltd, The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex PO19 8SQ, England, or emailed to [email protected], or faxed to (+44) 1243 770571. This publication is designed to offer Authors the opportunity to publish accurate and authoritative information in regard to the subject matter covered.
    [Show full text]
  • The Mccollough Effect: Dissociating Retinal from Spatial Coordinates
    Perception & Psychophysics 1993. 54 (4). 515-526 The McCollough effect: Dissociating retinal from spatial coordinates FELICE L. BEDFORD and KAREN S. REINKE University ofArizona, Tucson, Arizona Three experiments were conducted to dissociate the perceived orientation of a stimulus from its orientation on the retina while inducing the McCollough effect. In the first experiment, the typical contingency between color and retinal orientation was eliminated by having subjects tilt their head 90 0 for half of the induction trials while the stimuli remained the same. The only relation remaining was that between color and the perceived or spatial orientation, which led to only a small contingent aftereffect. In contrast, when the spatial contingency was eliminated in the second experiment, the aftereffect was as large as when both contingencies were present. Finally, a third experiment determined that part of the small spatial effect obtained in the first experiment could be traced to hidden higher order retinal contingencies. The study suggested that even under optimal conditions the McCollough effect is not concerned with real-world prop­ erties of objects or events. Implications for several classes of theories are discussed. The McCollough effect (ME) is viewed by some re­ how many pairs of attributes other than color and oriented searchers to be an instance of learning; the most well de­ lines can lead to contingent aftereffects (see Dodwell & veloped explanation in that class has appealed to Pavlov­ Humphrey, 1990; Harris, 1980; Siegel et al. 1992; ian conditioning (e.g., Allan & Siegel, 1986; Murch, 1976; Skowbo, 1984; Stromeyer, 1978)? Color can be made Siegel, Allan, & Eissenberg, 1992; Westbrook & Harri­ contingent on velocity, direction of motion can be made son, 1984).
    [Show full text]
  • –1– Study Guide for the Final Examination (Wednesday, 4 May
    Psychology of Perception Lewis O. Harvey, Jr.–Instructor Psychology 4165-100 Clare E. Sims–Assistant Spring 2011 09:30–9:45 TuTh Study Guide for the final examination (Wednesday, 4 May 2011, 16:30–19:00). Be able to answer the following questions and be familiar with the concepts involved in the answers. Review your homework and lab assignments and be familiar with the concepts included in them as well. 1. Draw a diagram of the eye including the following structures: cornea, lens, pupil, iris, sclera, aqueous humor, vitreous humor, choroid, retina, fovea, optic disk and optic nerve. 2. Offer an explanation of the Hermann Grid phenomenon based on ganglion cell receptive field characteristics. Why is this explanation insufficient? 3. What happens to contrast sensitivity and visual acuity as illumination goes down? Why is 3.2 Lux, the illumination level at the end of civil twilight, used as a rule-of- thumb reference level for the lower limit of practical visual performance? 4. Discuss the evidence that our color vision is based on three different types of cone receptors. 5. What are the major types of color defective vision and what are their causes? What kind of color experiences might a deuteranope have? Why 6. What is retinal disparity? How much disparity do you need for normal stereoscopic acuity? 7. Describe the “size/distance” (size constancy) hypothesis of certain visual illusions. Pick two such illusions and explain them in terms of this hypothesis. 8. A photograph is reproduced on a flat piece of paper. While you are viewing it, what could you do to enhance the impression of depth in a photograph? What are the basic principles? 9.
    [Show full text]
  • Fatigue and Structural Change: Two Consequences of Visual Pattern Adaptation
    Fatigue and Structural Change: Two Consequences of Visual Pattern Adaptation Jeremy M. Wolfe and Kathleen M. O'Connell In a tilt aftereffect (TAE) paradigm, 2 min of adaptation produced an aftereffect that decayed almost completely within 4 min. Four minutes of adapation produced a TAE that lasted more than 2 wk. Two modes of adaptation contribute to the TAE and account for other aftereffects: (1) short-term fatigue, produced very quickly and (2) long-term structural change, requiring more extended adaptation. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 27:538-543, 1986 Two classes of visual aftereffects can be produced TAE can be measured at least 2 wk later. This extreme by adaptation to a visual stimulus: (1) The threshold persistence probably reflects structural change in the for detecting that stimulus and similar stimuli may be visual nervous system. raised,1 and (2) The suprathreshold appearance of re- lated stimuli may be altered (eg, orientation, motion, Materials and Methods 2 5 spatial frequency, and color). " Threshold elevation The basic TAE paradigm is shown in Figure 1. If aftereffects are explained as temporary reductions in the the reader fixates Figure la and then shifts gaze to lb, sensitivity of the mechanisms detecting the stimulus. it will be seen that lb comes to look like lc. These Adaptation fatigues the mechanism, and some period stimuli and the data presented below demonstrate that of time is required for recovery. Fatigue has been the TAE develops rapidly. With just these stimuli, proposed as an explanation of aftereffects of the second however, the reader will have more difficulty producing kind.5"7 However, several of these, notably the Mc- 5 8 the week-long TAEs that are the main topic of this Collough effect and the motion aftereffect can last paper.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology of Perception Lewis O. Harvey, Jr.–Instructor Psychology 4165-100 Laramie E
    Psychology of Perception Lewis O. Harvey, Jr.–Instructor Psychology 4165-100 Laramie E. Duncan–Assistant Fall 2006 MUEN D156, 09:30–10:45 T&R Study Guide for the first examination (Tuesday, 17 October 2006). Be able to answer the following questions and be familiar with the concepts involved in the answers. 1. Describe the classical psychophysical methods of Fechner: the method of adjustment, the method of limits and method of constant stimuli. 2. Using your weight judgment experiment as an example, draw a psychometric function relating probability of making a “heavier” judgment to the weight of the test stimulus. Label both axes. Explain how the just noticeable difference (JND) is defined. Illustrate the JND on your graph of the psychometric function. 3. From the basic detection paradigm, define hit rate and false alarm rate. Describe the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) predicted by the High Threshold Model and the ROC predicted by the Signal Detection Theory of detection. How do you compute sensitivity (d’) from the hit rate and the false alarm rate for the equal-variance dual-Gaussian signal detection model? (Memorize the formula). 4. Be able to identify the following parts of the eye: cornea, lens, pupil, iris, sclera, aqueous humor, vitreous humor, choroid, retina, optic disk and optic nerve. 5. Define the term “receptive field.” Describe the receptive fields of retinal ganglion cells. How do ganglion cell receptive fields differ from those of cells in the primary visual cortex? 6. Describe and illustrate an explanation of the Hermann Grid phenomenon based on ganglion cell receptive field characteristics.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Psychophysics and Sensitivity Regulation
    Visual psychophysics, sensitivity regulation and adaptation Moonlight Sunlight Typical ambient light levels Starlight Indoor lighting Photopic retinal illuminance -4.3 -2.4 -0.5 1.1 2.7 4.5 6.5 8.5 (log phot td) Scotopic retinal illuminance -3.9 -2.0 -0.1 1.5 3.1 4.9 6.9 8.9 (log scot td) Visual function Absolute rod Cone Rod saturation Damage threshold threshold begins possible NEUR 3045 Visual Neuroscience Andrew Stockman Psychophysical measures of adaptation mechanisms What is visual psychophysics? Psychophysicists study human vision by measuring an observer’s performance on carefully chosen perceptual tasks. By manipulating the properties of simple visual stimuli, and measuring the effect of that manipulation on an observer’s performance we can infer the way the visual system processes particular properties of a stimulus. INPUT PROCESSING OUTPUT How do we process visual ? information? STIMULUS VISUAL PERCEPTION SYSTEM What is visual psychophysics? In psychophysics, we try to infer what is going on inside the visual system just from studying the input and output. So, to the psychophysicist the visual system is much like a “BLACK BOX”. We can learn a surprising amount about the black box and how it works from psychophysical measurements. INPUT PROCESSING OUTPUT How do we process visual ? ? information? STIMULUS VISUAL PERCEPTION SYSTEM Psychophysical tasks that can provide useful information: DETECTION TASKS Is a circle present? Is the circle flickering Is the pattern (grating) or steady? visible? In general, simple tasks are used. Can you think of other tasks? Which other tasks provide useful information? DISCRIMINATION OR MATCHING REACTION TIME Are the two halves the same or different? Respond to the appearance of a circle as quickly as you can.
    [Show full text]
  • A Conditioning Model for the Mccollough Effect
    Portland State University PDXScholar Dissertations and Theses Dissertations and Theses 1975 A Conditioning Model for the McCollough Effect Andreas D. Lord Portland State University Follow this and additional works at: https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds Part of the Psychology Commons Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Lord, Andreas D., "A Conditioning Model for the McCollough Effect" (1975). Dissertations and Theses. Paper 2419. https://doi.org/10.15760/etd.2416 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations and Theses by an authorized administrator of PDXScholar. For more information, please contact [email protected]. AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF Andreas D. Lord for the Master of Science in Psychology presented July 31, 1975 Title: A Conditioning M~del ror ~he McCollough Effect APPROVED BY MEMBERS OF THE THESIS COMMITTEE: Gerald M. Murch, Chairman James Paulson A model based on the laws of classical conditioning is posed as an explanation for the McCollough Effect, an orientation-specific color aftereffect. This model stands as an alternative to the color-coded edge detector hypothesis. Background and relevant issues are presented, Two experi- ments were performed, The first demonstrated that an auditory stimulus causes the effect to appear stronger to some subjects, a disinhioiting effect, It was also shown that some subjects experience spontaneous recovery of the effect after it has bee~ extinguished, •\' is ; The second experiment demonstrated that the after­ colors will generalize to lines of varying orientation,. including ~5°. Subjects adapted to both red-vertical and green~horizontal lines ~aw mostly pink on test lines more than~30° off horizontal.
    [Show full text]
  • A Review of Visual Aftereffects in Schizophrenia T ⁎ Katharine N
    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 101 (2019) 68–77 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev Review article A review of visual aftereffects in schizophrenia T ⁎ Katharine N. Thakkara,b, , Steven M. Silversteinc, Jan W. Brascampa a Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, United States b Division of Psychiatry and Behavioral Medicine, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, United States c Departments of Psychiatry and Ophthalmology, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, United States ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Psychosis—a cardinal symptom of schizophrenia—has been associated with a failure to appropriately create or Schizophrenia use stored regularities about past states of the world to guide the interpretation of incoming information, which Vision leads to abnormal perceptions and beliefs. The visual system provides a test bed for investigating the role of prior Aftereffect experience and prediction, as accumulated knowledge of the world informs our current perception. More spe- Adaptation cifically, the strength of visual aftereffects, illusory percepts that arise after prolonged viewing ofavisualsti- Predictive coding mulus, can serve as a valuable measure of the influence of prior experience on current visual processing. Inthis Excitation/inhibition balance Plasticity paper, we review findings from a largely older body of work on visual aftereffects in schizophrenia, attemptto reconcile discrepant findings, highlight the role of antipsychotic medication, consider mechanistic interpreta- tions for behavioral effects, and propose directions for future research. 1. Background system for understanding the role of experience in how individuals perceive and interpret the world and holds a clear translational value in Understanding the computational and neural mechanisms that understanding abnormalities in psychosis.
    [Show full text]
  • Configural and Perceptual Factors Influencing the Perception of Color Transparency
    CONFIGURAL AND PERCEPTUAL FACTORS INFLUENCING THE PERCEPTION OF COLOR TRANSPARENCY THÈSE NO 3181 (2005) PRÉSENTÉE À LA FACULTÉ INFORMATIQUE ET COMMUNICATIONS Institut de systèmes de communication SECTION DES SYSTÈMES DE COMMUNICATION ÉCOLE POLYTECHNIQUE FÉDÉRALE DE LAUSANNE POUR L'OBTENTION DU GRADE DE DOCTEUR ÈS SCIENCES PAR Peggy GERARDIN licence de sciences cognitives, Université Lumière, Lyon 2, France et de nationalité française acceptée sur proposition du jury: Prof. S. Süsstrunk, directrice de thèse Prof. M. D'Zmura, rapporteur Prof. M. Herzog, rapporteur Dr K. Knoblauch, rapporteur Lausanne, EPFL 2005 Peggy GERARDIN THÈSE NO 3181 (2005) Acknowledgments Many people supported me during the completion of this thesis with criticism, helpful assistance and refer- ences. This work would have never been possible without them. First of all, I would like to express my gratitude to my supervisor Prof. Sabine Süsstrunk who gave me the opportunity to join her group, the best research environment I ever had. I would like to thank her for her guidance, encouragement and excellent advice throughout this study. I owe a special thank to Dr. Kenneth Knoblauch (INSERM U371, Lyon, France), whose patience, assistance and motivation were greatly appreciated. I stopped counting the numerous emails we sent to each other, the abundant lunches we shared and the million discussions we had on color vision. I would like to give a special thank to Prof. Michael D’Zmura, who welcomed me in his laboratory at the University of California, Irvine (USA). He proposed me a very interesting subject and helped me during all my stay. I thank him for the nice barbecues we had together and private concerts at home.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Adaptation
    Swarthmore College Works Psychology Faculty Works Psychology 2000 Visual Adaptation Frank H. Durgin Swarthmore College, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://works.swarthmore.edu/fac-psychology Part of the Psychology Commons Let us know how access to these works benefits ouy Recommended Citation Frank H. Durgin. (2000). "Visual Adaptation". Encyclopedia Of Psychology. Volume 8, 183-187. https://works.swarthmore.edu/fac-psychology/218 This work is brought to you for free by Swarthmore College Libraries' Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology Faculty Works by an authorized administrator of Works. For more information, please contact [email protected]. VISUAL ADAPTATION 183 Visual Memory points throughout the life span. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 10, 1509-1516. Vision in complex environments requires observers not only to detect and discriminate stimuli presented si­ Robert Sekuler and Allison B. Sekuler multaneously, but also to integrate information appro­ priately over space and time. One index of this ability is short-term memory for visual attributes, such as spa­ tial frequency, that resist semantic encoding. Although VISUAL ADAPTATION refers to reversible changes in older observers typically perform poorly on tasks that perception caused by visual experience. For example, involve verbal memory, this age-related loss seems to after staring at the downward motion of a waterfall, a spare memory for visual attributes. Nevertheless, brain vivid sensation of upward motion will be perceived imaging (positron emission tomography) studies reveal when looking at a stationary object. Adaptation serves that different neural systems are responsible for spatial a functional role in the visual system by calibrating it frequency memory in younger and older observers (Mc­ to prevailing conditions and thus increasing the effi­ Intosh et al., 1997).
    [Show full text]