Implicit Bias Articles

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Implicit Bias Articles Implicit Bias Articles Office of the Vice President for Intercultural Affairs Helping Loyola Marymount University move toward Inclusive Excellence Office of the Vice President for Intercultural Affairs Loyola Marymount University University Hall, Suite 4826 (310) 338-7598 [email protected] Abdollah, T. (2015). Police agencies line up to learn about unconscious bias. Police One. Retrieved from https://www.policeone.com/patrol-issues/articles/8415353- Police-agencies-line-up-to-learn-about-unconscious-%20bias/ Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Altemeyer, R. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba Press. American Bar Association Section of Litigation, Implicit Bias Task Force. (2012). Toolbox Power- point Instruction Manual. Retrieved from https://www.americanbar.org/content/dam/aba/administrative/litigation/implicit- bias/Facilitator-Instruction-Manual.authcheckdam.pdf. Anderson, A. J., et al. (2015). The effectiveness of three strategies to reduce the influence of bias in evaluations of female leaders. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 45(9), 522-39. Andrew, R. T., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Galinsky, A. D. (2011). Perspective taking combats the denial of intergroup discrimination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 738- 745. Retrieved from https://ac.els-cdn.com/S0022103111003015/1-s2.0-S0022103111003015- main.pdf?_tid=92eacc0d-055e-42ac-bd55- e9471d2acfc9&acdnat=1530035777_072e31874f6c1b803e87cff9f24f408e. Ansell, D. A., & McDonald, E. K. (2015). Bias, black lives, and academic medicine. The New England Journal of Medicine, 372(12), 1085-1087. Retrieved from https://www.nejm.org/doi/10.1056/NEJMp1500832. Arellano-Jackson, J. (2015). But what can we do? How juvenile defenders can disrupt the school-to-prison pipeline. Seattle Journal for Social Justice, 13(3), 751–797. Aronson, J., & McGlone, M. S. (2009). Stereotype and social identity threat. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 153-178). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press. Azevedo, R. T., Macaluso, E., Avenanti, A., Santangelo,V., Cazzato, V., & Aglioti, S. M. (2013). Their pain is not our pain: Brain and automatic correlates of empathic resonance with the pain of same and different race individuals. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 3168–3181. Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2013). Blind spot: Hidden biases of good people. New York, NY: Bantam Books. Banaji, M. R., Hardin, C., & Rothman, A. J. (1993). Implicit stereotyping in person judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 272–281. 1 | Page Office of the Vice President for Intercultural Affairs 10/18/2018 Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 230–244. Batson, C. D., Early, S., & Salvarani, G. (1997). Perspective taking: Imagining how another feels versus imagining how you would feel. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(7), 751-758. doi: 10.1177/0146167297237008. Beattie, G., Coehn, D., & McGuire, L. (2013). An exploration of possible unconscious ethnic biases in higher education: The role of implicit attitudes on selection for university posts. Semiotica, 197, 171-201. Benforado, A., & Hanson, J. (2008). The great attributional divide: How divergent views of human behavior are shaping legal policy. Emory Law Journal, 57(2), 311–408. Bennett, M. W. (2010). Unraveling the Gordian knot of implicit bias in jury selection: The problems of judge-dominated voir dire, the failed promise of Batson, and proposed solutions. Harvard Law and Policy Review, 4(1), 149–171. Berthold, A., Leight, C., Methner, N., & Gaum, P., (2013). Seeing the world with the eyes of the outgroup: The impact of perspective taking on the prototypicality of the ingroup relative to the outgroup. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 1034-1041. Bertrand, M., Chugh, D., & Mullainathan, S. (2005). Implicit discrimination. The American Economic Review, 95(2), 94–98. Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. The American Economic Review, 94(4), 991-1013. Retrieved from http://www.nber.org/papers/w9873.pdf Betancourt, J. R. (2004). Not me!: Doctors, decisions, and disparities in health care. Cardiovascular Reviews and Reports, 25(3), 105–109. Blair, I. V., Havranek, E. P., Price, D. W., Hanratty, R., Fairclough, D. L., Farley, T., Hirsh, H. K. & Steiner, J. F. (2013). Assessment of biases against Latinos and African Americans among primary care providers and community members. American Journal of Public Health, 103(1), 92-98. Blair, I. V., Judd, C. M., & Chapleau, K. M. (2004). The influence of Afrocentric facial features in criminal sentencing. Psychological Science, 15(10), 674-679. Retrieved from http://journals.Sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00739.x . 2 | Page Office of the Vice President for Intercultural Affairs 10/18/2018 Blair, I. V., Steiner, J. F., Fairclough, D. L., Hanratty, R., Price, D. W., Hirsh, H. K., Wright, L. A., Bronsert, M., Karimkhani, E., Magid, D. J., & Havranek, E. P, (2013). Clinicians’ implicit ethnic/racial bias and perceptions of care among Black and Latino patients. The Annals of Family Medicine, 11(1), 43-52. Boscardin, C. K. (2015). Reducing implicit bias through curricular interventions. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 30(12), 1726–1728. Boser, U., Wilhelm, M., & Hanna, R. (2014). The power of the pygmalion effect: Expectations have a deep influence on student performance. Retrieved from https://www.americanprogress.org/issues/education/ report/2014/10/06/96806/the-power- of-the-pygmalion-effect/. Boysen, G. A., & Vogel, D. L. (2009). Bias in the classroom: Types, frequencies, and responses. Teaching Of Psychology 36(1), 12-17. Brayer, P.C. (2015). Hidden racial bias: Why we need to talk with jurors about Ferguson. Northwestern University Law Review, 109, 163-170. Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. Srull, T. K., Wyer Jr., R. S. (Eds.), A dual process model of impression formation. (pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Burgess, D. J. (2010). Are providers more likely to contribute to healthcare disparities under high levels of cognitive load? How features of the healthcare setting may lead to biases in medical decision-making. Medical Decision Making, 30(2), 246–257. Burgess, D. J., Beach, M. C., & Saha, S. (2017). Mindfulness practice: A promising approach to reducing the effects of clinician implicit bias on patients. Patient Education and Counseling, 100(2), 372-376. Burgess, D. J., Phelan, S., Workman, M., Hagel, E. ,Nelson, D., Fu, S. S., & Ryn, M. V. (2014). The effect of cognitive load and patient race on physician’s decisions to prescribe opiods for chronic low back pain: A randomized trial. Pain Medicine, 15, 965–974. Burgess, D. J., van Ryn, M., Dovidio, J., & Saha, S. (2007). Reducing racial bias among health care providers: Lessons from social-cognitive psychology. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 22(6), 882–887. Byrne, A., & Tanesini, A. (2015). Instilling new habits: Addressing implicit bias in healthcare professionals. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 20(5), 1255- 1262. 3 | Page Office of the Vice President for Intercultural Affairs 10/18/2018 Capatosto, K. (2015). Strategies for addressing implicit bias in early childhood education. The Kirwan Institute for the Study of Race and Ethnicity. Retrieved from http://kir-waninstitute.osu.edu/wp-content/uploads/2015/06/ implicit-bias-strategies.pdf. Carlson, J. M., & Iovini, J. (1985). The transmission of racial attitudes from fathers to sons: A study of blacks and whites. Adolescence, 20, 233–237. Carnes, M., Devine, P. G., Isaac, C., Manwell, L. B., Ford, C. E., Byars-Winston, A., Fine, E., Sheridan, J. T. (2012). Promoting institutional change through bias literacy. Journal of Diversity in Higher Education, 5(2), 63-77. Carter, P., Skiba, R., Arrendondo, M., & Pollock, M. (2014). You can’t fix what you don’t look at: Acknowledging race in addressing racial discipline disparities. In The Discipline Dispartities Research to Practice Collaborative (Ed.), Acknowledging Race Series. Casey, P. M., Warren, R. K., Cheesman, F. L., & Elek, J. K. (2013). Addressing implicit bias in the courts. Court Review, 49, 64–70. Caspi, A. 1984. Contact hypothesis and inter-age attitudes: A field study of cross-age contact. Social Psychology, 47, 74–80. Chapman, E. N., Kaatz, A., & Carnes, M. (2013). Physicians and implicit bias: How doctors may unwittingly perpetuate health care disparities. Journal of General Internal Medicine, (28)11, 1504-1510. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3797360/. Chandran, A. (2015). Color in the ‘Black Box’: Addressing racism in juror deliberations. Columbia Journal of Race and Law, 5(1), 28–52. Cheryan, S., Plaut, V. C., Davies, P. G., & Steele, C. M. (2009). Ambient belonging: How stereotypical cues impact gender participation in computer science. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 97(6), 1045–1060. Chokshi, N. (2018, May 5). Native American brothers pulled from campus tour after nervous parent calls police. The New York Times. Retrieved from http://nytimes.com. Clark, P., & Zygmunt, E. (2014). A close encounter with personal bias: Pedagogical implications for teacher education. The Journal of Negro Education,
Recommended publications
  • Critical Multiculturalism and Intersectionality in a Complex World
    Critical Multiculturalism and Intersectionality in a Complex World Critical Multiculturalism and Intersectionality in a Complex World SECOND EDITION Lacey M. Sloan Mildred C. Joyner Catherine J. Stakeman Cathryne L. Schmitz 1 1 Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide. Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and certain other countries. Published in the United States of America by Oxford University Press 198 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10016, United States of America. © Oxford University Press 2018 First Edition published in 2008 Second Edition published in 2018 All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press, or as expressly permitted by law, by license, or under terms agreed with the appropriate reproduction rights organization. Inquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope of the above should be sent to the Rights Department, Oxford University Press, at the address above. You must not circulate this work in any other form and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer. Library of Congress Cataloging- in- Publication Data Names: Sloan, Lacey M., author. Title: Critical multiculturalism and intersectionality in a complex world / Lacey M. Sloan [and three others]. Other titles: Critical multicultural social work. Description: Second edition. | New York, NY : Oxford University Press, [2018]| Earlier edition published in 2008 as: Critical multicultural social work.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the Psychology of Implicit Intergroup Bias and the Prospect Of
    1 The Psychology of Implicit Intergroup Bias and the Prospect of Change Calvin K. Lai1 & Mahzarin R. Banaji2 1 Washington University in St. Louis 2 Harvard University Last updated: June 24, 2020 Correspondence should be addressed to: Calvin Lai at [email protected]. Paper should be cited as: Lai, C. K., & Banaji, M. R. (2020). The psychology of implicit intergroup bias and the prospect of change. In D. Allen & R. Somanathan (Eds.), Difference without Domination: Pursuing Justice in Diverse Democracies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. 2 1. Introduction Over the course of evolution, human minds acquired the breathtaking quality of consciousness which gave our species the capacity to regulate behavior. Among the consequences of this capacity was the possibility of internal dialogue with oneself about the consistency between one’s intentions and actions. This facility to engage in the daily rituals of deliberative thought and action is so natural to our species that we hardly reflect on it or take stock of how effectively we are achieving the goal of intention-action consistency. We do not routinely ask at the end of each day how many of our actions were consistent with the values so many individuals hold: a belief in freedom and equality for all, in opportunity and access for all, in fairness in treatment and justice for all. Even if we wished to compute the extent to which we succeed at this task, how would we go about doing it? As William James (1904) pointed over a century ago, the difficulty of studying the human mind is that the knower is also the known, and this poses difficulties in accessing, in modestly objective fashion, the data from our own moral ledger.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Bias in Juvenile Justice That Would Be Most Useful for Those Who Are Interested in the Issue Are Outlined in the Following Sections
    θωερτψυιοπασδφγηϕκλζξχϖβνµθωερτψ υιοπασδφγηϕκλζξχϖβνµθωερτψυιοπασδ Implicit Bias and Juvenile Justice: φγηϕκλζξχϖβνµθωερτψυιοπασδφγηϕκλζ A Review of the Literature ξχϖβνµθωερτψυιοπασδφγηϕκλζξχϖβνµ θωερτψυιοπασδφγηϕκλζξχϖβνµθωερτψ Prepared by Zenobia Bell and Ana Rasquiza υιοπασδφγηϕκτψυιοπασδφγηϕκλζξχϖβνon behalf of the National Center for Youth Law Edited by Michael Harris and Hannah Benton January 2014 µθωερτψυιοπασδφγηϕκλζξχϖβνµθωερτ ψυιοπασδφγηϕκλζξχϖβνµθωερτψυιοπα σδφγηϕκλζξχϖβνµθωερτψυιοπασδφγηϕκ λζξχϖβνµθωερτψυιοπασδφγηϕκλζξχϖβ νµθωερτψυιοπασδφγηϕκλζξχϖβνµθωερτ Implicit Bias and Juvenile Justice 2 Table of Contents EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 3 1) RACIAL DISPARITIES IN JUVENILE JUSTICE? 4 2) IMPLICIT BIAS A CAUSE OF RACIAL DISPARITIES IN JUVENILE JUSTICE? 6 3) IMPLICIT BIAS AND THE LAW 12 4) IMPLICIT BIAS AND JUVENILE JUSTICE DECISION-MAKING 14 4.A) JUDGES 15 4.B) LAW ENFORCEMENT OFFICERS 18 4.C) INDIGENT JUVENILE DEFENDERS 21 4.D) PROSECUTORS 24 4.E) PROBATION OFFICERS 25 5) DE-BIASING 27 6) FUTURE RESEARCH AND CONCLUSION 32 WORKS CITED 36 APPENDIX 40 KEY SEARCH TERMS USED 40 Implicit Bias and Juvenile Justice 3 Executive Summary It has long been established that there are racial disparities in the juvenile justice system. Indeed the U.S. Congress created the Disproportionate Minority Contact (DMC) mandate requiring states to address racial disproportionality in the juvenile justice system. However, scholars have not come to a consensus about the causes of these racial disparities within the juvenile justice system. Some scholars argue that disparities in the system are caused by differences in crime rates by race. Other scholars cite overt racism, institutional racism, and bias within other systems as causal factors. Increasingly, leaders in the field are looking at the “implicit biases” or “unconscious attitudes” of decision makers in the juvenile justice system as a possible cause for these disparities.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the Psychology of Implicit Intergroup Bias
    1 The Psychology of Implicit Intergroup Bias and the Prospect of Change Calvin K. Lai & Mahzarin R. Banaji Harvard University NOTE: This is a penultimate draft. The paper is currently awaiting editorial corrections. Correspondence should be addressed to: Calvin Lai at [email protected] 2 Over the course of evolution, human minds acquired the breathtaking quality of consciousness which gave our species the capacity to regulate our behavior to be consistent with intentions and goals. This facility to engage in the daily rituals of deliberative thought and action is so natural to us that we hardly reflect on it. We do not routinely ask at the end of each day how many of our actions were consistent with all the values we hold: a belief in freedom and liberty for all, in opportunity and access for all, in fairness in treatment and justice for all. Even if we wished to compute the extent to which we succeed at this task, how would we go about doing it? As William James (1904) pointed over a century ago, the difficulty of studying the human mind is that the knower is also the known, and this poses difficulties in accessing, in modestly objective fashion, the data from our own moral ledger. Ask people and you will learn that people believe themselves to be good moral actors who may not be perfect but are largely behaving well (Aquino & Reed, 2002). This is especially likely when we probe people’s attitudes and beliefs about social groups (Banaji & Greenwald, 2013). And yet, as the data from the social and behavioral sciences repeatedly show, significant differences in treatment as a function of one’s social group membership remain.
    [Show full text]
  • View / Open Benforado.Pdf
    OREGON 2010 VOLUME 89 LAW NUMBER 1 REVIEW Articles ADAM BENFORADO* Quick on the Draw: Implicit Bias and the Second Amendment Abstract ................................................................................................ 2 Introduction .......................................................................................... 3 A. Racism, Guns, and Fear...................................................... 3 B. Another Connection Story .................................................. 8 I. Racism and Gun Rights ........................................................... 11 A. A Changing Landscape .................................................... 11 1. Guns ........................................................................... 12 a. A New View of the Second Amendment .............. 12 b. A Thriving Gun Culture ........................................ 14 * Assistant Professor of Law, Drexel University Earle Mack School of Law. I would like to extend my gratitude to William M. Carter, Jr., Richard Frankel, Alex Geisinger, L. Song Richardson, and participants at the Faculty Colloquium at Temple University Beasley School of Law for helpful comments during the formulation of this Article. I would also like to thank Stephen Lund, Gene Markin, and Jeffrey Katz for their research assistance, particularly in formulating the Appendix. [1] 2 OREGON LAW REVIEW [Vol. 89, 1 c. More Guns, More Gun Ownership, More Gun Permits .................................................................. 18 d. Gun Use: Private Enforcers? ................................
    [Show full text]
  • Der Rassismus Im Eigenen Denken
    Der Rassismus im eigenen Denken Christoph Bublitz 2020-06-15T16:58:56 Die Psychologie legt eine unbequeme Wahrheit nahe: Der Großteil der Menschen trägt – ohne sich dessen notwendigerweise bewusst zu sein – Vorurteile aller Art mit sich herum. Dieser Umstand lässt aktuelle Debatten um latenten Rassismus in Polizei und Justiz empirisch uninformiert erscheinen und sollte einen selbstkritischen Blick auch auf das eigene Denken motivieren. Dieser Blick ist individuell wie institutionell unangenehm, weil er auf etwas hinweist, was moralisch nicht sein soll und verfassungsrechtlich nicht sein darf: Die ungleiche stereotypische Beurteilung und Behandlung von Personen aufgrund von Gruppenzugehörigkeiten und -zuschreibungen. Das menschliche Denken ist anfällig für allerlei solche Verzerrungen, Biases. Bezüglich von Gruppen finden sich Biases u.a. für gender, Alter, Attraktivität, sexuelle Orientierung, sowie der hier interessierende racial bias (der in der psychologischen Forschung v.a. auf Hautfarbe und Ethnizität bezogen wird). Solche Biases wirken oft unbewusst und auch in wohlmeinenden, liberalen Geistern. Die ihnen zugrundeliegenden Mechanismen zählen zur Architektur der menschlichen Kognition, ihre Inhalte sind erlernt, und sie können Denkmuster und Verhaltensweisen auch von denjenigen prägen, die stereotypische Beurteilungen explizit ablehnen. Das reflexhafte Zurückweisen von Vorwürfen gruppenbezogener diskriminierender Behandlung läuft der ehrlichen Analyse des Problems und der Suche nach Abhilfe entgegen. Die Verringerung des Einflusses
    [Show full text]
  • Trial by Tabloid: Can One-Time Education Reduce Pretrial
    TRIAL BY TABLOID: CAN ONE-TIME EDUCATION REDUCE PRETRIAL PUBLICITY BIAS? by Courtney Nicole Meyers HONORS THESIS Submitted to Texas State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for graduation in the Honors College May 2020 Thesis Supervisor: Angela M. Jones Second Reader: Kimberly Wong ABSTRACT Negative-defendant pretrial publicity (PTP) is prevalent and can threaten a defendant’s right to a fair trial. Extensive research supports the contention that negative defendant PTP can bias juror decision-making by rendering a juror incapable of determining a verdict based solely on trial evidence (Steblay et al., 1999; Ruva et al., 2007). Voir dire is used to assess potential jurors’ knowledge of the publicity surrounding the case and potential bias against the defendant. In high profile cases it might be difficult to find jurors who have not seen or heard anything about the case. A variety of safeguards have been proposed to address the negative effects of PTP (e.g., voir dire, deliberation), though with minimal impact. The state of Washington recently developed an educational video on unconscious bias. This video is shown to jurors just prior to voir dire to reduce bias and increase the fairness of jurors’ decisions. Yet, little is known regarding the effectiveness of this proposed remedy. The current study tested whether this video reduces PTP-related biases. A total of 251 students participated in the 2 (PTP: negative PTP vs. unrelated) x 2 (One-time education: debiasing training video vs. none) between- subjects factorial design. When participants received PTP, they were more likely to convict, found the defendant more culpable and less credible, and sentenced more harshly.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Social Cognition and Law
    Implicit Social Cognition and Law Kristin A. Lane, 1,2 Jerry Kang,3 and Mahzarin R. Banaji2 1Department ofPsychology, Bard College, Annandale-on-Hudson, New York 12504; email: [email protected] 2Department ofPsychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138; email: [email protected] JSchool ofLaw, University ofCalifornia, Los Angeles, California 90095; email: [email protected] [TJo be as intelligent as we can is a moral when the data reveal unappealing reflections obligation-that intelligence is one of the ofhuman behavior, including our own. talents for the use ofwhich we shall be called to account-that if we haven't exhausted ev­ ery opportunity to know whether what we THE SCIENCE are doing is right, it will be no excuse for us Imagine sitting at a computer. Your job ap­ to say that we meant well. pears simple: As words such as happy and an­ gry appear sequentially on the screen, indicate John Erskine, American Character whether each is good (happy is good) or bad and Other Essays, 1915 (angry is bad) by pressing marked keys on a In the early years of the twentieth century, keyboard. But more than the words appear on John Erskine, American educator and author, the screen. In fact, each word to be judged as worried about a lack of faith in intelligence good or bad is preceded by a black or white as a virtue, and contrasted it specifically with face (i.e., individuals with origins in Africa or the elevated status accorded to goodness as Europe) that you see but do not respond to.
    [Show full text]
  • An Examination of the Role of Automatic Bias and Controlled Processes
    GENDER AND SHOOTING BEHAVIOUR The Influence of Target Gender on Shooting Behaviour: An Examination of the Role of Automatic Bias and Controlled Processes by Evanya Musolino A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate and Post Doctoral Affairs in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Psychology Carleton University Ottawa, Ontario ©2012 Evanya Musolino Library and Archives Bibliotheque et Canada Archives Canada Published Heritage Direction du Branch Patrimoine de I'edition 395 Wellington Street 395, rue Wellington Ottawa ON K1A0N4 Ottawa ON K1A 0N4 Canada Canada Your file Votre reference ISBN: 978-0-494-91565-3 Our file Notre reference ISBN: 978-0-494-91565-3 NOTICE: AVIS: The author has granted a non­ L'auteur a accorde une licence non exclusive exclusive license allowing Library and permettant a la Bibliotheque et Archives Archives Canada to reproduce, Canada de reproduire, publier, archiver, publish, archive, preserve, conserve, sauvegarder, conserver, transmettre au public communicate to the public by par telecommunication ou par I'lnternet, preter, telecommunication or on the Internet, distribuer et vendre des theses partout dans le loan, distrbute and sell theses monde, a des fins commerciales ou autres, sur worldwide, for commercial or non­ support microforme, papier, electronique et/ou commercial purposes, in microform, autres formats. paper, electronic and/or any other formats. The author retains copyright L'auteur conserve la propriete du droit d'auteur ownership and moral rights in this et des droits moraux qui protege cette these. Ni thesis. Neither the thesis nor la these ni des extraits substantiels de celle-ci substantial extracts from it may be ne doivent etre imprimes ou autrement printed or otherwise reproduced reproduits sans son autorisation.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Multiple Racial Stereotypes in Simulated Shooting Decisions by Sarah Harmer a Thesis Submitted to the Faculty Of
    The Role of Multiple Racial Stereotypes in Simulated Shooting Decisions by Sarah Harmer A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Graduate and Post Doctoral Affairs in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Psychology Carleton University Ottawa, ON ©2012 Sarah Harmer Library and Archives Bibliotheque et Canada Archives Canada Published Heritage Direction du 1+1Branch Patrimoine de I'edition 395 Wellington Street 395, rue Wellington Ottawa ON K1A0N4 Ottawa ON K1A 0N4 Canada Canada Your file Votre reference ISBN: 978-0-494-94335-9 Our file Notre reference ISBN: 978-0-494-94335-9 NOTICE: AVIS: The author has granted a non­ L'auteur a accorde une licence non exclusive exclusive license allowing Library and permettant a la Bibliotheque et Archives Archives Canada to reproduce, Canada de reproduire, publier, archiver, publish, archive, preserve, conserve, sauvegarder, conserver, transmettre au public communicate to the public by par telecommunication ou par I'lnternet, preter, telecommunication or on the Internet, distribuer et vendre des theses partout dans le loan, distrbute and sell theses monde, a des fins commerciales ou autres, sur worldwide, for commercial or non­ support microforme, papier, electronique et/ou commercial purposes, in microform, autres formats. paper, electronic and/or any other formats. The author retains copyright L'auteur conserve la propriete du droit d'auteur ownership and moral rights in this et des droits moraux qui protege cette these. Ni thesis. Neither the thesis nor la these ni des extraits substantiels de celle-ci substantial extracts from it may be ne doivent etre imprimes ou autrement printed or otherwise reproduced reproduits sans son autorisation.
    [Show full text]
  • A Psychological Perspective on Police Brutality: Current Statistics, Characteristics, and Trends Regarding Excessive Use of Force
    National Louis University Digital Commons@NLU Dissertations 6-2021 A Psychological Perspective on Police Brutality: Current Statistics, Characteristics, and Trends Regarding Excessive Use of Force Ashley Salter National Louis University Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.nl.edu/diss Part of the Clinical Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Salter, Ashley, "A Psychological Perspective on Police Brutality: Current Statistics, Characteristics, and Trends Regarding Excessive Use of Force" (2021). Dissertations. 571. https://digitalcommons.nl.edu/diss/571 This Dissertation - Public Access is brought to you for free and open access by Digital Commons@NLU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons@NLU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. A Psychological Perspective on Police Brutality: Current Statistics, Characteristics, and Trends Regarding Excessive Use of Force Ashley Salter, MA Florida School of Professional Psychology at National Louis University Gary Howell, Psy.D. Chair Patricia Dixon, Psy.D. Member A Clinical Research Project submitted to the Faculty of the Florida School of Professional Psychology at National Louis University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Psychology in Clinical Psychology. Tampa, Florida April, 2021 The Doctorate Program in Clinical Psychology Florida School of Professional Psychology at National Louis University CERTIFICATE OF APPROVAL ___________________________ Clinical Research Project ___________________________ This is to certify that the Clinical Research Project of Ashley Nicole Salter has been approved by the CRP Committee on April 16, 2021 as satisfactory for the CRP requirement for the Doctorate of Psychology degree with a major in Clinical Psychology Examining Committee: ________________________________________ Committee Chair: Gary Howell, Psy.D.
    [Show full text]
  • THE ROLE of RACIAL BIAS in JUDGING FACES by Jenna Anne
    MODELING DECISION PROCESSES IN THE USE OF LETHAL FORCE: THE ROLE OF RACIAL BIAS IN JUDGING FACES By Jenna Anne Harder A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Psychology – Doctor of Philosophy 2020 ABSTRACT MODELING DECISION PROCESSES IN THE USE OF LETHAL FORCE: THE ROLE OF RACIAL BIAS IN JUDGING FACES By Jenna Anne Harder To empirically address the question of whether and why police officers are more likely to shoot Black than White suspects, psychologists have developed the First-Person Shooting Task (FPST): a laboratory task in which participants must make shooting decisions based on rapid assessments of whether a Black or White target is holding a gun versus a harmless object. Typically, studies employing the FPST have found that participants’ errors and reaction times show a bias toward shooting Black targets over White targets. Evidence for the mechanisms behind this bias is mixed, but several studies point to stereotypic associations between the category “Black” and some indication of threat (e.g. weapon possession). Collectively, this past work is suggestive that racial bias on the FPST is influenced by racial bias in threat perception. I investigated this hypothesis across three studies. Participants rated Black and White faces with regard to how “threatening” the faces appeared, then completed the FPST 3-15 days later. Behavioral and process-level (Drift Diffusion Model) methods were used to determine whether racial bias in a participant’s threat ratings explained racial bias in the FPST. Across two stimulus sets, results indicated that although participants displayed process-level racial bias, this was not explained by biased threat perceptions.
    [Show full text]